35,241
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Study Habits and Procrastination: The Role of Academic Self-Efficacy

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 1141-1160 | Received 28 Jun 2020, Accepted 08 Jul 2021, Published online: 03 Aug 2021

ABSTRACT

Inefficient study skills increase the probability that study work is perceived as difficult and aversive, with procrastination as a likely result. As a remedy, more effective study skills and habits may be encouraged. However, research indicates that good study skills and habits may not by themselves be sufficient to remedy problems, as this relationship may be mediated by efficacy beliefs related to academic functioning. We investigated this hypothesis across three student samples (total N = 752). As predicted, structural equation modeling (SEM) indicated that study self-efficacy mediated the study habits—procrastination relation. The mediation effects were medium to large. We conclude that training of, and advice on, study skills and habits should be accompanied by measures that build study self-efficacy.

University students confront a challenging situation as they enter academic life, as adapting to a relatively unguided and complex educational environment requires skills and competencies related to study work, planning, and others. However, only a minority of students have received instruction on such skills (e.g., Dunlosky & Rawson, Citation2015; Dunlosky et al., Citation2013). Although research on effective study skills is becoming increasingly more available, universities seem to be slow in adopting them (Goffe & Kauper, Citation2014; Wieman & Gilbert, Citation2015). Moreover, academic work also benefits from skills related to planning, organization of own learning, and self-motivation, generally referred to as strategies for self-regulated learning (SRL; e.g., Pintrich & De Groot, Citation1990; Zimmerman, Citation1990). As is the case for study skills, SRL strategies are not normally part of the study curriculum, and when they are offered, it is often in one-off seminars. Still, they are important for academic success (Kreber et al., Citation2005). In sum, many students do not possess the sufficient levels of skills and competencies needed for efficient academic work, negatively affecting academic performance and retention (Robbins et al., Citation2004).

In the absence of formal training in study skills and skills related to SRL, academic staff and advisors resort to a more straightforward solution—they advise students on behaviors and habits beneficial in the study situation. Such advice is often provided at lectures and seminars, with summaries occasionally published on university websites. For example, our university has published a list of smart study habits, recommending study habits such as practicing self-test, preparing before lectures, and participating actively in seminars and discussion groups. Such advice cannot replace formal training, but it is an easy way of communicating insights from research, with an expectation that students following the advice presented will be better off in their academic work.

However, even when students possess knowledge of sound study habits beneficial to academic work, they may not practice them (e.g., Jairam, Citation2019). For example, in a study of university students in Austria, Foerst et al. (Citation2017) found a discrepancy between students’ knowledge of SRL strategies and their actual use of such strategies. Specifically, even if students demonstrated knowledge of SRL strategies, they did not necessarily put this knowledge effectively into action. Foerst et al. (Citation2017) traced the discrepancy between knowledge and actual use of effective skills to several sources, such as lack of time and doubt about their effectiveness. Notably, one reason for this gap reported by the students was a lack of perceived ability to use such strategies. Thus, it seems that knowledge of efficient study habits is a necessary but not sufficient factor for practicing them effectively. Students’ efficacy beliefs in their capabilities to carry out, organize, and perform student skills successfully (e.g., Bandura, Citation1997; Pajares & Valiante, Citation1997; Zimmerman, Citation1990) may be vital in translating knowledge of efficient study habits into action (Schunk, Citation2012).

Efficacy beliefs are positively and moderately related to academic outcomes (e.g., grades) but demonstrate considerable heterogeneity and complex relations to other relevant variables (e.g., Honicke & Broadbent, Citation2016, for review). This complexity is to be expected, as academic self-efficacy affects outcomes in direct as well as indirect ways. For example, self-efficacy for self-regulated learning, closely related to self-efficacy for academic achievement (Zimmerman et al., Citation1992) helps the student to accomplish long-term tasks through the use of self-regulation strategies such as self-monitoring, self-evaluation, goal setting, and planning (Zimmerman, Citation1990). An important characteristic of self-efficacy is domain-specificity (Bandura, Citation1997), meaning that efficacy beliefs (confidence in achieving a desired outcome) relate to a specific domain (e.g., academic efficacy beliefs) that do not easily generalize.

