1,619
Views
56
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Trait emotional intelligence profiles of students from different university faculties

, &
Pages 51-57 | Accepted 01 Mar 2010, Published online: 04 Mar 2010

Abstract

This study investigated the trait emotional intelligence (trait EI or trait emotional self-efficacy) profiles of 512 students from five university faculties: technical studies, natural sciences, social sciences, arts, and humanities. Using the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire, it was hypothesised that (a) social sciences would score higher than technical studies in Emotionality, (b) arts would score higher than technical studies in Emotionality, (c) arts would score lower than technical studies in Self-control, and (d) there would be an interaction between gender and faculty, whereby female students would score higher than male students within the social sciences only. Several other exploratory comparisons were also performed. Results supported hypotheses (a), (b), and (d), but not hypothesis (c), although the differences were in the predicted direction.

Trait emotional intelligence (trait EI or trait emotional self-efficacy) is defined as a constellation of emotion-related self-perceptions and dispositions located at the lower levels of personality hierarchies (Petrides, Pita, & Kokkinaki, Citation2007). The construct provides a comprehensive operationalisation of the affect-related aspects of personality () and lies wholly outside the taxonomy of human cognitive ability (Carroll, Citation1993).

Table I. Internal consistencies for the trait EI factors and global score (N = 512)

Research on trait EI has gathered significant momentum in the last few years, which has helped to establish a nomological network for the construct. It is only in the presence of such a network that group comparison studies, such as the one presented in this article, can be meaningfully interpreted and linked to the core nature of a psychological construct (Cronbach & Meehl, Citation1955). Some salient contributions to this line of research include studies showing that trait EI is related to affective decision making (Sevdalis, Petrides, & Harvey, Citation2007), emotion regulation (Mikolajczak, Nelis, Hansenne, & Quoidbach, Citation2008), peer-ratings of behaviour (Petrides, Sangareau, Furnham, & Frederickson, Citation2006; Smith, Heaven, & Ciarrochi, Citation2008), performance in laboratory tasks (Austin, Citation2004, Citation2009), psychopathology (Malterer, Glass, & Newman, Citation2008), and relationship satisfaction (Smith, Ciarrochi, & Heaven, Citation2008). A growing number of studies have shown mediational and incremental trait EI effects over various relevant variables (e.g., Austin, Saklofske, & Mastoras, Citation2010; Downey, Johnston, Hansen, Birney, & Stough, Citation2010; Gardner & Qualter, Citation2010; Hogan et al., Citation2010; Johnson, Batey, & Holdsworth, Citation2009; Kluemper, Citation2008; Mavroveli, Petrides, Sangareau, & Furnham, Citation2009; Petrides, Pérez-González, & Furnham, Citation2007; Saklofske, Austin, & Minski, Citation2003; Schutte et al., Citation2010).

Strong evidence from a number of personality questionnaires (e.g., Cattell, Eber, & Tatsuoka, Citation1970; Costa & McCrae, Citation1992) supports the view that students in different academic subjects have different personality profiles. Given the mounting theoretical and empirical links between personality and trait EI (Vernon, Villani, Schermer, & Petrides, Citation2008), the extensive literature on personality profiles can steer the development of hypotheses in trait EI research. It is, in fact, one of the main advantages of trait EI theory that it can link the construct to the mainstream personality literature. This is particularly useful when tackling novel research questions for which there is no prior empirical literature, as in the present case.

Arts students tend to score higher in neuroticism than natural sciences and social sciences students (Rubinstein, Citation2005) and also appear to be less sociable and extraverted than students in other faculties (Csikszentmihalyi & Getzels, Citation1973). Natural sciences students, on the other hand, prefer precision over ambiguity and score higher in conscientiousness and conformity (Harris, Citation1993; Kline & Lapham, Citation1992). Students in social science disciplines have higher scores than technical and natural sciences students in openness to experience, empathy, and cooperation (Babbage & Ronan, Citation2000; Beauchamp & McKelvie, Citation2006). Introversion is a trait frequently found among technical studies students (Kirkcaldy, Citation1988). In addition, students with social career aspirations (e.g., guidance counsellors and teachers) tend to score higher in agreeableness than students in engineering (Larson, Wei, Wu, Borgen, & Bailey, Citation2007).

