1,183
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Psychometric properties of an Arabic Version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen Scale

ORCID Icon
Article: 2138543 | Received 30 Jun 2022, Accepted 12 Oct 2022, Published online: 03 Nov 2022

ABSTRACT

Objective

Few studies have investigated the Dark Triad and its impact on behaviour in Saudi Arabia, mostly due to the lack of validated instruments. The aim of this study was to investigate the psychometric properties of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, in the context of Saudi Arabia.

Method

A sample of 1,329 respondents (59.8% female, mean age = 26.79, SD = 8.47) completed a survey containing the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DD), the Corruption Propensity Scale and the Propensity to Morally Disengage scale, as well as a demographics questionnaire. Confirmatory factor analysis, measurement invariance across gender, internal consistency reliability analysis, test–retest reliability analysis, and concurrent and convergent validity analyses were performed to validate the DD.

Results

The CFA supported a three-factor model with adequate factor loadings ranging between 0.29 and 0.83 and sufficient fit indices. The scale was gender invariant. The internal consistency reliability and test–retest reliability were adequate (0.70–0.86 and 0.58–0.75, respectively). Moderate-to-high Pearson correlations supported the convergent and concurrent validity of the scale.

Conclusion

The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen is a reliable and valid measure that can be used in Saudi Arabia.

Key points

What is already known about this topic:

(1) In the past two decades, there has been considerable attention in the psychological literature to the Dark Triad personality traits in explaining malicious behaviours.

(2) Scales to measure the Dark Triad personality traits in individuals have been developed, among them the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale.

(3) The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale is widely used and has been validated in many countries, but not in Saudi Arabia.

What this topic adds:

(1) This study presents the results of the first validation of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale in the context of Saudi Arabia.

(2) Results of this study confirmed the psychometric properties of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale in Saudi Arabia, joining previous research in other countries.

(3) The study results indicate that this scale is a valid and reliable tool for practitioners and researchers to assess the Dark Triad personality traits in Saudis.

Introduction

In the fields of social psychology and personality psychology, many researchers have investigated negatively connoted aspects of personality and the Dark Triad model of such “dark” personality traits has been widely discussed (Jonason et al., Citation2012; Paulhus & Williams, Citation2002). The Dark Triad is a cluster term that encompasses a trio of undesirable personality traits: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy (Paulhus & Williams, Citation2002). Individuals who exhibit the trait of Machiavellianism tend to manipulate others in order to achieve their personal goals, and they lack internalised morality (Pechorro et al., Citation2021). Psychopathy is characterised by affective problems such as lack of remorse, behavioural problems including impulsivity, manipulativeness and lack of responsibility, and interpersonal problems such as abusive charming (Hare & Neumann, Citation2008). Narcissism is characterised by excessive vanity and obsession with one’s own qualities, which have a deleterious effect on relationships with others (Pineda et al., Citation2018). Individuals with Dark Triad personalities exhibit a constellation of disagreeable characteristics, including deceit and lack of empathy. The trio of constructs are distinct, yet related and overlapping (Paulhus & Williams, Citation2002). The three have in common the core trait of callous manipulation (Furnham et al., Citation2013).

The early work on assessing the Dark Triad in individuals used separate scales for each of the three personality traits. Narcissism was investigated using the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (Raskin & Terry, Citation1988). Several scales to assess Machiavellianism were developed by Christie and Geis (Citation1970). These scales were the MACH-I, MACH-II, MACH-III, MACH-IV and MACH-V, but MACH-IV was the one that was widely used (Fehr et al., Citation1992). Psychopathy was assessed with the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale (Hare, Citation1980; Neal & Sellbom, Citation2012), the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (Hare et al., Citation1990) and the Psychopathy Personality Inventory-Revised (Lilienfeld & Widows, Citation2005). The Arabic versions of the MACH IV, the Psychopathic Personality Inventory-Revised, the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory were also made and used in previous research (Latzman et al., Citation2015; Schwartz et al., Citation2017; Starr, Citation1975; Thomas et al., Citation2013). However, these scales had limitations of length, having too many items, which hampered usability. Therefore, single, shorter instruments were developed to assess the three dark traits, including the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010) and the Short Dark Triad (Jones & Paulhus, Citation2014).

The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DD) (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010) is the shortest measure, which can be efficient when time and resources are scarce, and has been validated and used in many cultures (despite its name, the Short Dark Triad (Jones & Paulhus, Citation2014) contains 27 items, whereas the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen contains only 12). The DD has been validated in Canada (Savard et al., Citation2017), Germany (Küfner et al., Citation2015), Poland (Czarna et al., Citation2016), Spain (Pineda et al., Citation2018), Serbia (Dinić et al., Citation2018), Turkey (Özsoy et al., Citation2017), Sweden (Garcia et al., Citation2018), Portugal (Macedo et al., Citation2017), Iran (Yousefi & Piri, Citation2016), France (Gamache et al., Citation2018) and Bangladesh (Ahmed et al., Citation2020), but not yet in Saudi Arabia.