In the present paper, we focus on these topics from the perspective of another issue challenging the success of a student, procrastination—the habit of voluntarily putting off tasks despite expecting to be worse off (Steel, Citation2007). Students are especially prone to dilatory behavior (Schouwenburg, Citation2004; Tice & Baumeister, Citation1997), often delaying academic tasks unnecessarily (Pychyl et al., Citation2000). Procrastination is maladaptive in the long run, with negative consequences such as missing deadlines (Zarick & Stonebraker, Citation2009), increased stress and anxiety (Tice & Baumeister, Citation1997), lower academic achievement (Kim & Seo, Citation2015), and dropping out of studies (Grau & Minguillon, Citation2013). Prior research has documented a relatively strong negative relation of procrastination with self-efficacy and self-efficacy to self-regulate (Klassen et al., Citation2008), and lack of academic skills and self-regulated learning strategies are often listed as reasons for not starting intended tasks in time (Grunschel et al., Citation2013; Klingsieck et al., Citation2013; Van Eerde, Citation2003).

However, the question of how the use of study skills is affected by self-efficacy beliefs in the context of procrastination has not received much attention in the research literature. Specifically, if students do not receive formal training in study skills, they will likely perceive study tasks as difficult, with increased procrastination as a predictable outcome (Grunschel et al., Citation2013; Klingsieck et al., Citation2013; Schraw et al., Citation2007). When universities then offer advice on sound study habits, adopting such habits should make study work appear as easier, with reduced procrastination as a likely outcome. However, as discussed, that effect should be expected to be dependent on the self-efficacy beliefs that students hold toward their study work. Some students may follow study advice without necessarily believing that their efforts will succeed, whereas others may hold stronger study self-efficacy beliefs. It is not known whether such self-efficacy differences influence the effect of practicing recommended study habits, but we find it likely. Given the extensive literature on self-efficacy as an important factor or moderator variable in many forms of motivated behavior, including self-regulation (e.g., Bandura, Citation1997; Klassen et al., Citation2008), it may be expected that habit execution is also moderated by self-efficacy beliefs. For example, Prat-Sala and Redford (Citation2010) demonstrated that students low in self-efficacy in academic work (reading and writing tasks) were more likely to adopt a surface approach (less time and effort put into school-related work), whereas those high in self-efficacy adopted a deep or strategic approach to studying (more time and effort).

The Present Studies

In three studies, we assess the importance of academic study habits in procrastination, given study self-efficacy as a possible mediating factor. Because Norwegian universities do not provide formal study skills training but rather convey advice regarding recommended study habits, we approached this issue by asking students to report their use of such recommended habits in their study situation. We compiled a list of five habits often recommended by teaching staff and advisors into an index, a Study Skill Habits (SSH) scale. Examples are “I practice self-testing” and “Before every lecture, I prepare by making myself familiar with the topic.” Students who endorse more of these statements should be able to manage a variety of academic challenges better than students who endorse fewer of these statements. Thus, we expected that the SSH scale should demonstrate a positive correlation with study performance (e.g., self-reported grades). Furthermore, as procrastination is more likely when facing difficult and aversive tasks, we expected—consistent with prior research (Grunschel et al., Citation2013; Jung, Citation2013; Klingsieck et al., Citation2013; Schraw et al., Citation2007)—that students scoring low on the SSH scale would also demonstrate an increased probability of academic procrastination.

However, as discussed, even if students practice relatively healthy study habits, the students’ beliefs in the efficacy of executing these habits, their study self-efficacy, may tell a different story. Specifically, lower study self-efficacy may hamper performance, dictate lower ambitions, reduce effort and persistence (Bandura, Citation1997), and, in sum, represents a handicap for the student even when practicing recommended study habits. Hence, even if there is an overall negative relationship between the study habit measure and academic procrastination, that link could be affected by study self-efficacy.