Much research has focused on the links between personality traits and academic and vocational interests. Costa, McCrae, and Holland (Citation1984) found that extraversion was related to social and enterprising interests, while openness to experience was related to artistic and investigative interests, with recent meta-analyses confirming these relationships (Barrick, Mount, & Grupta, Citation2003; Larson, Rottinghaus, & Borgen, Citation2002). Costa et al. (Citation1984) also reported a positive relationship between neuroticism and artistic interests (although only for male participants), and a negative relationship between extraversion and investigative interests. Further studies have ascertained that agreeableness, wellbeing, social closeness, and warmth are all closely linked to social interests (DeFruyt & Mervielde, Citation1997; Staggs, Larson, & Borgen, Citation2007; Sullivan & Hansen, Citation2004).

Hitherto, there has been little research into trait EI profile differences across faculties. Although much is known about general personality differences, more data are necessary to clarify specific emotional differences across various groups of students. The present study seeks to contribute in this direction.

In a preliminary study, Pérez and Castejón (2005) applied the Schutte Assessing Emotions Scale to a sample of university students and found that those in educational degrees scored higher in global trait EI than those in technical studies. We extended this research by looking at more disciplines and using a comprehensive instrument that captures all of the facets of trait EI.

Various studies have established interaction effects of gender and discipline on personality traits (Beauchamp & McKelvie, Citation2006; Roberti, Fox, & Tunick, Citation2003; Rubinstein, Citation2005). For example, female participants with social career aspirations score significantly higher than their male peers in agreeableness, in contrast to female participants with investigative or artistic interests (Larson et al., Citation2007). In the light of such findings, we thought it important to also incorporate gender into the design in order to test for possible interaction effects.

Given the paucity of closely related work, there was a salient exploratory aspect to this research. Nevertheless, based on our review of the personality literature, we advanced four specific hypotheses: H1, social sciences will score higher in the Emotionality factor of trait EI than technical studies; H2, arts will score higher in the Emotionality factor of trait EI than technical studies; H3, arts will score lower in the Self-control factor of trait EI than technical studies; and H4, there will be a Gender × Faculty interaction in the Emotionality factor of trait EI, whereby female students will score higher than male students, but in the social sciences only.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 512 students (202 male) from 17 to 44 years of age (M = 21.37 years, SD = 3.79). Students came from the following faculties: technical studies (N = 73; 40 in computer science and 33 in engineering); natural sciences (N = 65; 36 in medicine and 29 in chemistry/biology); social sciences (N = 291; 262 in psychology and 29 in sociology/education/social work); arts (N = 54; 20 in drama/music studies, 34 in ballet); and humanities (N = 29 in history/philosophy/linguistics).

Measure and procedure

The dataset was extracted from the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue) (Petrides, Citation2009) data archives. We chose to include only participants studying in the UK in order to avoid ambiguities arising from variations in course content and cultural differences. The TEIQue is a 153-item questionnaire providing comprehensive coverage of the sampling domain of trait EI. The instrument has shown excellent psychometric properties in a series of studies (Freudenthaler, Neubauer, Gabler, & Scherl, Citation2008; Mikolajczak, Luminet, Leroy, & Roy, Citation2007). Items are scored on a 7-point Likert scale and completion time is approximately 20 min. The 20 TEIQue variables (15 facets, four factors, and global trait EI) are presented in , along with descriptive statistics and brief explanations. All TEIQue instruments are available, free of charge, for academic research purposes.

Results

We performed a univariate factorial ANOVA with global trait EI as the dependent variable, and faculty (technical studies/natural sciences/social sciences/arts/humanities) and gender as the independent variables. For a more detailed analysis of the dataset and in order to examine possible factor-level differences, we also carried out a MANOVA with an optimised linear combination of the four trait EI factors as the dependent variable, and faculty and gender as the independent variables. This was followed up by univariate ANOVAs and, where appropriate, Games–Howell post hoc tests.

Faculty and gender differences in trait EI

The ANOVA indicated differences in global trait EI scores between the five faculties, F(4,508) = 3.21, p < .05, η2 = .16. Neither gender nor the Faculty × Gender interaction were statistically significant (). Games–Howell post hoc tests showed that arts students (M = 4.80, SD = 0.53) scored significantly higher than humanities students (M = 4.38, SD = 0.77, p < .05).

Table II. ANOVA of global trait EI vs. faculty and gender

The MANOVA yielded significant main effects of both faculty, Wilk's λ = .939, F(4,508) = 1.96, p < .05, and gender, Wilk's λ = .960, F(4,508) = 5.21, p < .01, as well as a significant interaction between the two, Wilk's λ = .930, F(4,508) = 2.28, p < .05.

