1,513
Views
63
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Beyond common and privileged: Gradient representations of common ground in real-time language use

Pages 62-89 | Received 06 Aug 2010, Accepted 05 Nov 2010, Published online: 12 Feb 2011
 

Abstract

The present research tested the hypothesis that on-line language processing is guided by gradient representations of linguistic common ground that reflect details of how common ground was established, including the discourse context and partner feedback. This hypothesis was contrasted with a simpler hypothesis that interpretation processes are only sensitive to simple binary representations of whether a potential discourse referent is or is not common ground. In order to evaluate these hypotheses, participants engaged in a task-based conversation with an experimenter in which some of the participant's game-pieces were hidden from the experimenter. On critical trials, the participant revealed the identity of the hidden game-pieces. Critical utterances contained referring expressions temporarily ambiguous between a visually shared game-piece, and a hidden game-piece. Analysis of participant eye movements during interpretation of these utterances revealed that participants were more likely to consider the hidden game-piece a potential referent if the experimenter had initially asked about its identity; whether the experimenter provided clear feedback that s/he understood its identity modulated this effect somewhat. These results provide key evidence for the richness of common ground representations, and are discussed in terms of the implications for models of the underlying representations of common ground.

Acknowledgements

Thank you to Gary Dell, Melissa Duff, and Duane Watson for helpful conversations regarding this research. This work was supported by NSF Grant 10-19161 to S. Brown-Schmidt.

Notes

1In cases where the probability of a privileged or common ground fixation was zero, ln(p(PG)/p(CG)) is undefined. In these cases, +1 was added to both the numerator and denominator in the calculation of both p(PG) and p(CG), (see Heller et al., 2008 for a related approach); this affected 8% of datapoints.

2 t-values are reported with MCMC estimated p-values; df for the t-statistic is not reported due to the complications of estimating df in mixed-models (see Baayen et al., Citation2008 for discussion).

3Including experimenter as a random intercept is necessary to take individual differences (particularly in pitch) into account.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 444.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.