These relationships were investigated across three studies with study skill habits and study self-efficacy used as predictors of procrastination. We had two expectations for the present data: First, the Study Skill Habits and Study Self-Efficacy measures should be negatively correlated with procrastination (Ferrari et al., Citation1992; Haycock et al., Citation1998; Steel, Citation2007; Tuckman, Citation1991; Wolters, Citation2003). Second, given the literature discussed, we expect that Study Self-Efficacy significantly mediates the effect of Study Skill Habits on procrastination. In Study 1, relatively young students from a single study discipline participated; Studies 2 and 3 included a more diverse range of students, varying in study experience (Study 2) and academic discipline (Study 3). In this way, the roles of Study Skill Habits and Study Self-Efficacy were assessed in relatively heterogeneous samples, ensuring the robustness of findings.

Method

Participants

Three samples were included in the present paper. In Study 1, 86 students (76.7% female) from an introductory psychology course participated. Most were first-year students invited to participate via a closed website (total number of students was approximately 140). Age ranged from 18–41 with a mean age of 21.14 years (SD = 3.45). Participants in Study 2 were 483 students (68.7% female) in different stages of their studies at the university: first year (22.5%), second-year (23.4%), third-year (26.5%), fourth-year (13.3), fifth-year (7%) and six years or more (7.3%). Age ranged from 19–55, with most being less than 26 years old (70%), with a mean of 24.9 years (SD= 5.74). Finally, participants in Study 3 were 183 students (67.6% female) studying medicine/odontology (65%), humanities/social sciences (17.5%) and natural sciences/other (17.5%). Age ranged from 19–57, most being less than 26 years old (90.2%), with a mean age of 22.47 years (SD = 5.40). Participants in Studies 2 and 3 were recruited through lectures, invitations on open university websites, and social media announcements via student assistants.

Materials

Study Skill Habits (SSH)

We developed a custom scale focusing on study habits, Study Skill Habits, based on advice typically given at Norwegian universities. All authors discussed possible items to include and agreed on a list containing assertions addressing skills that have been demonstrated to be effective (e.g., “I test myself in the material I read”) as well as study habits actively encouraged by teaching staff without specific research basis (e.g., “I am active in seminars and study groups”). Items rated on a five-point Likert scale (1–5), with higher scores indicating more usage of study recommended study habits. See Appendix for the complete list of questions.

Items in this scale address a variety of different indicators that sum up to a formative construct (Roberts & Thatcher, Citation2009). Constructs can be termed reflective or formative depending on the nature and direction of relationships between a construct and its indicators. Reflective indicators represent reflections or manifestations of a latent construct, which means that variation in the construct leads to variation in its indicators. That is, constructs are viewed as causes of reflective indicators, and indicators are interchangeable implying that removal of an indicator does not change the construct. Hence, internal consistency among indicators is expected. On the other hand, constructs can be formed or induced by their indicators. Such indicators are termed formative indicators and are viewed as causes of the constructs. Commonly, formative constructs are regarded as composites of specific component variables or dimensions. Indicators are not interchangeable, and omitting an indicator is omitting part of the construct. Therefore, correlations among indicators may not have a specific pattern that produces internal consistency.

To determine whether a construct should be regarded as reflective or formative, decision rules can be applied (Jarvis et al., Citation2003). Roberts and Thatcher (Citation2009) describe these rules as (1) to assess the theoretical causal direction from the construct to indicators; (2) to examine the interchangeability of the indicators; (3) to assess if the indicators covary with one another; (4) to determine whether or not the indicators have the same antecedents and consequences. In the present context, the Study Skill Habits (SSH) measure, which encompasses a variety of different behaviors, may not be appropriately specified as reflective indicators. For example, the SSH includes different indicators referring to self-testing, working with fellow students, and preparation before lectures, making it quite evident that the construct includes indicators that are formative in nature. As formative indicators are not expected to be highly correlated (opposite of reflective indicators), Diamantopoulos and Siguaw (Citation2006) suggest addressing the issue of a formative or reflective model by testing for multicollinearity among indicators. The variance inflation factor (VIF) statistic can help determine if the formative indicators are too highly correlated (i.e., a VIF value greater than 3.3 indicates high multicollinearity among formative indicators) and, thus, should be modeled as reflective indicators (or both). In the current three studies, the highest VIF is 2.46, and most VIF values are below 1.70. Therefore, the Study Skill Habits measure is specified as a formative construct, indicated by the causal direction going from the indicators to the construct (see Appendix, Studies 1–3).