Subsequent ANOVAs () indicated significant differences () in three of the four trait EI factors: Wellbeing, F(4,508) = 2.28, p < .05, η2 = .13, with arts scoring higher than humanities; Self-control, F(4,508) = 2.79, p < .05, η2 = .14, with natural sciences scoring higher than both social sciences and humanities; and Emotionality, F(4,508) = 4.09, p < .01, η2 = .17, with technical studies scoring lower than both social sciences and arts, and humanities scoring lower than social sciences (p < .05 for all pairwise comparisons, Games–Howell post hoc test). There were no significant differences in the sociability factor. With respect to gender differences, male students scored higher in the Self-control factor, F(1,508) = 8.92, p < .01, η2 = .13.

Table III. ANOVA of the trait EI factors vs. faculty and gender

Table IV. TEIQue descriptives vs. faculty and gender

As hypothesised, there was a significant Faculty × Gender interaction in Emotionality, F(4,508) = 3.18, p < .05, η2 = .15. Simple main effect analysis showed that, in the social sciences faculty, male students scored significantly lower than female students, F(1,506) = 15.16, p < .01, while the opposite was the case in humanities, F(1,506) = 5.26, p < .05.

Discussion

We investigated the profiles of students from different university faculties and found statistically significant differences in global trait EI as well as in Wellbeing, Self-control, and Emotionality. We also found a significant Gender × Faculty interaction in Emotionality.

Differences between faculties

The first hypothesis was supported by the results, because social sciences students scored higher than their technical studies peers in Emotionality (H1). This accords well with findings showing that the former are more agreeable, cooperative, and empathic than the latter (Babbage & Ronan, Citation2000; Beauchamp & McKelvie, Citation2006; Larson et al., Citation2007).

Arts scored significantly higher in Emotionality than technical studies, thus supporting H2. This is in line with Kirkcaldy (Citation1988), who found that introverted students are more inclined to enrol in technical degrees. In contrast, there was not enough evidence to support H3. Although arts did score lower than natural sciences and technical studies in Self-control, neither of these differences reached statistical significance. Thus, we have only limited evidence that arts students perceive themselves as less able to regulate their emotions and control their stress levels.

In fact, the arts group scored higher than all other groups in Wellbeing and global trait EI, although only the comparison against humanities reached significant levels. This perhaps contradicts studies reporting high levels of neuroticism in artists (Eysenck, Citation1972; Götz & Götz, Citation1979; Rubinstein, Citation2005). The higher global trait EI score of the arts group also contradicts the idea that arts students are relatively uninterested in social closeness (Roberti et al., Citation2003) and see themselves as aloof, cold, and reserved (Csikszentmihalyi & Getzels, Citation1973; Eysenck, Citation1972). It is noteworthy, however, that higher levels of introversion have often been noted in the non-performing arts population (e.g., visual artists) (Cox, Citation1999; Csikszentmihalyi & Getzels, Citation1973), while the present study focused on performing arts specialties, such as ballet, music, and drama. Further research is necessary to elucidate this issue looking at both professional and amateur samples of both performing and non-performing artists.

Results supported our hypothesis of an interaction between gender and faculty in Emotionality (H4). In the social sciences, female students scored higher than male students, while in the humanities the opposite was the case. Previous studies have reported that female participants tend to be more agreeable (Larson et al., Citation2007), but also more empathic and perceptive than male participants (Brody & Hall, 1993). Because these attributes are especially required in the social sciences (Mount, Barrick, & Stewar, Citation1998; Van der Zee, Zaal, & Piekstra, Citation2003), gender differences may be more evident in them. Traits such as emotion expression and empathy may not be equally important in specialties such as history, philosophy, or linguistics because they are in the social sciences (e.g., social work and psychology), where a people-oriented approach is dominant and one-to-one interactions are often required.

There were no gender differences in global trait EI or in any factor other than Self-control, where male students scored higher. This is in line with results reported by Petrides (Citation2009), who provided a detailed discussion of gender differences in trait EI. In the present study the effect of faculty on trait EI was greater than that of gender, which accords well with the view that “gender differences in personality traits are small relative to individual variation within genders” (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, Citation2001, p. 322).