There have been noteworthy criticisms of the Dark Triad concept and the scales that have been used to assess Dark Triad personality traits. A review by Miller et al. (Citation2019) concluded that it is evident that the Dark Triad is multidimensional, but existing research largely considers each construct independently. Similarly, the Dark Triad scales assess each construct as an independent unidimensional construct. Miller et al. (Citation2019) claimed that the existing measures and research concerning the Dark Triad assess narcissism and two different versions of psychopathy. They also argued that research on the Dark Triad has relied on the use of multivariate analyses that pose statistical and interpretation difficulties, and has been conducted with methodological concerns such as convenience sampling. Miller et al. (Citation2017) argued that existing measures of Machiavellianism do not match expert ratings. As mentioned earlier, the three Dark Triad traits can overlap and Vize et al. (Citation2018) questioned whether the Dark Triad components are sufficiently distinct from one another. They asserted, for example, that psychopathy and Machiavellianism overlap substantially. And Glenn and Sellbom (Citation2015) state that narcissism and Machiavellianism may be parts of a single psychopathy construct. Other concerns have been raised concerning the conceptualisation of narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy under a single construct termed the “Dark Triad” (Kajonius et al., Citation2016).

Empirical evidence has supported the commonalities and overlap among the Dark Triad traits. In a meta-analysis carried out by Muris et al. (Citation2017), the overlap between the Dark Triad traits was observed as follows: the correlation between Machiavellianism and narcissism was r = 0.34, between Machiavellianism and psychopathy, r = 0.58, and between narcissism and psychopathy, r = 0.34. Similarly, O’Boyle et al. (Citation2012) reported commonalities between the Dark Triad traits, where correlations between traits ranged from r = 0.23 to r = 0.46 in their meta-analysis. Emphasising the overlap between Dark Triad traits, a study by Rogoza and Cieciuch (Citation2019) supported the differentiation of the Dark Triad traits into only two constructs, narcissism and a “Dark Dyad” of Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Other researchers (Book et al., Citation2016; Buckels et al., Citation2013) have suggested adding a fourth trait, sadism, to the Dark Triad, making it the “dark tetrad”. Studies by Moshagen et al. (Citation2018) considered a large number of dark personality characteristics simultaneously and led the researchers to specify an uncategorised core set of negative ethical, moral and social traits that they labelled the “Dark Factor of Personality” or the “D-factor”.

Despite the critics, these researchers acknowledged the theoretical distinction between the dark triad constructs. It is on empirical grounds that the distinction between Machiavellianism and psychopathy seems nearly unclear (Miller et al., Citation2017; Vize et al., Citation2018). Other studies also reported high correlations between Machiavellianism and psychopathy (Pabian et al., Citation2015). Nonetheless, there are other studies that have found quite moderate correlations between the two constructs (Malesza et al., Citation2019). Therefore, there is a need for further investigation, with other samples, of the factorial structure of the dark triad dirty dozen, a short scale consisting of only 12 items that allows facility of research.

In addition, most of the studies on the Dark Triad have been conducted in Western countries with individualistic cultures. There is little research from collectivistic cultures such as Saudi Arabia, where people value most group identity and social harmony (Markus & Kitayama, Citation1991). For example, in the meta-analysis by O’Boyle et al. (Citation2012), only 4 of 186 studies were based on samples from collectivistic cultures. Dark Triad characteristics such as manipulativeness, self-importance and lack of empathy are incompatible with the characteristics of social harmony in collectivistic cultures. Further, despite the vast utility of the DD, this scale has not yet been validated in Saudi Arabia. This may have contributed to the lack of research in this population. Only two studies could be identified that have investigated the Dark Triad in the context of Saudi Arabia. Wright et al. (Citation2017) investigated malevolent behaviours in Saudi youth using the DD and reported that the Dark Triad was predictive of violent delinquency. Pietenpol et al. (Citation2018) reported that a potential factor influencing Mutaween (Saudi religious police) stops was individuals with Dark Triad traits. With the purpose of facilitating cross-cultural comparison and to encourage more research on the Dark Triad in the Kingdom, this study aimed to contribute to the literature by investigating the psychometric properties of the DD in Saudi Arabia.

Methods

Participants

This study used a convenience sample of students in order to reach as many and diverse participants as possible. A link to the survey was sent to universities who, in turn, sent the link via emails, Facebook, WhatsApp and Twitter to their students. A university was selected from each of the main region of the Kingdom of Saudi Arabia. Those regions are Mecca region, Riyadh region, Eastern region, Asir region, Jazan region, Medina region, Al-Qassim region, Tabuk region, Ha’il region, Najran region, Al-Jawf region, Northern Borders region and Al-Bahah region. In total, 13 universities were contacted. Respondents were informed about the aim and the intended outcome of the study, and they provided informed consent. Around 1850 students returned the survey, but a total of 1,329 respondents returned the completed survey with no missing data. The sample included 795 females (59.8%) and 534 males (40.2%). The sample had a mean age of 26.79 (SD = 8.47), ranging between 17 and 39. The socio-demographic characteristics of the sample are displayed in . Respondents were asked if they wished to be contacted to complete the survey a second time; 79 accepted and were contacted again 3 weeks later. However, only 55 of these returned the completed survey and these data were used to conduct a test–retest reliability analysis.