Self-Ratings of Study Skills

Studies 1 and 2 also included a question asking respondents to evaluate the quality of their study skills: “I think that I have good study skills” rated on a scale from 1–5 (1= “does not apply at all to me”—5= “applies very well to me”). This item constituted an independent alternative measure of study skills. Due to few respondents at the first and last levels, levels 1–2 and 4–5 were merged, resulting in a measure of three levels (i.e., 1= does not apply well to me; 2= applies sometimes; 3= “applies very well to me”). The self-rated study skill item correlated positively with the SSH scale (Study 1, r = .39; Study 2, r = .44).

Self-Reported Grades

We also recorded self-reported grades (range 1-6, higher numbers = better grades). As expected, the SSH scale correlated positively with grades (Study1, r = .44; Study 2, r = 24, Study 3, r = .25), indicating support for the assumption that adherence to advice about study habits is positively associated with performance (e.g., Robbins et al., Citation2004).Footnote1 Similarly, the self-rated study skill item correlated positively with grades (Study 1, r = .35; Study 2, r = .30).

Study Self-Efficacy scale (SSE)

This scale measures students’ confidence in their ability to achieve desired academic outcomes. Items were adapted from the general self-efficacy scale by Schwarzer and Jerusalem (Citation1995), rephrased to tap academic self-efficacy specifically. Items addressed confidence in the utility of study skill habits (items 1, 2, 3, i.e., “study habit self-efficacy”), general outcome expectations (items 4 and 6), as well as one persistence item (5). We avoided explicit comparisons to other students (cf. the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire, MSLQ; Pintrich & De Groot, Citation1990), and items were formulated to address academic tasks but still intended to remain neutral to specific study contents. In Study 1, items included were (1) “When I get a study task to work with, I have a hard time finding a solution,” (2) “I have little faith in my ability to study effectively,” and (3) “It is difficult for me to follow the study curriculum when something unexpected happens.” Three items were added in Studies 2 and 3: (4) “I am capable of learning the course contents for this year,” (5) “When I have decided to complete something important to me, I continue even if it proves more difficult than I believed,” and (6) “I am sure that I will accomplish the academic goals I have set for myself.” Items were rated on a five-point Likert scale (1–5), higher scores indicating higher academic self-efficacy (three first items reverse coded). Cronbach’s alphas across the three studies were .63, .75, and .66, respectively. Of note, Honicke and Broadbent (Citation2016, p. 67) pointed out that higher levels of internal reliability in self-efficacy measures are observed in content-specific scales compared to more global measures. In the present studies, item 5 demonstrated the lowest factor loadings (.33 in Study 2; .28 −.30 in Study 3).

Procrastination

All studies measured procrastination by the six non-reversed items from the Irrational Procrastination Scale (IPS, Steel, Citation2010) using the Norwegian version translated by Svartdal (Citation2017). Items are rated on a five-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more procrastination. These items have been documented to measure procrastination similarly to the full scale (Svartdal & Steel, Citation2017). The IPS often is taken to measure trait procrastination, and as such, reflects a relatively stable tendency to delay unnecessarily. To be used as a dependent variable in the present context, it must be assumed that this scale reflects procrastination in the study context (i.e., measures academic procrastination) and that answers in principle can be affected by the predictor variables. As for the first requirement, studies (e.g., Steel et al., Citation2018) have demonstrated a high correlation between the IPS and more direct measures of academic procrastination. Also, several of the items in the IPS address delays of activities that are important to the person, which for students include academic work. Thus, examination of the individual items of the IPS reveals that most items address habitual, context-specific tendencies to put things off (e.g., item 5 “At the end of the day, I know I could have spent the time better”). Hence, for students asked to rate the items in an academic context, this scale should tap academic procrastination. This assumption was further assessed in Study 3, which included both academic procrastination and the IPS scales. The correlation between the IPS and the academic procrastination scale was r = .85. Second, the IPS has been used previously as an indicator of changed procrastination after interventions (e.g., Rozental et al., Citation2015), suggesting that this scale can reflect changes when controlling variables are changed.Footnote2 Cronbach’s alpha ranged from .90 – .94 across the three studies.