The present analysis was limited by the relatively small group sizes once students were split by gender, and even more once split by faculty. Male and female students were not equally distributed across academic fields (), reflecting the fact that female participants disproportionately choose social subjects and male students, technical subjects (there were only eight male students in the humanities subsample) (Bonnot & Croizet, Citation2007; Rees, Luzzo, & Doyle, Citation2007; Trusty, Ng, & Ray, Citation2000). Future research will have to recruit samples of sufficient size to allow more detailed study of gender differences in trait EI across faculties. Additional faculties (e.g., business and management) should also be considered for a more complete picture.

Conclusions and implications for future research

This study has contributed data about emotion-related personality differences across several higher education faculties. Such knowledge has the potential to inform career counselling and ongoing student assessment (e.g., Vandervoort, Citation2006). Career counselling practitioners may want to take into account the role of trait emotional self-efficacy in advising individuals about their career choices and dealing with problems of academic engagement and satisfaction (Brown, George-Curran, & Smith, Citation2003; Menhart, Citation1999). Personality inventories have been used extensively in this area, but the emotion-related aspects of personality had hitherto been assessed only partially and indirectly.

Establishing trait EI profiles across faculties can help achieve congruence between students' personalities and their chosen academic field. Congruence, in turn, facilitates academic achievement and successful professional development (Henry, Citation1989). The present findings also have implications for teachers wishing to understand the personality of their students with a view to forging mutually beneficial academic interactions. Finally, this line of research offers clues about boosting student motivation and developing appropriate reinforcements and effective modes of training. For example, technical studies students tend to be less empathic and emotionally expressive than their social sciences peers, therefore, collaborative tasks and group work may elicit increased resistance from the former, while being more useful for the latter.

Trait EI assessment may also contribute to the development of self-reflection and enhanced awareness of one's personality. Within vocational and counselling psychology (person–environment fit: Furnham, Citation2001; Holland, Citation1997; Pervin, Citation1968), academic and vocational choices are understood as expressions of personality dispositions. Knowledge of their trait EI profile can help students make life decisions that are consistent with their affective dispositions. Such decisions can be taken in the context of academic study, as we have examined in this paper, extracurricular activities (Petrides, Niven, & Mouskounti, Citation2006), or personal relationships (Smith, Heaven, et al., Citation2008) and may ultimately have a positive impact on one's life satisfaction.

Acknowledgement

This research was partially supported by an Erasmus Mundus PhD studentship to the first author awarded through the Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.