Table 1. Sample characteristics and ANOVA tests.

Measures

The survey contained three main scales as well as demographic questions. The scales were the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DD) scale (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010), The Corruption Propensity Scale (Agbo & Iwundu, Citation2015) and the Propensity to Morally Disengage scale (Moore et al., Citation2012). The Corruption Propensity Scale and the Propensity to Morally Disengage scale were used to check the convergent and concurrent validity of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DD) scale. These scales were chosen because of their small number of items compared to scales that were previously used consisting of too many items. The literature recommends the use of scales with low number of items in order to avoid fatigue and disengagement in respondents (Steyn, Citation2017).

The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DD) scale (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010) is a short 12-item instrument designed to measure the Dark Triad personality traits in individuals. It contains four items for each of the Dark Triad traits: Machiavellianism, with items such as “I tend to manipulate others to get my way”; psychopathy, with items such as “I tend to lack remorse”; and narcissism, with items like “I tend to want others to admire me”. The scale is scored on 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree) for each item; thus, the total score for each trait ranges from 4 to 36. The scale has demonstrated adequate psychometric properties (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010).

The Corruption Propensity Scale (Agbo & Iwundu, Citation2015) was used to assess the convergent validity of DD. This is an 18-item measure designed to assess corruption tendency. Each item in the scale is scored on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). Higher scores indicate greater intentions to engage in corruption. The scale exhibited an adequate internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.90.

The Propensity to Morally Disengage scale (Moore et al., Citation2012) was used in this study to evaluate the concurrent validity of DD. This is a 24-item measure to assess eight domains of moral disengagement, with three items for each. For example, “It is alright to lie to keep your friends out of trouble”. Each item is scored on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The reported internal consistency reliability was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.90). In this study, the Cronbach’s alphas for each of the eight domains were as follows: 0.77 for moral justification, 0.78 for euphemistic labelling, 0.71 for advantageous comparison, 0.75 for displacement of responsibility, 0.79 for diffusion of responsibility, 0.73 for distortion of consequences, 0.74 for dehumanisation and 0.70 for attribution of blame.

Procedure

This was a cross-sectional study conducted online. All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with the ethical standards of King Abdulaziz University and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. The respondents completed the demographic questionnaire first, and then they were presented the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen questionnaire. Before the start, respondents were given instructions and only upon consent, they could move to the questionnaires.

Data analysis

All the data analyses were conducted using the RStudio statistical software (Rstudio Team, Rs, Citation2022). Prior to analysis, a data check and cross-check were done to verify the correct tabulation of the data. Inspection of outliers was also performed. The Dirty Dozen scale and the scales to be used for convergent and concurrent validity of Dirty Dozen were subjected to internal consistency reliability analysis. The descriptive statistics for the sample characteristics were computed first, followed by ANOVA tests. Second, Pearson correlations and Cronbach’s alphas were computed with the use of the “psych” software package (Revelle, Citation2017). The exploratory factor analysis was done using the maximum likelihood extraction method and the varimax rotation method. The confirmatory factor analysis was done using the “lavaan” software package (Rosseel, Citation2012). The paths were plotted using the “lavaanPlot” package (Lishinski, Citation2020). The model fit of the DD was checked using several fit indices, including the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and Standardised Root Square Mean Residual (SRMR) (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999). Values <.08 for RMSEA and SRMR and values >.90 for CFI and TLI were used to indicate a good model fit (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999). Further, we assessed the measurement invariance across gender following (Van de Schoot et al., Citation2012) checklist using multi-group CFA.

Translation process and pilot study

The translation of the DD into Arabic was performed following the World Health Organization’s recommendations (WHO, Citation2011). Two independent experts experienced in instrument translation, adaptation and validation performed the translation using forward and backward translation methods. The results of the initial translation processes were discussed in multiple steps until consensus was achieved. The final version of the translated DD resembled the original. In a pilot study, this final version was sent to experts in the field for content validity. The experts gave their feedback concerning the content and adequacy of the items. This version was also sent to 55 students, and the data obtained were subjected to internal consistency reliability analysis. The Cronbach’s alphas of the items were adequate, ranging between 0.76 and 0.89. The content validity and internal consistency results provided evidence of suitability of the items to be used in the main study.

Results

Sample socio-demographics and differences

The descriptive characteristics of the sample as well as ANOVA tests are summarised in . Slightly less than 60% of the respondents were female, 57.8% were single, 24.2% were married, 12% were divorced and 6% were widowed. Slightly over 30% had income of less than 5000 RS per month, 24.3% had monthly income between 5000 and 10,000 RS, 16.6% between 10,000 and 15,000 RS and 28.6% had income of at least 15,000 RS.

