Publication Cover
Laterality
Asymmetries of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognition
Volume 25, 2020 - Issue 5
1,461
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

From observed laterality to latent hemispheric differences: Revisiting the inference problem

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 560-582 | Received 29 Feb 2020, Accepted 28 Apr 2020, Published online: 26 May 2020

Figures & data

Figure 1. Probability of being left or non-left hemispheric dominant given a single measurement of perceptual laterality. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 1. Probability of being left or non-left hemispheric dominant given a single measurement of perceptual laterality. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 2. The boxplots show the distribution of dichotic listening scores for left dominant, unclassified, and right dominant subjects, according to the Bayesian classification, for the example in Section “Empirical data example”. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 2. The boxplots show the distribution of dichotic listening scores for left dominant, unclassified, and right dominant subjects, according to the Bayesian classification, for the example in Section “Empirical data example”. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Table 1. Comparison between classifical classification with a cut-off at zero and classification based on the Bayesian model developed in this paper.

Figure 3. If a first measurement with value given by the x-axis has been obtained for a right-handed subject, the curves show the value of the final probability of left hemispheric dominance after a second measurement has been obtained with value given by the colour legend. The dashed black line shows the same probabilities in the case where only a single measurement has been obtained. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 3. If a first measurement with value given by the x-axis has been obtained for a right-handed subject, the curves show the value of the final probability of left hemispheric dominance after a second measurement has been obtained with value given by the colour legend. The dashed black line shows the same probabilities in the case where only a single measurement has been obtained. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Table 2. The table shows the probability of left brain asymmetry after obtaining one, two, three, or four measurements of the same magnitude for a right-handed subject.

Table 3. The table shows how the number of classification with 80% probability or higher depends on the number of measurements for left-handers and right-handers.

Figure 4. How an individual subject's probability of being left dominant (black lines) changes by successively entering 1, 2, 3, and 4 laterality measures into the prediction. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 4. How an individual subject's probability of being left dominant (black lines) changes by successively entering 1, 2, 3, and 4 laterality measures into the prediction. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Table 4. The table shows how the number of classification with 95% probability or higher depends on the number of measurements for the Bless App data.

Figure 5. Results of the simulation experiment described in Section “Simulation experiment.” The figures show the proportion of subjects classified correctly, incorrectly, or undecided at an 80% threshold. Shaded regions represent 95% confidence bands. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 5. Results of the simulation experiment described in Section “Simulation experiment.” The figures show the proportion of subjects classified correctly, incorrectly, or undecided at an 80% threshold. Shaded regions represent 95% confidence bands. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 6. Proportion correctly classified, incorrectly classified and undecided subjects as a function of the chosen probability threshold for classification. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Figure 6. Proportion correctly classified, incorrectly classified and undecided subjects as a function of the chosen probability threshold for classification. [To view this figure in color, please see the online version of this journal.]

Table A1. Satz's model for right-handers, using updated prior information.

Table A2. Satz's model for left-handers, using updated prior information.

Table A3. Probabilities of left or right brain dominance in Satz's model, using updated prior information.

Data availability statement

R scripts for reproduction of all numerical examples and simulation results, including figures and tables, are available at the Open Science Framework, https://osf.io/mkwcr. The data from Bless et al. (Citation2013) analysed in Section “Empirical data example” is also available at https://osf.io/mkwcr. We cannot share the data from Hugdahl et al. (Citation2009) and Bless et al. (Citation2015) analysed in Section “Empirical data example” nor the data from Karlsson et al. (Citation2019) analysed in “Empirical data example .”