4,756
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Measuring Students’ Plant Awareness: A Prerequisite for Effective Botany Education

ORCID Icon, , , ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Received 30 Aug 2022, Accepted 07 Dec 2022, Published online: 27 Dec 2022

ABSTRACT

The term ‘Plant Blindness’ describes people tending to overlook plants, even though they have an enormous importance for life on earth, especially because of their ability to photosynthesise. Many attempts have been made to counteract plant blindness in biology education. However, so far there is no comprehensive tool to assess the degree of plant awareness (instead of plant blindness) in students. Here, plant awareness is conceptualised in four domains: (1) visual perception of plants, (2) categorising plants as living organisms, (3) knowledge about plants, and (4) attitudes towards plants. We present results from the two developed scales ‘Plants as living organisms’ and ‘Knowledge about plants’. To test the new scales, 345 Austrian secondary school students took part in a questionnaire study. A confirmatory factor analysis indicates sufficient model fit. Results show that students consider plants as ‘less alive’ than animals but ‘more alive’ than bacteria. Those who attribute the characteristics of life to plants have more knowledge about plants, indicating that both domains of plant awareness might have a common basis. The development of a plant awareness questionnaire will eventually provide a powerful tool to investigate the effectiveness of learning environments fostering plant awareness, a prerequisite to reach the sustainable development goals.

Introduction

Obstacles specific to botany education have been observed since the 1900s. Nichols (Citation1919) described an underrepresentation of plants during laboratory time in biology classes at university, Monahan (Citation1930) found a significant decrease of enrolments in high school botany courses. Dating back even further, Ganong (Citation1906) criticised the simplification of botany and teaching methods in schools. Moreover, biology students at university as well as pupils often perceive Botany as an uninteresting subject (Elster Citation2007; Uno Citation2009). With the introduction of the term ‘Plant Blindness’, Wandersee and Schussler (Citation2001) took a first step in addressing the underlying issue and started to tried to systematically search for find the roots of the observed mentioned obstacles.

Plant blindness

The term ‘Plant Blindness’ was coined by Wandersee and Schussler (Citation2001) describing the phenomenon that people tend to overlook plants in their everyday lives. Meanwhile many studies already confirmed a discrepancy in the recollection (Schussler and Olzak Citation2008; Schussler et al. Citation2010) and visual recognition (Balas and Momsen Citation2014; Zani and Low Citation2022) of plants compared to animals. At the same time, people tend to categorise plants as non-living organisms (e.g. Yorek, Şahin, and Aydın Citation2009; Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021). Moreover, botanical knowledge, plant species, and their role in different essential cycles, are topics that are often underrepresented in the curriculum (Hershey Citation1992) and by extension in collective human knowledge.

As a reason for this phenomenon, researchers assume that plants are rather ignored because they have never actively attacked humans or their ancestors, and, therefore, during evolution, attention focused on animals that move or pose a threat to humans. Moreover, they tend to move only slowly (Attenborough Citation1995), have no eyes that can look at us (New, Cosmides, and Tooby Citation2007), and are often seen as less spectacular (Nyberg, Brkovic, and Sanders Citation2021). Interestingly, even botanical scientific research seems to be biased by plant blindness, as colours and morphological traits influence the choice of research subjects (Adamo et al. Citation2021).

In the past 20 years, research studies described many facets of Plant Blindness, among them the inability to recall plants from memory (Zani and Low Citation2022; Schussler and Olzak Citation2008), the decline of students’ as well as teachers’ ability to identify and name different plants (Borsos, Borić, and Patocskai Citation2021; Kaasinen Citation2019; Bebbington Citation2005; Frisch, Unwin, and Saunders Citation2010; Palmberg et al. Citation2015), as well as the inability to recognise plants on the way to school (Lindemann-Matthies Citation2006). Also, researchers reported the lack of or incorrect knowledge about the reproduction of plants (Lampert et al. Citation2020) as well as the lack of knowledge about the necessity of plants being pollinated through zoogamy (Christ and Dreesmann Citation2022). Therefore, plant blindness is a serious obstacle for studying biology since the role of plants is often ignored by both teachers and students (Hershey Citation1993).

The importance of knowing about plants

In order to tackle important future challenges like the climate crisis, it is essential that people know the role of plants in important biological processes, for example the carbon cycle (Dillon et al. Citation2006; Howard, Ougham, and Sanders Citation2022). However, most people are not aware what plants need to grow (Barman et al. Citation2006). They often think that plants take up their mass through their roots (Driver et al. Citation2014). Additionally, there is a misunderstanding about which substances are needed for photosynthesis (Marmaroti and Galanopoulou Citation2006). Therefore, most people do not understand the necessity of photosynthesis for plant growth (Messig and Groß Citation2018). Without this knowledge, it is impossible to understand the important function of trees (and woods) as carbon sinks.

Moreover, Ryplova and Pokorny (Citation2020) found a lack of understanding regarding the impact of vegetation on its environment in form of natural cooling due to transpiration. Bofferding and Kloser (Citation2015) described similar findings: less than ten percent of students were able to name possible steps to adapt to the climate crisis, with the help of plants, e.g. planting of trees. These relations between missing basic botanical knowledge and matters that impact humanity on the whole planet are amongst the most important reasons why educators should try to counteract plant blindness and enforce plant awareness.

Counteracting plant blindness

Many attempts have been made to counteract plant blindness by focusing on plants in formal and non-formal biology education. These interventions reach from the introduction of plants as essential for world nutrition (Drea Citation2011) to the use of specific plant species or groups that students find more interesting than others (Pany Citation2014). Borsos, Borić, and Patocskai (Citation2021) successfully implicated gamification into the botany learning process. A huge emphasis lies in hands-on approaches through planting and caring for plants to form emotional connections (Strgar Citation2007; Krosnick, Baker, and Moore Citation2018; Stagg Citation2020).