Academic Procrastination Scale

In Study 3, a subset of six items from the Academic Procrastination Scale (APS; McCloskey & Scielzo, Citation2015; Yockey, Citation2016) measured academic procrastination (e.g., “I get distracted by other, more fun, things when I am supposed to work on schoolwork”). The items were translated to Norwegian with backward translations and discussion/correction (Nordby, unpublished). All scale items are rated on a five-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating more procrastination. Cronbach alpha was .88.

Procedure and Ethics

Respondents answered all questions in a web-based survey (www.qualtrics.com). Participation was anonymous and voluntary. All were informed that they could withdraw at any time and agreed to participate by actively pressing a start survey button after reading general information about the study. Only completed surveys were included.

The current project is part of a study on procrastination with ethical approvals from the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics in Northern Norway (REC North 2014/2313).

Model Specification and Estimation

The conceptual model, shown in , assumes that the influence of Study Skill Habits on academic procrastination is mediated by Study Self-Efficacy. The SSH construct is specified as a formative latent construct, whereas SSE and procrastination are specified as reflective latent constructs. In Studies 1 and 2, sensitivity analysis was employed using an alternative measure of study skills (i.e., “I think that I have good study skills”) that was specified as the observed independent variable. Responses were “does not apply well to me” (1), “applies sometimes” (2), and “applies very well to me” (3). Gender (Male= 0; Female= 1) was included as a control variable in all studies, as gender differences have been observed in procrastination (e.g., Gröpel & Steel, Citation2008; Mandap, Citation2016; Steel & Ferrari, Citation2013; Washington, Citation2004), study skills (e.g., Ekuni et al., Citation2020; Khan & Rasheed, Citation2019), and self-efficacy (e.g., Huang, Citation2013). In Study 2, university experience (first year= 1; second year= 2; third year= 3; fourth year= 4; fifth year or more= 5) was added as a control variable. As the factors included in our model may be affected by study experience, it is of great interest to assess the relations between these variables among students with varying degrees of study experience. For example, deep and strategic approaches to learning have been shown to be affected by study experience (e.g., Brown & Murdolo, Citation2016; Richardson, Citation2010), and the effect of study self-efficacy tend to vary as a predictor of performance at early versus later study stages (e.g., Gore, Citation2006; Phan, Citation2013; Zeegers, Citation2004). Procrastination also differs as a function of study experience. For example, in a study by Stewart et al. (Citation2016), procrastination levels were higher in the second year than first-year students. Finally, in Study 3, study topic (Medicine/odontology= 1; social sciences/humanities= 2; Natural sciences/other = 3) was added as a control variable. Previous research (Nordby et al., Citation2017) has demonstrated that students from various study disciplines (e.g., medicine, social sciences, humanities) differ in procrastination, motivating a closer assessment of the factors included in our model over different study fields. Hence, study discipline, in addition to gender, was included as control variables in Study 3.

Figure 1. Conceptual model. SSH = Study Skill Habits; SSE = Study Self-Efficacy; PROC = Academic procrastination.

Figure 1. Conceptual model. SSH = Study Skill Habits; SSE = Study Self-Efficacy; PROC = Academic procrastination.

Post-hoc power analysis (Kenny, Citation2017), given the sample size (Study1, n = 85; Study 2, n = 483; Study 3. n = 183), an alpha level of .05, and the betas in the model revealed a power level virtually at 1, except for the direct path c’ that was .754 (Study 1) and .789 and .125 (Study 3).