In terms of differences, males had significantly higher scores on Machiavellianism and psychopathy, whereas females had significantly higher scores on narcissism. Concerning marital status, there were differences in Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism scores, where those respondents who were widowed scored significantly higher on Machiavellianism and psychopathy, and those who were single had significantly higher scores on narcissism.

Exploratory factor analysis

The exploratory factor analysis (EFA) used the maximum likelihood extraction method and the varimax rotation method. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test showed the measure of sampling adequacy (MSA) to be 0.87 and the Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (Bartlett’s K-squared = 938.74, df = 11, p-value <0.01). The factor loadings for each item ranged between 0.32 and 0.86 (). The EFA had a good model fit (χ2 = 242.74, df = 33, p < 0.001, RMSA = 0.03, TLI = 0.937, RMSEA = 0.06). The scree plot indicated a three-factor model – Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism – as in the original DD study (Jonason & Webster, Citation2010) (see ). The results of the EFA showed a structure of the three factors explaining 49.3% of the variance.

Figure 1. Scree plot of the exploratory factor analysis.

Figure 1. Scree plot of the exploratory factor analysis.

Table 2. Mean, SD, reliability, and EFA and CFA factor loadings.

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of DD

The CFA model allowed all DD items to load on their respective factors. The model exhibited adequate model fit (χ2 = 552.22, df = 51, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.925, TLI = 0.904, RMSEA = 0.08, SRMR = 0.06). The CFA factor loadings for each item ranged between 0.29 and 0.83 (). This model is plotted in .

Figure 2. Path diagram of the CFA model.

The numbers on the arrows represent the CFA factor loadings and the covariance between the factors.
Figure 2. Path diagram of the CFA model.

We also investigated the measurement invariance across gender of DD using the steps recommended by Van de Schoot’s et al. (Citation2012). The configural invariance exhibited adequate model fit (χ2 = 422.06, df = 102, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMR = 0.05). For the metric invariance analysis, we set the factor loadings to be equal across gender and adequate fit indices were obtained (χ2 = 513.77, df = 111, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.90, TLI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.07, SRMR = 0.06). The differences in CFI, TLI, SRMR and RMSEA between the configural and metric model were less than 0.01, which indicates metric invariance. To test the scalar invariance, we added the intercepts to the previous model and the model had adequate model fit as well (χ2 = 606.44, df = 120, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.89, TLI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.06, SRMR = 0.06). The differences in CFI, TLI, SRMR and RMSEA between the metric invariance model and scalar invariance model were less than 0.01, which indicates scalar invariance. Finally, to test the strict invariance model, along with the factor loadings and intercepts, we allowed residuals to be equal across gender and the model showed good model fit (χ2 = 608.65, df = 120, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.90, TLI = 0.89, RMSEA = 0.07, SRMR = 0.06). The differences in CFI, TLI, SRMR and RMSEA between the scalar and strict invariances were less than 0.01, which indicates full uniqueness of measurement invariance.

Convergent validity of DD

The convergent validity of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale was established by correlating DD with the Corruption Propensity Scale. All the correlations were positive and significant at p < 0.001 and the correlations ranged between r = 0.53 and r = 0.78, which provides evidence of the convergent validity of DD. The results of the Pearson correlations are displayed in .

Table 3. Concurrent and convergent validity.

Concurrent validity of DD

Pearson correlations between the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale and the Moral Disengagement Scale were used to determine the concurrent validity of DD, and all the correlations were positive and significant at p < 0.001. The correlations ranged between r = 0.40 and r = 0.81, which provides evidence of the concurrent validity of DD. The results of the Pearson correlations for each of the eight Moral Disengagement Scale domains are displayed in .

Reliability of DD

The mean scores, standard deviations and standardised Cronbach’s alphas for the three constructs and each of the items in the DD as well as the test–retest reliability coefficients for each construct are displayed in . The mean score for Machiavellianism was 9.31 (SD = 6.31, range = 4–36), the average score for psychopathy was 10.32 (SD = 5.77, range = 4–36) and the mean score for narcissism was 18.67 (SD = 8.80, range = 4–36). The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83 for Machiavellianism, 0.70 for psychopathy and 0.86 for narcissism. The test–retest reliability coefficients were 0.85 for Machiavellianism, 0.89 for psychopathy and 0.85 for narcissism.

Discussion

The small number of studies examining the consequences and impact of the Dark Triad on society in Saudi Arabia is attributable to the lack of validated measures. This clearly implies a need for a validated scale that can be used in this population. The aim of the present study was to investigate the psychometric properties of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen in Saudi Arabia. The results of the EFA and CFA supported the three-factor model of the original DD study of Jonason and Webster (Citation2010). The items exhibited adequate factor loading for both the EFA and CFA. The scale had good internal consistency reliability, good test–retest reliability and good concurrent and convergent validity. Furthermore, the scale seemed to be gender invariant.