Furthermore, out-of-school learning settings like botanical gardens often attempt to counteract plant blindness (e.g. Lindemann-Matthies Citation2006). Different studies found a positive impact on students’ awareness of plants after visiting educational interventions in botanical gardens which enabled sensory exploration beyond the visual (Krishnan et al. Citation2019). Kissi and Dreesmann (Citation2018) successfully used digital tools in extracurricular learning areas. Additionally, studying plants in the field improves identification skills (Borsos, Borić, and Patocskai Citation2021). Moreover, outdoor education not only boosts plant species knowledge, but also influences attitudes towards plants in a positive way (Fančovičová and Prokop Citation2011).

In addition, Lohr and Pearson-Mims (Citation2005) confirmed that contact with nature in childhood has a positive influence on attitudes towards trees and enables emotional connections with them (Moormann, Lude, and Möller Citation2021; Gebhard Citation2001). This is remarkable because in environmental education living organism are commonly used to motivate learners for nature conservation but educators tend to less often emphasise plants (Balding and Williams Citation2016). This leads to less funding and insufficient protection of plants that are threatened by extinction (Roberson et al. Citation2020), even though extinction of plant species is at its highest level in human history (Nic Lughadha et al. Citation2020).

From plant blindness to plant awareness

The core definition of the term ‘plant blindness’ has not changed since Wandersee and Schussler (Citation2001). Although this initial description of the phenomenon ‘plant blindness’ has without doubt been a very important step in research since it pointed to an important problem that subsequently has widely been investigated during the last two decades (e.g. Amprazis and Papadopoulou Citation2020; Mung and Williams Citation2016; Krosnick, Baker, and Moore Citation2018; Frisch, Unwin, and Saunders Citation2010; Zani and Low Citation2022; Allen Citation2003; Jose, Wu, and Kamoun Citation2019; Schussler and Olzak Citation2008), the construct starts to become obscure and difficult to navigate (Amprazis and Papadopoulou Citation2020). Therefore, it is necessary to clarify and update the definition of the term to enable a sound discussion about plant blindness.

Recently, Parsley (Citation2020) enriched the discussion about plant blindness by proposing to change the term ‘plant blindness’ to ‘plant awareness disparity’ because of ableism. This suggestion has already been picked up by recent researchers (Parsley Citation2021; Laura and Dreesmann Citation2022; Brownlee, Parsley, and Sabel Citation2021). Though, since it is well investigated that language influences our thinking (e.g. Bloom and Keil Citation2001; Topping et al. Citation2004), the authors of this paper decided to go one step further and use the bright side of the medal to describe our research. Following recent scientific results (Ädel Citation2017; Harker, Dean, and Monsen Citation2017; Watts Citation2017; Olsen Citation2018), we want to use a solution-oriented way of thinking about the long-known problem of plant blindness.

Following the ideas of McDonough MacKenzie et al. (Citation2019) we want to add a new impulse to the discussion by not only circling around the problem but focusing on a quality we want students to develop: plant awareness. Maybe this slightly changed point of view enables new attitudes for both scientists and educators alike, and frees them from a struggle against something but leads them to work in favour of something. Therefore, in the present paper we will neither use the terms ‘plant blindness’ nor ‘plant awareness disparity’ but ‘plant awareness’. In case the term ‘plant blindness’ is inevitable in the future, we suggest simply considering it as ‘lack of plant awareness’.

Evaluating the degree of plant awareness

Although much research has been done on the lack of plant awareness, there is still no comprehensive tool to assess the degree of plant awareness of an individual. Most of the few tools that were developed to identify an individual’s degree of plant blindness tested only one or two of the manifold symptoms without clear differentiation (e.g. Batke, Dallimore, and Bostock Citation2020). Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis (Citation2021) quantified (besides other aspects of plant blindness) how many interviewed students named one or more plants when told to write down five living organisms.

However, in order to develop effective lesson tools to enhance students’ awareness of plants it is necessary to test the impact of many different educational interventions on someone’s plant awareness. Therefore, the development of a questionnaire that enables researchers and teachers to quantify the degree of plant awareness is the aim of the present study Since recent studies show rather weak (though statistically significant) correlations between attitudes towards plants and other symptoms of low plant awareness (e.g. the number of plants students listed when asked for five living organisms [Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021], knowledge about monocotyls [Kubiatko, Fančovičová, and Prokop Citation2021]), the main focus of our research lies in the question whether different aspects of plant awareness can be considered as separate components and in what way they may correlate with each other.

Materials and Methods

The paper presented describes the first steps of the construction of a questionnaire to successfully assess and quantify plant awareness. Based on the original definition (Wandersee and Schussler Citation2001) and following the ideas of Parsley (Citation2020), who started first attempts to redefine the term ‘plant awareness disparity’ by sorting the possible symptoms into attention, knowledge, attitude, and interest, we reviewed recent literature and finally conceptualised plant awareness as consisting of four domains:

(1) visual perception of plants (e.g. Balas and Momsen Citation2014; Schussler and Olzak Citation2008; Zani and Low Citation2022)

(2) categorising plants as living organisms (e.g. Yorek, Şahin, and Aydın Citation2009; Ahi, Atasoy, and Balci Citation2018; Lindemann-Matthies Citation2005; Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021)

(3) knowledge about plants (identification and plant biology) (e.g. Kaasinen Citation2019; Palmberg et al. Citation2015; Sanders et al. Citation2022; Anderson, Ellis, and Jones Citation2014; Barman et al. Citation2006)

(4) attitudes towards plants (e.g. Colon et al. Citation2020; Kubiatko, Fančovičová, and Prokop Citation2021; Fančovičová and Prokop Citation2010; Lohr and Pearson-Mims Citation2005)

These four domains (see ) may correlate with each other, since for example Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis (Citation2021) or Kubiatko, Fančovičová, and Prokop (Citation2021) found weak connections between different aspects of plant awareness.