A structural equation model using weighted least squares parameter (WLSMV) estimation was employed. The WLSMV estimation is appropriate when manifest variables are categorical or ordinal. Model fit to data was examined using standard fit indices, i.e., chi-square test, the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). CFI and TLI values greater than 0.95 and an SRMR less than 0.08 indicate good fit (Hu & Bentler, Citation1999), and RMSEA less than 0.05 indicate close fit (MacCallum et al., Citation1996). Standardized parameter estimates across main variables are reported with bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals based on 10000 bootstrap draws (MacKinnon et al., Citation2004). However, since the interpretation of standardized estimates of categorical variables is difficult, only the continuous outcome variable was standardized for the sensitivity analysis. For control variables, unstandardized estimates are reported. Missing values were left open, with pairwise deletion of cases. In line with Preacher and Kelley (Citation2011), kappa-squared (k2) values at 0.01, 0.09, and 0.25 are interpreted as small, medium, and large mediation effect sizes, respectively. All analyses were performed with Mplus version 8.1.

Results and Discussion

Study 1

presents the means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations between procrastination (IPS), Study Skill Habits (SSH), and Study Self-Efficacy (SSE). As expected, there was a negative correlation between the outcome variable procrastination and the predictor variables SSH (r = −0.49) and SSE (r = −0.59), and a positive correlation between SSH and SSE (r = 0.42).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations.

The overall model fit was very good, chi-square= 77.942, df = 72, p = 0.296, CFI= 0.996; TLI= 0.995; RMSEA = 0.031 (0.000–0.071); SRMR = 0.065. The model results are presented in . The direct effect from SSH to SSE was significant (β = 0.560, SE = 0.140, p < .001), indicating that self-efficacy increases as a function of study skills, whereas procrastination decreases as a function of self-efficacy (β = −0.603, SE = 0.158, p < .001). The direct effect from SSH to procrastination was non-significant (β = −0.236, boot SE = .205, p = .250), while the indirect effect of study skills on procrastination via SSE was significant (β = −0.219, 95% Bias-corrected CI [−0.662, −0.093], boot SE = 0.152, p = .027), indicating that SSE fully mediates the relationship between SSH and procrastination. Gender was not a significant predictor. The mediation effect was large (k2 = .35).

Table 2. Standardized model estimates (n = 85).

Overall, as is seen in , these results support the conceptual model depicting that the effect of Study Skill Habits on procrastination is facilitated by Study Self-Efficacy. In effect, good Study Skill Habits by themselves are not enough to reduce academic procrastination. Study Self-Efficacy is a crucial component of how study habits impact academic procrastination.

Results from a sensitivity analysis specifying the item “I think I have good study skills” as the independent variable revealed similar results both in terms of model fit (i.e., chi-square = 66.760, df = 47, p = .031; CFI = 0.986; TLI = 0.981; RMSEA = 0.070 (90% CI 0.022–0.106); SRMR = 0.058) and structural relationships. Model estimates are reported in Appendix ().

Study 2

Study 1 examined a relatively small sample of young students from an introductory course in psychology. Study 2 used a larger sample with study experience ranging from short (first-year students) to long (more than five years). All variables were measured with similar scales as in Study 1, except that Study 2 added three items to the Study Self-Efficacy scale (see Methods section).

Descriptive statistics and correlations are displayed in . Compared to Study 1, mean scores were quite similar across variables, but Study Self-Efficacy was higher. This makes sense considering that the Study 1 sample comprised first-year psychology students, whereas Study 2 included students with long experience as well. Correlations were also similar to those of Study 1 in that SSH and SSE were negatively correlated with procrastination and positively correlated with each other.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics and correlations.

The conceptual model produced a significant chi-square test (chi-square = 291.923., df = 110, p < .001). However, the chi-square test statistic is commonly significant in larger samples (Hooper et al., Citation2008). Other alternative fit indices indicated that the model produces a good fit to the data, CFI = 0.989; TLI = 0.987; RMSEA = 0.043 (0.035-0.051); SRMR = 0.074. As seen in , SSH was positively associated with SSE (β = 0.415, boot SE = 0.062, p < .001), which, in turn, was negatively associated with procrastination (β = −.529, boot SE = 0.057, p < .001). The direct effect of SSH on procrastination was significant (β = −0.220, boot SE = .058, p < .001). The indirect effect of study skills on procrastination via SSE was significant (p < .001), β = −0.220, boot SE = 0.044, 95% bias-corrected CI [−0.309, −0.137]. This represents a medium effect size (k2 = .23).