The results of the confirmatory factor analysis corroborate those of Jonason and Webster (Citation2010) and other validation studies. This is also in line with the Arabic versions of the separate measures of the dark triad constructs. A study investigated the construct validity of the Self-Report Psychopathy scale (PPI-R) and reported similar properties of the scale among American and Saudi students (Latzman et al., Citation2015). Using an Arabic version of the Mack IV scale, there were no differences noted in a study comparing American and Arab respondents on the construct of Machiavellianism (Starr, Citation1975). Comparing UK samples and UAE samples on narcissism, Thomas et al. (Citation2013) reported no major differences between the two samples on the scores of Narcissistic Personality Inventory. The three-factor model showed adequate fit indices and factor loadings. The factor loadings ranged between 0.29 and 0.83, which is similar to those reported by Savard et al. (Citation2017) (ranging between 0.28 and 0.86), Pineda et al. (Citation2018) (ranging between 0.22 and 0.77), Jonason and Webster (Citation2010) (ranging between 0.39 and 0.84) and Jonason and Luévano (Citation2013) (ranging between 0.28 and 0.79). Although the EFA and CFA supported the three-factor model, Machiavellianism and psychopathy were very highly correlated, which corroborates the finding that these two dimensions might not be sufficiently differentiated (Rogoza & Cieciuch, Citation2019; Vize et al., Citation2018) and may be two sides of the same coin (O’Boyle et al., Citation2015). Pabian et al. (Citation2015) also found in their SEM model a high correlation between Machiavellianism and psychopathy. The multigroup CFA indicated that DD was gender invariant. This is in line with previous studies that reported measurement invariance of DD (Ahmed et al., Citation2020; Chiorri et al., Citation2019; Pechorro et al., Citation2021).

The results of the reliability analysis indicated adequate internal consistency analysis, with Cronbach’s alpha of 0.83 for Machiavellianism, 0.70 for Psychopathy and 0.86 for Narcissism. These Cronbach’s alphas are similar to those found in Bangladesh, 0.85 for Machiavellianism, 0.70 for psychopathy and 0.86 for narcissism (Ahmed et al., Citation2020). Overall, the results suggested good internal consistency reliability of DD. This was also found in previous studies (Chiorri et al., Citation2019; Jonason & Webster, Citation2010; Savard et al., Citation2017; Özsoy et al., Citation2017). The test–retest reliability ranged between 0.85 and 0.89. These results are higher than those reported in previous research. Yousefi and Piri (Citation2016) examined the psychometric properties of the DD in Iran and reported test–retest reliability coefficients ranging between 0.66 and 0.80. Our study’s correlations were also higher than those reported in a DD validation study in Poland (Czarna et al., Citation2016). These findings support the temporal stability of DD.

The results of the Pearson correlations between the DD and the Moral Disengagement Scale and the Corruption Propensity Scale, respectively, supported the concurrent and convergent validity of DD. The correlations were moderate to high, ranging between 0.40 and 0.81. These results are in line with those of prior research (Chiorri et al., Citation2019; Jonason & Webster, Citation2010; Milošević et al., Citation2022; Pechorro et al., Citation2021). These findings indicate that DD is related to other measures that assess the dark side of the human nature.

Although there may be cultural differences in terms of expression, it seems that the Dark Triad personality traits exist in both individualistic and collectivistic cultures. For example, in individualistic cultures, people with high levels of the Dark Triad traits tend to manipulate others in order to achieve their goals (Furnham et al., Citation2013; Pechorro et al., Citation2021). On the other hand, in collectivistic cultures, people with Dark Triad traits manipulate others by provoking their sense of responsibility because it is more efficient and acceptable (Robertson et al., Citation2016).

In terms of differences, this study found that men had higher scores on Machiavellianism and psychopathy, whereas women had higher scores on narcissism. These findings are similar to those found in Poland, where men had higher scores on Machiavellianism and psychopathy, but similar scores to women on narcissism (Czarna et al., Citation2016). The literature has also established gender differences in narcissism and psychopathy, where males were found to score higher than females (Cale & Lilienfeld, Citation2002; Grijalva et al., Citation2015), but the results regarding gender differences in Machiavellianism have not been consistent (Furnham et al., Citation2013). The differences where men score higher on all the three traits were also reported by Chiorri et al. (Citation2019). Another study investigating DD in eight regions of the world reported gender differences in the three constructs in those regions, where men had higher scores on the three traits, except in Asia where differences in psychopathy were not conclusive (Rogoza et al., Citation2021).

Research on gender differences in narcissism is less consistent than for the other constructs. Our findings revealed that females scored higher than males on narcissism, aligning with previous studies that reported higher scores in females on vulnerable narcissism (Green et al., Citation2020; Pincus et al., Citation2009; Wright et al., Citation2010). However, others reported no gender difference for narcissism (Besser & Priel, Citation2010; Bizumic & Duckitt, Citation2008; Bleske-Rechek et al., Citation2008; Miller et al., Citation2010) and others found higher levels in males than in females (Grijalva et al., Citation2015; Tschanz et al., Citation1998). Given the inconsistencies, further research is needed, especially in collectivistic societies.