Figure 1. The four postulated domains of plant awareness.

Figure 1. The four postulated domains of plant awareness.

As one domain of plant awareness, we consider the visual perception of plants, the initial main testimony of Wandersee and Schussler (Citation2001). Only if people recognise plants and visually perceive individuals out of the ‘green background’, they can categorise them as living organisms. If people indeed see plants as living organisms, they may build up knowledge about plants (e.g. physiology, anatomy, and ecology) and learn to identify them. And, finally, if they gather knowledge about the diversity of plants and their manifold important roles in our environment, people may develop positive attitudes towards plants not only because they look or smell attractive. In this study we tested two parts of our assessment tool: the domain ‘plants as living organisms’ (in comparison to animals and bacteria) and three subscales of the domain ‘knowledge about plants’.

After developing the single-choice-items of the two scales (see ) based on recent literature (e.g. Campbell et al. Citation2015; Cooper Citation2008; Tunnicliffe and Reiss Citation2000; Kaasinen Citation2019; Krüger and Burmester Citation2005; Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021), the questionnaire was first administered to a group of 41 students who had the opportunity to comment on the items. If necessary, comments were used for item editing.

Table 1. Results of the Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the ‘Plants as living organisms’ scale: Unstandardised and standardised factor loadings. N = 345.

Table 2. Results of the Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the ‘Knowledge bout plants’ scale: Unstandardised and Standardised Factor Loadings. N = 345.

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics: Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation Coefficients, Means and Standard Deviations for the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale. N = 345.

Participants

409 students from six different secondary schools in the eastern parts of Austria took part in a questionnaire study. 64 questionnaires had to be excluded due to missing answers or zig-zag-patterns. The final sample comprised 345 individuals (58.5% female) aged between 11 and 19 (Mage = 14.14 ± 2.36). Students stem from 18 school classes, 10 classes from lower secondary level (grade 5–8, Mage = 12.7, 56.6% female, 0.5% others), and 8 classes from upper secondary level (grade 9–12, Mage = 15.8, 61% female, 1.3% others).

The research team was guided by the ‘Guidelines for Good Scientific Practice’ (Austrian Agency for Research Integrity Citation2016). Prior to participation, students were informed about the aims of the research, duration, procedure, and anonymity of the data. In order to avoid students focusing on their plant awareness and therefore possibly influencing the data, they were only informed that the questionnaire is about plants and should help improve biology lessons. Participation was always voluntary, and only those students, who (or whose parents) gave consent to participate in the study, were included in the data analysis. Data was collected and analysed anonymously. Under Austrian law, approval by an ethics committee was not necessary as this study did not involve patients, was non-invasive, and participation was voluntary and anonymous.

Instruments

In both scales items are coded such that a higher value indicates higher plant awareness. For example, a high value in the subscale ‘plants’ in the scale ‘plants as living organisms’ shows that a test person attributes many characteristics of life to plants. ‘Plant blindness’ is therefore reflected by a low value in a (sub-)scale. The items of the scales as well as the used plant species are shown in .

‘Plants as living organisms’ scale

The scale ‘plants as living organisms’ assesses to what degree students categorise plants as living organisms compared to animals and bacteria. For this purpose, students had to choose separately for each group of organisms (animals, plants, and bacteria) whether they show eight characteristics of life following Campbell et al. (Citation2015) (see ). Cronbach’s α based on tetrachoric correlation matrices show high item reliabilities for the three subscales (.97 for the sub-scale ‘animals’, .91 for the sub-scale ‘plants’, and .87 for the subscale ‘bacteria’).

‘Knowledge about plants’ scale

The scale ‘knowledge about plants’ assesses basic knowledge about plants. For this purpose, we developed three sub-scales: ‘plant species identification’ (following Kaasinen Citation2019; Bebbington Citation2005), ‘identification of spice plants’ (following Pany et al. Citation2019) and ‘anatomy of edible plants’ (following Pany and Heidinger Citation2017) (see ). We chose these three subscales for our first approach since their objects are close to students’ everyday lives (Krüger and Burmester Citation2005; Tunnicliffe and Reiss Citation2000).

The subscale ‘plant species identification’ was tested by giving the students the name of a common Mid-European plant species (e. g. dandelion Taraxacum officinalis) and showing them four pictures of different plant species. Students had to choose which picture represents the plant species indicated by the name. In the subscale ‘identification of spice plants’ students either had to choose spice plants out of a list of different plant species or they were given a picture of a spice plant (rosemary and parsley). They then had to pick the correct name out of a list of eight plant species. The subscale ‘anatomy of edible plants’ showed pictures of edible plants and students had to choose from a list which part of the plant is commonly used for culinary purposes (e.g. ‘fruit’ for peas Pisum sativum). Cronbach’s α based on tetrachoric correlation matrices show sufficient to high item reliabilities for the three subscales (.66 for the subscale ‘plant species identification’, .84 for the subscale ‘identification of spice plants’, and .83 for the subscale ‘anatomy of useful plants’).

Analytic Strategy

In order to investigate the factor structure of the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale, in a first step confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) (Brown Citation2015) for dichotomous indicator variables, using weighted least square mean- and variance- adjusted estimation method (WLSMV), was conducted in Mplus 8.6 (Muthén and Muthen Citation2017). CFA models were evaluated using the fit indices CFI, TLI, RMSEA, and SRMR based on common cut-off criteria (Kline Citation2016). Thereafter, weighted sum scores according to the factor loadings of the CFA model for the sub-scale animals, plants, and bacteria of the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale were computed and subsequently used in the analysis (DiStefano, Zhu, and Mindrila Citation2009).

