471
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Meter for what? Meter strength affects ordinal but not temporal learning

& | (Reviewing editor)
Article: 1537062 | Received 17 May 2018, Accepted 13 Oct 2018, Published online: 29 Oct 2018

Figures & data

Figure 1. Sequences for serial recall. (A) The two temporal structures used in defined meter vs. weak-meter conditions (short vertical lines = beat onsets; crosses = sequence events). Arrows indicate an example of concurrent ordinal information. (B) Ordinal structures were random series of nine tones, high or low, in the auditory conditions, nine spatial positions, up or down, in the visual ones. In both cases, participants used the arrow up/down keys of the keyboard to reproduce the sequence

Figure 1. Sequences for serial recall. (A) The two temporal structures used in defined meter vs. weak-meter conditions (short vertical lines = beat onsets; crosses = sequence events). Arrows indicate an example of concurrent ordinal information. (B) Ordinal structures were random series of nine tones, high or low, in the auditory conditions, nine spatial positions, up or down, in the visual ones. In both cases, participants used the arrow up/down keys of the keyboard to reproduce the sequence

Table 1. Design of the experiment

Table 2. Predictors of temporal error

Table 3. Predictors of ordinal accuracy

Figure 2. Mean temporal error across block (1–5), temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning), modality (auditory vs. visual) and meter-strength levels (defined vs. weak). Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. Temporal performance improved across blocks regardless of temporal instruction, modality, or meter strength

Figure 2. Mean temporal error across block (1–5), temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning), modality (auditory vs. visual) and meter-strength levels (defined vs. weak). Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. Temporal performance improved across blocks regardless of temporal instruction, modality, or meter strength

Figure 3. Mean temporal error across temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning), meter strength (defined vs. weak) and modality. Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. There were no meter-strength effects. Modality had an effect on beat processing (main effect on temporal performance), but not on meter processing (no interaction with meter-strength effects)

Figure 3. Mean temporal error across temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning), meter strength (defined vs. weak) and modality. Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. There were no meter-strength effects. Modality had an effect on beat processing (main effect on temporal performance), but not on meter processing (no interaction with meter-strength effects)

Figure 4. Mean ordinal accuracy across temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning) and meter strength (defined vs. weak). Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. Meter strength enhanced ordinal learning

Figure 4. Mean ordinal accuracy across temporal instruction (incidental vs. intentional learning) and meter strength (defined vs. weak). Vertical bars represent the standard error of the mean. Meter strength enhanced ordinal learning