Some limitations of this study have to be acknowledged. First, the design of the study was cross-sectional. Second, the study relied on subjective measures for which respondents could exaggerate or understate their responses. Future research should include objective measures, such as tracking real-world behaviours. Third, we lacked a cross-cultural sample for cross-cultural comparison; future research should investigate cross-cultural measurement invariance.

Conclusion

There is little research on the Dark Triad of personality in Saudi Arabia mainly due to the lack of validated measures. This study contributed to the literature by establishing in the Saudi context the psychometric properties of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen, a short scale that measures the Dark Triad traits of personality: Machiavellianism, psychopathy and narcissism. The confirmatory factor analysis supported the three-factor structure reported by Jonason and Webster (Citation2010). Nonetheless, the high correlation found between Machiavellianism and psychopathy supports the current critics about the little distinction between the two constructs. The internal consistency reliability and test–retest reliability were adequate and in line with prior research. Concurrent and convergent validity was also supported by moderate-to-high Pearson correlations with the Corruption Propensity Scale and the Propensity to Morally Disengage scale. In sum, the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen scale is a reliable and valid measure that can be used in Saudi Arabia to investigate the Dark Triad and assess individuals’ Dark Triad personality traits.

Acknowledgments

The author, therefore, acknowledges with thanks to DSR for their technical and financial support.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Additional information

Funding

The Deanship of Scientific Research (DSR) at King Abdulaziz University (KAU), Jeddah, Saudi Arabia, has funded this project under grant no. PH:008-246-1443.