In a second step, repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was conducted in R 4.2.0 (R. Core-Team Citation2013) to test for mean differences between the sub-scales animals, plants, and bacteria of the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale to validate the subscales. Based on recent literature (Balas and Momsen Citation2014; Yorek, Şahin, and Aydın Citation2009; Lindemann‐Matthies Citation2005), a significantly lower value for plants than for animals could be expected. In order to check whether other life forms also are seen as ‘inferior’ compared to animals, we included bacteria in a separate subscale. In the last step, we investigated whether students who see plants as rather ‘lifeless’ beings also have little knowledge about plants. Consequently, correlation coefficients between the sub-scale ‘plants’ of the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the sub-scales of the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale were inspected.

Results

Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale

Results of the CFA showed an acceptable model fit (χ2(241) = 273.44, CFI = .992, TLI = 0.991, RMSEA = 0.020, SRMR = 0.082) with standardised factor loadings between 0.81 and 0.99 for the sub-scale ‘animals’, 0.46 to 0.95 for the sub-scale ‘plants’, and 0.25 and 0.89 for the sub-scale ‘bacteria’ (see ).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis for the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale

Results of the CFA showed an acceptable model fit (χ2(167) = 186.30, CFI = .991, TLI = 0.989, RMSEA = 0.018, SRMR = 0.080) with standardised factor loadings between 0.30 and 0.73 for the sub-scale ‘plant species identification’, 0.32 and 0.90 for the sub-scale ‘identification of spice plants’, and 0.24 to 0.82 for the sub-scale ‘anatomy of useful plants’ (see ).

Mean Differences in the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale

Results of the repeated-measures ANOVA showed a statistically significant main effect for the factor sub-scale, indicating mean differences between animals, plants, and bacteria, (F(1.87, 688) = 259.81, Huynh-Feldt adjusted p < .001). Post-hoc tests with Holm correction for multiple testing revealed statistically significant mean differences between animals and plants (Manimals = 7.42 ± 1.37, Mplants = 5.95 ± 2.02, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.85) as well as animals and bacteria (Manimals = 7.42 ± 1.37, Mbacteria = 4.96 ± 2.24, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 1.37). Moreover, post-hoc testing showed a statistically significant mean difference between plants and bacteria (Mplants = 5.95 ± 2.02, Mbacteria = 4.96 ± 2.24, p < .001, Cohen’s d = 0.45) (see ). Students attributed the characteristics of life to animals in 80.9% to 97.4% (see ). None of the characteristics of life was attributed to plants to a similar extent (except for the characteristic ‘plants grow’), indeed, the characteristic ‘plants can move’ was only attributed in about 27.5% of the cases. These results clearly show that plants are seen as ‘less alive’ than animals but ‘more alive’ than bacteria, although the difference between animals and plants is considerably larger than between plants and bacteria.

Table 4. Frequencies (in percent) of the characteristics of life attributed to animals, plants, and bacteria. N = 345.

Correlation between the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale

Correlation coefficients for the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale are reported in . Results showed a statistically significant correlation between the sub-scale ‘plants’ of the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale and the sub-scales ‘plant species identification’ (r = 0.19, p < .001), ‘identification of spice plants’ (r = 0.19, p < .001), and ‘anatomy of edible plants’ (r = 0.38, p < .001). This analysis shows that those students who do not attribute the characteristics of life to plants have less knowledge about plants. It also indicates that both domains of plant awareness might have a common basis.

Discussion

The lack of plant awareness (also known as ‘plant blindness’ or ‘plant awareness disparity’) is a serious impediment for understanding the enormous importance of plants for life on this planet (especially because of their ability to carry out photosynthesis and thus being the basis of many food chains as well as acting as carbon sinks) and for developing an integrated view of nature. It is the reason for many problems students have in understanding and evaluating future challenges such as climate change or sustainability (Dillon et al. Citation2006; Wandersee and Schussler Citation2001; Roberson et al. Citation2020; Mung and Williams Citation2016; Howard, Ougham, and Sanders Citation2022). Therefore, it is key to find ways for educators to improve students’ awareness of plants.

Validity of the investigated scales

As has to be expected from recent literature (e.g. Yorek, Şahin, and Aydın Citation2009; Lindemann‐Matthies Citation2005), our results also show that plants are seen as ‘less alive’ than animals, which gives a clue that the ‘plants as living organisms’ scale is well suitable for assessing whether students see plants as living beings. However, in contrast to previous studies where students were asked to write down five living things (e.g. Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021), our results allow a deeper insight into which characteristics of life students rarely apply to plants. The apparent inability to move (like animals do) is one of the most important reasons that plants are seen as ‘less alive’ than animals. Moreover, our results go further by proving that plants are seen as ‘more alive’ than bacteria. This fits well into the idea that organisms that resemble humans are more easily considered ‘living beings’. What regrettably leaves a bitter taste is the fact that plants still are closer to bacteria than to animals.

The three subscales of the ‘knowledge about plants’ scale seem to be an appropriate approach to approximating students’ views on plants in their everyday lives. Whereas previous research only could prove rather weak correlations between different aspects of plant awareness (e.g. Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021), the scales of both domains investigated in this study – ‘plants as living organisms’ and ‘knowledge about plants’ – do correlate (see ). That means that students who do see plants as living beings have more knowledge about plants. This is a useful hint that both domains really have a common basis and can interact with each other. These results may encourage botany educators to further develop learning tools which may finally enable students to understand that plants are living organisms as well as animals.