References

  • Austin, E. J., 2004. An investigation of the relationship between trait emotional intelligence and emotional task performance, Personality and Individual Differences 36 (2004), pp. 1855–1864.
  • Austin, E. J., 2009. A reaction time study of responses to trait and ability emotional intelligence test items, Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009), pp. 381–383.
  • Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., and Mastoras, S. M., 2010. Emotional intelligence, coping, and exam-related stress in Canadian undergraduate students, Australian Journal of Psychology 62 (2010), pp. 42–50.
  • Babbage, D. R., and Ronan, K. R., 2000. Philosophical worldview and personality factors in traditional and social scientists: Studying the world in our own image, Personality and Individual Differences 28 (2000), pp. 405–420.
  • Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., and Grupta, R., 2003. The Five-Factor model of personality and Holland's RIASEC types: A meta-analysis, Personnel Psychology 56 (2003), pp. 45–74.
  • Beauchamp, K, and McKelvie, S. J., 2006. Personality traits and university program, Psychological Reports 99 (2006), pp. 277–291.
  • Bonnot, V, and Croizet, J. C., 2007. Stereotype internalization, math perceptions, and occupational choices of women with counter-stereotypical university majors, Swiss Journal of Psychology 66 (2007), pp. 169–178.
  • Brody, L. R., and Hall, J. A., 1993. "Gender and emotion". In: Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Barrett, L. F., eds. Handbook of emotions. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. pp. 447–461.
  • Brown, C., George-Curran, R., and Smith, M. L., 2003. The role of emotional intelligence in the career commitment and decision-making process, Journal of Career Assessment 11 (2003), pp. 379–392.
  • Carroll, J. B., 1993. Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1993.
  • Cattell, R. B., Eber, H. W., and Tatsuoka, M. M., 1970. Handbook for the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality Ability Testing; 1970.
  • Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R., 1992. The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.
  • Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., and Holland, J. L., 1984. Personality and vocational interests in an adult sample, Journal of Applied Psychology 69 (1984), pp. 390–400.
  • Costa, P. T., Terracciano, A., and McCrae, R. R., 2001. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 (2001), pp. 322–331.
  • Cox, A. J., 1999. Psychological vulnerability and the creative disposition: Etiological factors associated with pathology in visual artists, Dissertation Abstracts International 60 (1999), p. 361.
  • Cronbach, L. J., and Meehl, P. E., 1955. Construct validity in psychological tests, Psychological Bulletin 52 (1955), pp. 281–302.
  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Getzels, J., 1973. The personality of young artists: An empirical and theoretical exploration, British Journal of Psychology 64 (1973), pp. 91–104.
  • DeFruyt, F., and Mervielde, I., 1997. The five-factor model of personality and Holland's RIASEC interest types, Personality and Individual Differences 23 (1997), pp. 87–103.
  • Downey, L. A., Johnston, P. J., Hansen, K., Birney, J., and Stough, C., 2010. Investigating the mediating effects of emotional intelligence and coping on problem behaviours in adolescents, Australian Journal of Psychology 62 (2010), pp. 20–29.
  • Eysenck, H. J., 1972. Personal preferences, aesthetic sensitivity and personality in trained and untrained subjects, Journal of Personality 40 (1972), pp. 544–557.
  • Freudenthaler, H. H., Neubauer, A. C., Gabler, P., and Scherl, W. G., 2008. Testing the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue) in a German-speaking sample, Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008), pp. 673–678.
  • Furnham, A., 2001. Vocational preference and P-O fit: Reflections on Holland's theory of vocational choice, Applied Psychology 50 (2001), pp. 5–29.
  • Gardner, K. J., and Qualter, P., 2010. Concurrent and incremental validity of three trait emotional intelligence measures, Australian Journal of Psychology 62 (2010), pp. 5–13.
  • Götz, K. O., and Götz, K., 1979. Personality characteristics of professional artists, Perceptual and Motor Skills 49 (1979), pp. 327–334.
  • Harris, J. A., 1993. Personalities of students in three faculties: Perception and accuracy, Personality and Individual Differences 15 (1993), pp. 351–352.
  • Henry, P., 1989. Relationship between academic achievement and measured career interest: Examination of Holland's theory, Psychological Reports 64 (1989), pp. 34–40.
  • Holland, J. L., 1997. Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work environments . Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1997.
  • Hogan, M. J., Parker, J. D. A., Wiener, J., Watters, C., Wood, L. M., and Oke, A., 2010. Academic success in adolescence: Relationships among verbal IQ, social support and emotional intelligence, Australian Journal of Psychology 62 (2010), pp. 30–41.
  • Johnson, S. J., Batey, M., and Holdsworth, L., 2009. Personality and health: The mediating role of trait emotional intelligence and work locus of control, Personality and Individual Differences 47 (2009), pp. 470–475.
  • Kirkcaldy, B., 1988. Sex and personality differences in occupational interests, Personality and Individual Differences 9 (1988), pp. 7–13.
  • Kline, P., and Lapham, S. L., 1992. Personality and faculty in British universities, Personality and Individual Differences 13 (1992), pp. 855–857.
  • Kluemper, D. H., 2008. Trait emotional intelligence: The impact of core-self evaluation and social desirability, Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2008), pp. 1402–1412.
  • Larson, L. M., Rottinghaus, P. J., and Borgen, F. H., 2002. Meta-analyses of Big Six interests and Big Five personality factors, Journal of Vocational Behaviour 61 (2002), pp. 217–239.