References

  • Agbo, A. A., & Iwundu, E. I. (2015). Corruption as a propensity: Personality and motivational determinants among Nigerians. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 150(4), 502–11. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2015.1107523
  • Ahmed, O., Naher, L., Islam, R., Akter, M., & Deb, S. (2020). Psychometric analyses of the Bangla version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Heliyon, 6(11), e05341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05341
  • Besser, A., & Priel, B. (2010). Grandiose narcissism versus vulnerable narcissism in threatening situations: Emotional reactions to achievement failure and interpersonal rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(8), 874–902. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.8.874
  • Bizumic, B., & Duckitt, J. (2008). “My group is not worthy of me”: Narcissism and ethnocentrism. Political Psychology, 29(3), 437–453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00638.x
  • Bleske-Rechek, A., Remiker, M. W., & Baker, J. P. (2008). Narcissistic men and women think they are so hot - but they are not. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(5), 420–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.018
  • Book, A., Visser, B. A., Blais, J., Hosker-Field, A., Methot-Jones, T., Gauthier, N. Y., D’Agata, M. T., D’Agata, M. T. (2016). Unpacking more “evil”: What is at the core of the dark tetrad? Personality and Individual Differences, 90, 269–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.009
  • Buckels, E. E., Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). Behavioral confirmation of everyday sadism. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2201–2209. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613490749
  • Cale, E. M., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2002). Sex differences in psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A review and integration. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(8), 1179–1207. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00125-8
  • Chiorri, C., Garofalo, C., & Velotti, P. (2019). Does the Dark Triad manifest similarly in men and women? Measurement invariance of the Dirty Dozen across sex. Current Psychology, 38(3), 659–675. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9641-5
  • Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/c2013-0-10497-7
  • Czarna, A. Z., Jonason, P. K., Dufner, M., & Kossowska, M. (2016). The Dirty Dozen scale: Validation of a Polish version and extension of the nomological net. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(MAR), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00445
  • Dinić, B. M., Petrović, B., & Jonason, P. K. (2018). Serbian adaptations of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DTDD) and Short Dark Triad (SD3). Personality and Individual Differences, 134(January), 321–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.018
  • Fehr, B. A., Samsom, D., & Paulhus, D. L. (1992). The construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty years later. In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (pp. 77–116). Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315827483-8
  • Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: A 10 year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(3), 199–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12018/full
  • Gamache, D., Savard, C., & Maheux-Caron, V. (2018). French adaptation of the short Dark Triad: Psychometric properties and a head-to-head comparison with the Dirty Dozen. Personality and Individual Differences, 122(September 2017), 164–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.027
  • Garcia, D., Persson, B. N., Al Nima, A., Brulin, J. G., Rapp-Ricciardi, M., & Kajonius, P. J. (2018). IRT analyses of the Swedish Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Heliyon, 4(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.e00569
  • Glenn, A. L., & Sellbom, M. (2015). Theoretical and empirical concerns regarding the dark triad as a construct. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(3), 360–377. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_162
  • Green, A., Maclean, R., & Charles, K. (2020). Unmasking gender differences in narcissism within intimate partner violence. Personality and Individual Differences, 167. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110247
  • Grijalva, E., Newman, D. A., Tay, L., Donnellan, M. B., Harms, P. D., Robins, R. W., & Yan, T. (2015). Gender differences in narcissism: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 261–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038231
  • Hare, R. D. (1980). A research scale for the assessment of psychopathy in criminal populations. Personality and Individual Differences, 1(2), 111–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(80)90028-8
  • Hare, R. D., Harpur, T. J., Hakstian, A. R., Forth, A. E., Hart, S. D., & Newman, J. P. (1990). The revised psychopathy checklist: Reliability and factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 2(3), 338–341. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.2.3.338
  • Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217–246. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452
  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Jonason, P. K., & Luévano, V. X. (2013). Walking the thin line between efficiency and accuracy: Validity and structural properties of the Dirty Dozen. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(1), 76–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.010
  • Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 420–432. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019265
  • Jonason, P. K., Webster, G. D., Schmitt, D. P., Li, N. P., & Crysel, L. (2012). The antihero in popular culture: Life history theory and the dark triad personality traits. Review of General Psychology, 16(2), 192–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027914
  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105
  • Kajonius, P. J., Persson, B. N., Rosenberg, P., & Garcia, D. (2016). The (mis)measurement of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen: Exploitation at the core of the scale. PeerJ, 2016(3), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1748
  • Küfner, A. C. P., Dufner, M., & Back, M. D. (2015). Das dreckige dutzend und die niedertrachtigen neun [The Dirty Dozen and the naughty nine]. Diagnostica, 61(2), 76–91. https://doi.org/10.1026/0012-1924/a000124
  • Latzman, R. D., Megreya, A. M., Hecht, L. K., Miller, J. D., Winiarski, D. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2015). Self-reported psychopathy in the Middle East: A cross-national comparison across Egypt, Saudi Arabia, and the United States. BMC Psychology, 3(1), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-015-0059-2
  • Lilienfeld, S. O., & Widows, M. R. (2005). Psychopathic personality inventory–Revised: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Lishinski, A. (2020). lavaanPlot 0.5.1. Retrieved December 1, 2021, from https://www.alexlishinski.com/post/lavaanplot-0-5-1/
  • Macedo, A., Araújo, A. I., Cabaços, C., Brito, M. J., Mendonça, L., & Pereira, A. T. (2017). Personality dark triad: Portuguese validation of the dirty dozen. European Psychiatry, 41, S711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.1268
  • Malesza, M., Ostaszewski, P., Büchner, S., & Kaczmarek, M. C. (2019). The adaptation of the Short Dark Triad personality measure – psychometric properties of a German sample. Current Psychology, 38(3), 855–864. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9662-0
  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
  • Miller, J. D., Dir, A., Gentile, B., Wilson, L., Pryor, L. R., & Campbell, W. K. (2010). Searching for a vulnerable Dark Triad: Comparing factor 2 psychopathy, vulnerable narcissism, and borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality, 78(5), 1529–1564. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00660.x
  • Miller, J. D., Hyatt, C. S., Maples-Keller, J. L., Carter, N. T., & Lynam, D. R. (2017). Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A distinction without a difference? Journal of Personality, 85(4), 439–453. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12251