The construct of plant awareness

Furthermore, these results strengthen the need for future research investigating whether other domains of plant awareness also correlate with each other. For that reason, it is important to better understand the construct plant awareness and its component parts. In contrast to previous research (Wandersee and Schussler Citation2001; Amprazis and Papadopoulou Citation2020; Batke, Dallimore, and Bostock Citation2020; Pedrera et al. Citation2021) the results of this study show for the first time, on a well-founded statistical basis, that plant awareness indeed consists of at least two different domains (see ) that seem to be interconnected. Previously, plant awareness was often assumed to be composed of different parts but without having empirical evidence for this idea. Hence, our data show that plant awareness should be conceptualised in different domains which now can be further explored. These different domains can be specifically addressed to foster plant awareness in formal and informal learning settings alike.

What is still an open field for prospective studies are the relations of the four postulated domains of plant awareness that may be organised in a hierarchical way. Probably, the most fundamental domain of plant awareness is the visual perception of plants, the starting point of Wandersee and Schussler (Citation2001). Only if people visually perceive single plant individuals out of the ‘green background’, can plants then be categorised as living organisms. If people indeed see plants as living organisms, they may build up knowledge about plants (e.g. physiology, anatomy, and ecology) and learn to identify them. And, finally, if they construct knowledge about the diversity of plants, people may recognise their manifold important roles in our environment and finally develop positive attitudes towards plants which go beyond seeing their flowers as pretty.

To enlighten this possible structure of plant awareness, the investigation of the remaining domains of plant awareness (visual perception and attitudes) must be tackled. For this purpose, there is a need for a reliable and valid instrument with which educators can assess and quantify plant awareness in its four postulated domains. By comparing the correlation of all domains, the plant awareness construct would be the first actualisation of the plant blindness theory on a comprehensive empirical basis.

Assessing plant awareness

A tool that tests all domains of plant awareness can assess the degree of plant awareness in a certain population. This possibility opens a gate into, for example, long-term studies on the development of plant awareness with increasing age. Moreover, a plant awareness questionnaire opens the possibility to assess the effectiveness of educational interventions designed to foster plant awareness (and therefore counteract plant blindness).

Our finalised plant awareness questionnaire could also help enlighten multiple questions already (but not sufficiently) investigated. The correlations between age or gender and plant awareness are such topics that were explored by different scientists (e.g. Sher et al. Citation2015; Strgar Citation2007) with contradictory results. However, these studies mostly tested only one domain of plant awareness. The same is true for the degree of plant awareness in rural areas compared to urban environments (Amprazis, Papadopoulou, and Malandrakis Citation2021; Villarroel et al. Citation2018).

In a next step, the questionnaire opens a gateway into assessing plant awareness in class in a quite simple way. If different domains of plant awareness actually correlate, one could concentrate in class on the domains simplest to assess, but nevertheless educators will get an impression how plant aware their students are. Based on these results, educators could choose interventions that are tailored to their target group.

