208
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Does superior visual working memory capacity enable greater distractor suppression?

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 573-586 | Received 08 Jun 2022, Accepted 03 Nov 2022, Published online: 16 Nov 2022
 

ABSTRACT

We asked whether individuals high in working memory capacity have a superior ability to proactively suppress features. If so, it would help explain why these individuals are more resistant to attention capture. We tested this hypothesis using the capture-probe paradigm employed in Lien et al. (2022. On preventing attention capture: Is singleton suppression actually singleton suppression? Psychological Research, 86(6), 1958–1971). Participants (N = 112) performed a colour change detection task, assessing visual working memory capacity. They then performed a visual search task (70% of the trials) intermixed with probe tasks (30% of the trials). For the visual search task, either a salient colour singleton distractor or non-salient distractor (a triplet) appeared with the target object. For the probe recall task, participants reported probe letters that briefly appeared inside each object. Replicating Lien et al., a suppression effect on probe recall accuracy was observed for both salient singletons and non-salient triplets. Critically, high and low visual working memory capacity individuals showed statistically equivalent ability to suppress colour distractors. These findings suggest that proactive suppression is not the mechanism by which high-capacity individuals achieve greater resistance to capture. Proactive suppression may be an implicit process that does not require special working memory capabilities.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Notes

1 Gaspar et al. (Citation2016) divided their 48 participants into high-K, medium-K, and low-K groups, and observed the absence of a PD in the low-K group only. To provide a finer-grained analysis, we followed Gaspar et al.’s approach and sorted participants based on their K-value into 3 groups (high-K [N = 37], medium-K [N = 38], and low-K [N = 37]). A significant suppression effect was still observed for the low-K group (−5%±2%), |t(36)| = 6.84, p < .001, dz = 1.12. This effect was not significantly different from either the high-K or medium-K group (-7%±2% and −6%±2%, respectively), |ts| < 1.0.

2 We also used JASP to run a linear mixed effect analysis of the relationship between the suppression effect, K-value, and trial type (singleton vs. triplet; dummy-coded). As fixed effects, we entered K-value and trial type into the model. We found no significant effects of K-value (βˆ = −.004, p = .48) or trial type (βˆ = .01, p = .10). Furthermore, no significant interaction was observed between K-value and trial type (βˆ = .003, p = .44).

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 238.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.