  • Miller, J. D., Vize, C., Crowe, M. L., & Lynam, D. R. (2019). A critical appraisal of the Dark-Triad literature and suggestions for moving forward. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 353–360. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419838233
  • Milošević, M., Božović, B., & Dopsaj, M. (2022). Psychometric properties of the Serbian version of mental toughness inventory and dark triad Dirty Dozen in police students. Nauka, Bezbednost, Policija, 27(1), 14–28. https://doi.org/10.5937/nabepo27-36389
  • Moore, C., Detert, J. R., Klebe Treviño, L., Baker, V. L., & Mayer, D. M. (2012). Why employees do bad things: Moral disengagement and unethical organizational behavior. Personnel Psychology, 65(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2011.01237.x
  • Moshagen, M., Hilbig, B. E., & Zettler, I. (2018). The dark core of personality. Psychological Review, 125(5), 656–688. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000111
  • Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Otgaar, H., & Meijer, E. (2017). The malevolent side of human nature: A meta-analysis and critical review of the literature on the Dark Triad (narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(2), 183–204. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616666070
  • Neal, T. M. S., & Sellbom, M. (2012). Examining the factor structure of the Hare self-report psychopathy scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 94(3), 244–253. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2011.648294
  • O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., & McDaniel, M. A. (2012). A meta-analysis of the Dark Triad and work behavior: A social exchange perspective. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 97(3), 557–579. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025679
  • O’Boyle, E. H., Forsyth, D. R., Banks, G. C., Story, P. A., & White, C. D. (2015). A meta-analytic test of redundancy and relative importance of the Dark Triad and five-factor model of personality. Journal of Personality, 83(6), 644–664. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12126
  • Özsoy, E., Rauthmann, J. F., Jonason, P. K., & Ardıç, K. (2017). Reliability and validity of the Turkish versions of Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DTDD-T), Short Dark Triad (SD3-T), and Single Item Narcissism Scale (SINS-T). Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 11–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.019
  • Pabian, S., De Backer, C. J. S., & Vandebosch, H. (2015). Dark Triad personality traits and adolescent cyber-aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 75, 41–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.11.015
  • Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 556–563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6
  • Pechorro, P., Jonason, P. K., Raposo, V., & Maroco, J. (2021). Dirty dozen: A concise measure of dark triad traits among at-risk youths. Current Psychology, 40(7), 3522–3531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00288-9
  • Pietenpol, A. M., Morgan, M. A., Wright, J. P., Almosaed, N. F., Moghrabi, S. S., & Bashatah, F. S. (2018). The enforcement of crime and virtue: Predictors of police and mutaween encounters in a Saudi Arabian sample of youth. Journal of Criminal Justice, 59(May), 110–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2018.05.007
  • Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G. C., & Levy, K. N. (2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory. Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 365–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016530
  • Pineda, D., Sandín, B., & Muris, P. (2018). Psychometrics properties of the Spanish version of two Dark Triad scales: The Dirty Dozen and the Short Dark Triad. Current Psychology, 39(5), 1873–1881. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9888-5
  • Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principal-components analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory and further evidence of its construct validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 890–902. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890
  • Revelle, W. (2017). Using the psych package to generate and test structural models. https://personality-project.org/r/psych_for_sem.pdf
  • Robertson, S. A., Datu, J. A. D., Brawley, A. M., Pury, C. L. S., & Mateo, N. J. (2016). The Dark Triad and social behavior: The influence of self-construal and power distance. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 69–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.03.090
  • Rogoza, R., & Cieciuch, J. (2019). Structural investigation of the Short Dark Triad questionnaire in Polish population. Current Psychology, 38(3), 756–763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9653-1
  • Rogoza, R., Żemojtel-Piotrowska, M., Jonason, P. K., Piotrowski, J., Campbell, K. W., Gebauer, J. E., Włodarczyk, A., Włodarczyk, A. (2021). Structure of Dark Triad Dirty Dozen across eight world regions. Assessment, 28(4), 1125–1135. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120922611
  • Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 2–4. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02
  • Rstudio Team, Rs. (2022). RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. RStudio, PBC, MA. http://www.rstudio.com/
  • Savard, C., Simard, C., & Jonason, P. K. (2017). Psychometric properties of the French-Canadian version of the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen. Personality and Individual Differences, 119, 122–128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.06.044
  • Schwartz, J. A., Said Al-Ghamdi, M., Nezar Kobeisy, A., Alqurashi, F. H., Connolly, E. J., & Beaver, K. M. (2017). Internalizing and externalizing problems and parenting: Results from a Saudi Arabian sample. International Journal of Social Welfare, 26(3), 263–275. https://doi.org/10.1111/ijsw.12230
  • Starr, P. D. (1975). Machiavellianism among traditional and Westernized Arab students. The Journal of Social Psychology, 96(Second Half), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1975.9923283
  • Steyn, R. (2017). How many items are too many? An analysis of respondent disengagement when completing questionnaires. African Journal of Hospitality, Tourism and Leisure, 6(2), 1–11.
  • Thomas, J., Hashmi, A. A., Chung, M. C., Morgan, K., & Lyons, M. (2013). The narcissistic mask: An exploration of ‘the defensive grandiosity hypothesis’. Personality and Mental Health, 7(2), 160–167. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1219
  • Tschanz, B. T., Morf, C. C., & Turner, C. W. (1998). Gender differences in the structure of narcissism: A multi-sample analysis of the narcissistic personality inventory. Sex Roles, 38(9–10), 863–870. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1018833400411
  • Van de Schoot, R., Lugtig, P., & Hox, J. (2012). A checklist for testing measurement invariance. The European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9(4), 486–492. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2012.686740
  • Vize, C. E., Lynam, D. R., Collison, K. L., & Miller, J. D. (2018). Differences among Dark Triad Components: A meta- analytic investigation differences among dark triad components: A meta-analytic investigation. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 9(2), 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000222
  • WHO. (2011). WHO | Process of translation and adaptation of instruments. https://www.coursehero.com/file/30372721/WHO-Process-of-translation-and-adaptation-of-instrumentspdf/
  • Wright, A., Lukowitsky, M. R., Pincus, A. L., & Conroy, D. E. (2010). The higher order factor structure and gender invariance of the pathological narcissism inventory. Assessment, 17(4), 467–483. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191110373227
  • Wright, J. P., Morgan, M. A., Almeida, P. R., Almosaed, N. F., Moghrabi, S. S., & Bashatah, F. S. (2017). Malevolent forces: Self-control, the dark triad, and crime. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 15(2), 191–215. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204016667995
  • Yousefi, R., & Piri, F. (2016). Psychometric properties of Persian version of Dirty Dozen Scale. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychology, 22(1), 67–76. https://www.sid.ir/fa/journal/ViewPaper.aspx?id=322338.