Enabling students to comprehend the importance of plants is an important goal of biology education. Ultimately, increased plant awareness is the prerequisite for increased plant knowledge and understanding of biological systems, which is necessary to reach important global goals, like the sustainable development goals (Howard, Ougham, and Sanders Citation2022). In conclusion, a detailed knowledge about the level of plant awareness is the prerequisite for effective botany education in schools, at university or within informal learning settings, like botanical gardens. By developing a plant awareness questionnaire, we aim to facilitate biology educators around the world in fostering their students’ plant awareness.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank C. Heidinger as well as E. Schönbrunner and two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on this paper.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Adamo, M., M. Chialva, J. Calevo, F. Bertoni, K. Dixon, and S. Mammola. 2021. “Plant Scientists’ Research Attention Is Skewed Towards Colourful, Conspicuous and Broadly Distributed Flowers.” Nature Plants 7 (5): 574–578. doi:10.1038/s41477-021-00912-2.
  • Ädel, A. 2017. “Remember that Your Reader Cannot Read Your Mind: Problem/Solution-Oriented Metadiscourse in Teacher Feedback on Student Writing.” English for Specific Purposes 45: 54–68. Elsevier. doi:10.1016/j.esp.2016.09.002.
  • Ahi, B., V. Atasoy, and S. Balci. 2018. “An Analysis of Plant Blindness in Turkish Textbooks Used at the Basic Education Level.” Journal of Baltic Science Education 17 (2): 277–287. doi:10.33225/jbse/18.17.277.
  • Allen, W. 2003. “Plant Blindness.” BioScience 53 (10): 926. doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2003)053[0926:PB]2.0.CO;2.
  • Amprazis, A., and P. Papadopoulou. 2020. “Plant Blindness: A Faddish Research Interest or A Substantive Impediment to Achieve Sustainable Development Goals?” Environmental Education Research 26 (8): 1065–1087. doi:10.1080/13504622.2020.1768225.
  • Amprazis, A., P. Papadopoulou, and G. Malandrakis. 2021. “Plant Blindness and Children’s Recognition of Plants as Living Things: A Research in the Primary Schools Context.” Journal of Biological Education 55 (2): 139–154. doi:10.1080/00219266.2019.1667406.
  • Anderson, J. L., J. P. Ellis, and A. M. Jones. 2014. “Understanding Early Elementary Children’s Conceptual Knowledge of Plant Structure and Function through Drawings.” CBE—Life Sciences Education 13 (3): 375–386. Am Soc Cell Biol. doi:10.1187/cbe.13-12-0230.
  • Attenborough, D. 1995. The Private Life of Plants: A Natural History of Plant Behaviour. Princeton: Princeton University Press. http://press.princeton.edu/titles/5702.html.
  • Austrian Agency for Research Integrity. 2016. Guidelines for Good Scientific Practice. Vienna: Austrian Agency for Research Integrity. https://oeawi.at/richtlinien/.
  • Barman, C. R., M. Stein, S. McNair, and N. S. Barman. 2006. “Students’ Ideas about Plants & Plant Growth.” The American Biology Teacher 68 (2): 73–79. doi:10.1662/0002-7685(2006)068[0073:SIAPPG]2.0.CO;2.
  • Batke, S. P., T. Dallimore, and J. Bostock. 2020. “Understanding Plant Blindness – Students’ Inherent Interest of Plants in Higher Education.” Journal of Plant Sciences 8 (4): 98–105. doi:10.11648/j.jps.20200804.14.
  • Bebbington, A. 2005. “The Ability of A-Level Students to Name Plants.” Journal of Biological Education 39 (2): 63–67. doi:10.1080/00219266.2005.9655963.
  • Balas, B., and J. L. Momsen. 2014. Attention ‘Blinks’ Differently for Plants and Animals.“ CBE—Life Sciences Education, edited by E. A. Holt, 13: 437–443. doi:10.1187/cbe.14-05-0080
  • Bofferding, L., and M. Kloser. 2015. “Middle and High School Students’ Conceptions of Climate Change Mitigation and Adaptation Strategies.” Environmental Education Research 21 (2): 275–294. doi:10.1080/13504622.2014.888401.
  • Borsos, É., E. Borić, and M. Patocskai. 2021. “What Can Be Done to Increase Future Teachers’ Plant Knowledge?” Journal of Biological Education 1–11. doi:10.1080/00219266.2021.1909632.
  • Brown, T. A. 2015. Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research. New York: Guilford publications.
  • Campbell, N. A., J. B. Reece, L. A. Urry, M. L. Cain, S. A. Wasserman, P. V. Minorsky, and R. B. Jackson. 2015. Campbell Biologie. 10th ed. London: Pearson Studium.
  • Colon, J., N. Tiernan, S. Oliphant, A. Shirajee, J. Flickinger, H. Liu, J. Francisco-Ortega, and M. McCartney. 2020. “Bringing Botany into Focus: Addressing Plant Blindness in Undergraduates through an Immersive Botanical Experience.” BioScience 70 (10): 887–900. doi:10.1093/biosci/biaa089.
  • Cooper, C. L. 2008. “Botanical Knowledge of a Group of South Carolina Elementary School Students.” Ethnobotany Research and Applications 6: 121–128. doi:10.17348/era.6.0.121-127.
  • Core-Team, R. 2013. “R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
  • Dillon, J., M. Rickinson, K. Teamey, M. Morris, M. Y. Choi, D. Sanders, and P. Benefield. 2006. “The Value of Outdoor Learning: Evidence from Research in the UK and Elsewhere”. School Science Review 87 (320): 107–111.
  • DiStefano, C., M. Zhu, and D. Mindrila. 2009. “Understanding and Using Factor Scores: Considerations for the Applied Researcher.” Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation 14 (1).
  • Drea, S. 2011. “The End of the Botany Degree in the UK.” Bioscience Education 17 (1): 1–7. doi:10.3108/beej.17.2.
  • Driver, R., A. Squires, P. Rushworth, and V. Wood-Robinson. 2014. Making Sense of Secondary Science. Abingdon: Routledge. doi:10.4324/9781315747415.
  • Elster, D. 2007. “Student Interests — The German and Austrian ROSE Survey.” Journal of Biological Education 42 (1): 5–10. doi:10.1080/00219266.2007.9656100.
  • Fančovičová, J., and P. Prokop. 2010. “Development and Initial Psychometric Assessment of the Plant Attitude Questionnaire.” Journal of Science Education and Technology 19 (5): 415–421. doi:10.1007/s10956-010-9207-x.