  • Larson, L. M., Wei, M., Wu, T. F., Borgen, F. H., and Bailey, D. C., 2007. Discriminating among educational majors and career aspirations in Taiwanese undergraduates: The contribution of personality and self-efficacy, Journal of Counselling Psychology 54 (2007), pp. 395–408.
  • Malterer, M. B., Glass, S. J., and Newman, J. P., 2008. Psychopathy and trait emotional intelligence, Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2008), pp. 735–745.
  • Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K. V., Sangareau, Y., and Furnham, A., 2009. Relating trait emotional intelligence to objective socioemotional outcomes in childhood, British Journal of Educational Psychology 79 (2009), pp. 259–272.
  • Menhart, S. F., 1999. Emotional intelligence: An alternative explanation of career success, Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: Sciences and Engineering 59 (1999), pp. 37–48.
  • Mikolajczak, M., Luminet, O., Leroy, C., and Roy, E., 2007. Psychometric properties of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire: Factor structure, reliability, construct, and incremental validity in a French-speaking population, Journal of Personality Assessment 88 (2007), pp. 338–353.
  • Mikolajczak, M., Nelis, D., Hansenne, M., and Quoidbach, J., 2008. If you can regulate sadness, you can probably regulate shame: Associations between trait emotional intelligence, emotion regulation and coping efficiency across discrete emotions, Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2008), pp. 1356–1368.
  • Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., and Stewar, G. L., 1998. Personality predictors of performance in jobs involving interaction with others, Human Performance 11 (1998), pp. 145–166.
  • Pérez, N., and Castejón, J. L., 2005. "[Differences in the emotional intelligence profile in university students from different degrees.]". In: Romay, J., and García, R., eds. Psicología de las Organizaciones, del Trabajo y Recursos Humanos y de la Salud. Madrid: Biblioteca Nueva; 2005. pp. 1971–203.
  • Pervin, L., 1968. Performance and satisfaction as a function of the individual–environment fit, Psychological Bulletin 69 (1968), pp. 56–68.
  • Petrides, K. V., 2009. Technical manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue) . London: London Psychometric Laboratory; 2009.
  • Petrides, K. V., Niven, L., and Mouskounti, T., 2006. The trait emotional intelligence of ballet dancers and musicians, Psicothema 18 (2006), pp. 101–107.
  • Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., and Furnham, A., 2007. On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence, Cognition and Emotion 21 (2007), pp. 26–55.
  • Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., and Kokkinaki, F., 2007. The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space, British Journal of Psychology 98 (2007), pp. 273–289.
  • Petrides, K. V., Sangareau, Y., Furnham, A., and Frederickson, N., 2006. Trait emotional intelligence and children's peer relations at school, School Development 15 (2006), pp. 537–547.
  • Rees, A. M., Luzzo, D. A., and Doyle, C., 2007. Relational personality theory and Holland's typology among women: An exploratory investigation, Career Development Quarterly 55 (2007), pp. 194–205.
  • Roberti, J. W., Fox, D. J., and Tunick, R. H., 2003. Alternative personality variables and the relationship to Holland's personality types in college students, Journal of Career Assessment 11 (2003), pp. 308–327.
  • Rubinstein, G., 2005. The big five among male and female students of different faculties, Personality and Individual Differences 38 (2005), pp. 1495–1503.
  • Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., and Minski, P. S., 2003. Factor structure and validity of a trait emotional intelligence measure, Personality and Individual Differences 34 (2003), pp. 707–721.
  • Schutte, N. S., Thorsteinsson, E. B., Hine, D. W., Foster, R., Cauchi, A., and Binns, C., 2010. Experiential and rational processing styles, emotional intelligence and wellbeing, Australian Journal of Psychology 62 (2010), pp. 14–19.
  • Sevdalis, N., Petrides, K. V., and Harvey, N., 2007. Predicting and experiencing decision-related emotions: Does trait emotional intelligence matter?, Personality and Individual Differences 42 (2007), pp. 1347–1358.
  • Smith, L., Ciarrochi, J., and Heaven, P. C. L., 2008. The stability and change of trait emotional intelligence, conflict communication patterns, and relationship satisfaction: A one-year longitudinal study, Personality and Individual Differences 45 (2008), pp. 738–743.
  • Smith, L., Heaven, P. CL., and Ciarrochi, J., 2008. Trait emotional intelligence, conflict communication patterns, and relationship satisfaction, Personality and Individual Differences 44 (2008), pp. 1314–1325.
  • Staggs, G. D., Larson, L. M., and Borgen, F. H., 2007. Convergence of personality and interests: Meta-analysis of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire and the Strong Interest Inventory, Journal of Career Assessment 15 (2007), pp. 423–445.
  • Sullivan, B. A., and Hansen, J. C., 2004. Mapping associations between interests and personality: Toward a conceptual understanding of individual differences in vocational behaviour, Journal of Counselling Psychology 51 (2004), pp. 287–298.
  • Trusty, J., Ng, K. M., and Ray, D., 2000. Choice of Holland's social type college majors for U.S. racial/ethnic groups, Journal of Career Development 27 (2000), pp. 49–64.
  • Van der Zee, K. I., Zaal, J. N., and Piekstra, J., 2003. Validation of the multicultural personality questionnaire in the context of personnel selection, European Journal of Personality 17 (2003), pp. 77–100.
  • Vandervoort, D. J., 2006. The importance of emotional intelligence in Higher Education, Current Psychology 25 (2006), pp. 4–7.
  • Vernon, P. A., Villani, V. C., Schermer, J. A., and Petrides, K. V., 2008. Phenotypic and genetic associations between the Big Five and trait emotional intelligence, Twin Research and Human Genetics 11 (2008), pp. 524–530.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.