  • Fančovičová, J., and P. Prokop. 2011. “Plants Have a Chance: Outdoor Educational Programmes Alter Students’ Knowledge and Attitudes Towards Plants.” Environmental Education Research 17 (4): 537–551. doi:10.1080/13504622.2010.545874.
  • Frisch, J. K., M. M. Unwin, and G. W. Saunders. 2010. “Name that Plant! Overcoming Plant Blindness and Developing a Sense of Place Using Science and Environmental Education.” In The Inclusion of Environmental Education in Science Teacher Education, 143–157. Berlin: Springer.
  • Ganong, W. F. 1906. “The Erroneous Physiology of the Element of Text-Books.” School Science and Mathematics 6 (4): 297–302. doi:10.1111/j.1949-8594.1906.tb00971.x.
  • Gebhard, U. 2001. Kind Und Natur. Berlin: Springer.
  • Harker, M. E., S. Dean, and J. J. Monsen. 2017. “Solution-Oriented Educational Psychology Practice.” In Frameworks for Practice in Educational Psychology: A Textbook for Trainees and Practitioners, 167–193. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
  • Hassan, S., A. Aldosari, A. Ali, and H. J. de Boer. 2015. “Indigenous Knowledge of Folk Medicines among Tribal Minorities in Khyber Pakhtunkhwa, Northwestern Pakistan.” Journal of Ethnopharmacology 166 (May): 157–167. doi:10.1016/j.jep.2015.03.022.
  • Hershey, D. R. 1992. “Making Plant Biology Curricula Relevant.” BioScience 42 (3): 188–191. doi:10.2307/1311824.
  • Hershey, D. R. 1993. “Plant Neglect in Biology Education.” Bioscience 43 (7): 418. doi:10.2307/1311898.
  • Howard, T., H. Ougham, and D. Sanders. 2022. “Plant Blindness and Sustainability.” International Journal of Sustainability in Higher Education 23 (1): 41–57. doi:10.1108/IJSHE-09-2020-0335.
  • Jose, S. B., W. Chih-Hang, and S. Kamoun. 2019. “Overcoming Plant Blindness in Science, Education, and Society.” Plants, People, Planet 1 (3): 169–172. doi:10.1002/ppp3.51.
  • Kaasinen, A. 2019. “Plant Species Recognition Skills in Finnish Students and Teachers.” Education Sciences 9 (2): 85. doi:10.3390/educsci9020085.
  • Kissi, L., and D. Dreesmann. 2018. “Plant Visibility through Mobile Learning? Implementation and Evaluation of an Interactive Flower Hunt in a Botanic Garden.” Journal of Biological Education 52 (4): 344–363. doi:10.1080/00219266.2017.1385506.
  • Kline, R. B. 2016. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. 4. ed. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Kristi, B., K. M. Parsley, and J. L. Sabel. 2021. “An Analysis of Plant Awareness Disparity within Introductory Biology Textbook Images.” Journal of Biological Education.
  • Krosnick, S. E., J. C. Baker, and K. R. Moore. 2018. “The Pet Plant Project: Treating Plant Blindness by Making Plants Personal.” The American Biology Teacher 80 (5): 339–345. doi:10.1525/abt.2018.80.5.339.
  • Krüger, D., and A. Burmester. 2005. “Wie Schüler Pflanzen Ordnen.” Zeitschrift Für Didaktik Der Naturwissenschaften 11: 85–102.
  • Kubiatko, M., J. Fančovičová, and P. Prokop. 2021. “Factual Knowledge of Students about Plants Is Associated with Attitudes and Interest in Botany.” International Journal of Science Education 43 (9): 1426–1440. doi:10.1080/09500693.2021.1917790.
  • Lampert, P., B. Müllner, P. Pany, M. Scheuch, and M. Kiehn. 2020. “Students’ Conceptions of Plant Reproduction Processes.” Journal of Biological Education 54 (2): 213–223. doi:10.1080/00219266.2020.1739424.
  • Laura, C., and D. C. Dreesmann. 2022. “SAD but True: Species Awareness Disparity in Bees Is a Result of Bee-Less Biology Lessons in Germany.” Sustainability 14 (5): 2604. doi:10.3390/su14052604.
  • Lindemann‐Matthies, P. 2005. “‘Loveable’ Mammals and ‘Lifeless’ Plants: How Children’s Interest in Common Local Organisms Can Be Enhanced through Observation of Nature.” International Journal of Science Education 27 (6): 655–677. doi:10.1080/09500690500038116.
  • Lindemann-Matthies, P. 2006. “Investigating Nature on the Way to School: Responses to an Educational Programme by Teachers and Their Pupils.” International Journal of Science Education 28 (8): 895–918. doi:10.1080/10670560500438396.
  • Little, T. D., D. W. Slegers, and N. A. Card. 2006. “A non-arbitrary method of identifying and scaling latent variables in SEM and MACS models.“ Structural Equation Modeling 13 (1): 59–72.
  • Lohr, V. I., and C. H. Pearson-Mims. 2005. “Children’s Active and Passive Interactions with Plants Influence Their Attitudes and Actions toward Trees and Gardening as Adults.” HortTechnology 15 (3): 472–476. doi:10.21273/HORTTECH.15.3.0472.
  • Lughadha, N., S. P. B. Eimear, T. C. C. Leão, F. Forest, J. M. Halley, J. Moat, C. Acedo, et al. 2020. “Extinction Risk and Threats to Plants and Fungi.” Plants, People, Planet 2 (5): 389–408. doi:10.1002/ppp3.10146.
  • MacKenzie, M., S. K. Caitlin, R. S. Barak, M. Bletz, J. Dudney, B. M. McGill, M. A. Nocco, T. Young, and R. K. Tonietto. 2019. “We Do Not Want to ‘Cure Plant Blindness’ We Want to Grow Plant Love.” Plants, People, Planet 1 (3): 139–141. doi:10.1002/ppp3.10062.
  • Marmaroti, P., and D. Galanopoulou. 2006. “Pupils’ Understanding of Photosynthesis: A Questionnaire for the Simultaneous Assessment of All Aspects.” International Journal of Science Education 28 (4): 383–403. doi:10.1080/09500690500277805.
  • Messig, D., and J. Groß. 2018. “Understanding Plant Nutrition—The Genesis of Students’ Conceptions and the Implications for Teaching Photosynthesis.” Education Sciences 8 (3): 132. doi:10.3390/educsci8030132.
  • Monahan, A. C. 1930. “Science and Its Recognition in the High School Curriculum.” School Science and Mathematics 30 (8): 875–880. doi:10.1111/j.1949-8594.1930.tb02795.x.
  • Moormann, A., A. Lude, and A. Möller. 2021. “Wirkungen von Naturerfahrungen Auf Umwelteinstellungen Und Umwelthandeln.” In Naturerfahrung Und Bildung, 57–78. Berlin: Springer.
  • Mung, B., and K. J. H. Williams. 2016. “Plant Blindness and the Implications for Plant Conservation.” Conservation Biology 30 (6): 1192–1199. doi:10.1111/cobi.12738.
  • Muthén, L. K., and B. Muthen. 2017. Mplus User’s Guide: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables, User’s Guide. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén.
  • New, J., L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby. 2007. “Category-Specific Attention for Animals Reflects Ancestral Priorities, Not Expertise.“ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 (42): 16598–16603.
  • Nichols, G. E. 1919. “The General Biology Course and the Teaching of Elementary Botany and Zoology in American Colleges and Universities.” Science 50 (1301): 509–517. doi:10.1126/science.50.1301.509.
  • Nyberg, E., I. Brkovic, and D. Sanders. 2021. “Beauty, Memories and Symbolic Meaning: Swedish Student Teachers Views of Their Favourite Plant and Animal.” Journal of Biological Education 55 (1): 31–44. doi:10.1080/00219266.2019.1643761.
  • Olsen, C. 2018. “Teaching Informed Argument for Solution-Oriented Citizenship.” English Journal 107 (3): 93–99.
  • Palmberg, I., I. Berg, E. Jeronen, S. Kärkkäinen, P. Norrgård-Sillanpää, C. Persson, R. Vilkonis, and E. Yli-Panula. 2015. “Nordic–Baltic Student Teachers’ Identification of and Interest in Plant and Animal Species: The Importance of Species Identification and Biodiversity for Sustainable Development.” Journal of Science Teacher Education 26 (6): 549–571. doi:10.1007/s10972-015-9438-z.
  • Pany, P. 2014. “Students’ Interest in Useful Plants: A Potential Key to Counteract Plant Blindness.” Plant Science Bulletin 60 (1): 18–27.
  • Pany, P., and C. Heidinger. 2017. “Useful Plants as Potential Flagship Species to Counteract Plant Blindness.” In Cognitive and Affective Aspects in Science Education Research, 127–140. Berlin: Springer.
  • Pany, P., A. Lörnitzo, L. Auleitner, C. Heidinger, P. Lampert, and M. Kiehn. 2019. “Using Students’ Interest in Useful Plants to Encourage Plant Vision in the Classroom.” Plants, People, Planet 1 (3): 261–270. doi:10.1002/ppp3.43.
  • Parsley, K. M. 2020. “Plant Awareness Disparity: A Case for Renaming Plant Blindness.” Plants, People, Planet 2 (6): 598–601. doi:10.1002/ppp3.10153.
  • Parsley, K. M. 2021. “Exploring New Approaches to the Problem of Plant Awareness Disparity in Undergraduate Students.” PhD Thesis, University of Memphis.
  • Bloom, P., and F. C. Keil. 2001. Thinking Through Language.“ Mind & Language. 16 (4): 351–367.
  • Pedrera, O., U. Ortega, A. Ruiz-González, J. Ramón Díez Díez, and O. Barrutia. 2021. “Branches of Plant Blindness and Their Relationship with Biodiversity Conceptualisation among Secondary Students.” Journal of Biological Education 1–27. doi:10.1080/00219266.2021.1933133.
  • Roberson, E. B., A. Frances, K. Havens, J. Maschinski, A. Meyer, and L. Ott. 2020. “Fund Plant Conservation to Solve Biodiversity Crisis.“ Science, 367 (6475): 258. doi:10.1126/science.aba4360
  • Ryplova, R., and J. Pokorny. 2020. “Saving Water for the Future via Increasing Plant Literacy of Pupils.” European Journal of Sustainable Development 9 (3): 313. doi:10.14207/ejsd.2020.v9n3p313.
  • Sanders, D., B. Eriksen, C. MacHale Gunnarsson, and J. Emanuelsson. 2022. “Seeing the Green Cucumber: Reflections on Variation Theory and Teaching Plant Identification.” Plants, People, Planet 4 (3): 258–268. doi:10.1002/ppp3.10248.
  • Sarada, K., T. Moreau, J. Kuehny, A. Novy, S. L. Greene, and C. K. Khoury. 2019. “Resetting the Table for People and Plants: Botanic Gardens and Research Organizations Collaborate to Address Food and Agricultural Plant Blindness.” Plants, People, Planet 1 (3): 157–163. doi:10.1002/ppp3.34.
  • Schussler, E. E., M. A. Link-Pérez, K. M. Weber, and V. H. Dollo. 2010. “Exploring Plant and Animal Content in Elementary Science Textbooks.” Journal of Biological Education 44 (3): 123–128. doi:10.1080/00219266.2010.9656208.
  • Schussler, E. E., and L. A. Olzak. 2008. “It’s Not Easy Being Green: Student Recall of Plant and Animal Images.” Journal of Biological Education 42 (3): 112–119. doi:10.1080/00219266.2008.9656123.
  • Stagg, B. 2020. Developing a Pedagogy for Reducing ‘Plant Blindness’. University of Exeter.
  • Strgar, J. 2007. “Increasing the Interest of Students in Plants.” Journal of Biological Education 42 (1): 19–23. doi:10.1080/00219266.2007.9656102.
  • Topping, K. J., C. Peter, P. Stephen, and M. Whale. 2004. “Cross-Age Peer Tutoring of Science in the Primary School: Influence on Scientific Language and Thinking.” Educational Psychology 24 (1): 57–75. doi:10.1080/0144341032000146449.
  • Tunnicliffe, S. D., and M. J. Reiss. 2000. “Building a Model of the Environment: How Do Children See Plants?” Journal of Biological Education 34 (4): 172–177. doi:10.1080/00219266.2000.9655714.
  • Uno, G. E. 2009. “Botanical Literacy: What and How Should Students Learn about Plants?” American Journal of Botany 96 (10): 1753–1759. doi:10.3732/ajb.0900025.
  • Villarroel, J. D., A. Antón, D. Zuazagoitia, and T. Nuño. 2018. “Young Children’s Understanding of Plant Life: A Study Exploring Rural–Urban Differences in Their Drawings.” Journal of Biological Education 52 (3): 331–341. doi:10.1080/00219266.2017.1385505.
  • Wandersee, J. H., and E. Schussler. 2001. “Toward a Theory of Plant Blindness.” Plant Science Bulletin 47 (1): 2–9.
  • Watts, D. J. 2017. “Should Social Science Be More Solution-Oriented?” Nature Human Behaviour 1 (1): 1–5. doi:10.1038/s41562-016-0015.
  • Yorek, N., M. Şahin, and H. Aydın. 2009. “Are Animals ‘More Alive’ than Plants? Animistic-Anthropocentric Construction of Life Concept.” EURASIA Journal of Mathematics, Science and Technology Education 5 (4): 371–380. doi:10.12973/ejmste/75287.
  • Zani, G., and J. Low. 2022. “Botanical Priming Helps Overcome Plant Blindness on a Memory Task”. Journal of Environmental Psychology 81. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2022.101808.