Table 4. Standardized model estimates (N = 483).

Overall, Study 2 repeated the findings from Study 1, further supporting the notion that study self-efficacy is an important factor that facilitates the effect of Study Skill Habits on academic procrastination. However, in the present results, the direct SSH—procrastination effect remained significant, whereas Study 1 indicated full mediation.

The results revealed that study experience had no effect on procrastination (p > .05), but Study Self-Efficacy generally increased with increasing study experience. This result was significant for the more experienced students (i.e., four years at university, β = 0.546, p = .002; five years or more at university, β = 0.839, p < .001), corresponding well to previous research (e.g., Gore, Citation2006; Phan, Citation2013; Zeegers, Citation2004). Also, gender was a significant predictor of procrastination (β = 0.285, p = .001) and of self-efficacy (β = 0.314, p < .01), indicating more procrastination and higher self-efficacy among males.

Results from sensitivity analysis specifying the item “I think I have good study skills” as the independent variable revealed similar results in terms of model fit (i.e., chi-square = 246.912, df = 120. p < .001; CFI = 0.989; TLI = 0.986; RMSEA = 0.047 (0.038–0.055); SRMR = 0.047). Estimates among main variables were also similar to those in the main model (see Appendix, Table 2). Similar to the above results, study year was a significant predictor of SSE for the most experienced students (i.e., fourth year at university β = 0.313, p = .051; fifth year at university, β = 0.625, p < .001).

Study 3

In Studies 1 and 2, procrastination was measured by the IPS (Steel, Citation2010). This scale addresses habitual, context-specific tendencies to put things off. As discussed, administering this scale in an academic context should tap academic procrastination. In Study 3, we included a scale that measures academic procrastination specifically, allowing us to assess the relationship between these two procrastination measures. A high correlation between them would support the assumption made in Studies 1 and 2 that IPS is a valid measure of academic procrastination. Also, participants for this study were selected from rather diverse fields of study.

Descriptive statistics and correlations are displayed in . Of particular interest here is the high correlation between general procrastination (IPS) and academic procrastination (APS), r = .85, indicating that IPS is a context-specific measure reflecting academic procrastination when administered in the study context. Note that the APS scores were markedly lower compared to the IPS scores, indicating that IPS scores may be somewhat exaggerated when used as an index of academic procrastination.

Table 5. Descriptive statistics and correlations.

The conceptual model, using APS at the dependent variable, produced good fit to the data: chi-square = 169.330 df = 136, p = .03, CFI = 0.970; TLI = 0.964; RMSEA = 0.037 (0.013–0.054); SRMR = 0.074. The direct, indirect, and total effects are shown in . SSE increased as a function of SSH (β = 0.462, boot SE = 0.088, p < .001), which was in turn is negatively related to procrastination (β = −0.664, boot SE = 0.104, p = .001). The direct effect of SSH to procrastination was marginally significant (β = −0.217, boot SE = .110, p = .049). This indirect effect of SSH on procrastination via SSE was significant (β = −0.307, 95% Bias-corrected CI [−0.484 −0.126], SE = 0.097, p < .001). This represents a large effect, k2 = .32.

Table 6. Standardized model estimates (n = 180).

Similar results were found when IPS was applied as a measure of procrastination. Chi-square = 188.451, df = 137, p = .002, CFI = 0.976; TLI = 0.971; RMSEA = 0.046 (0.028–0.061); SRMR = 0.073. Estimates are reported in Appendix (). SSE increased as a function of SSH (β = 0.485, boot SE = 0.085, p < .001), which was in turn negatively related to procrastination (β = −0.804, SE = 0.105, p < .001). The direct effect from SSH to procrastination was non-significant (β = −0.057, SE = .120, p = .633). The indirect effect of study skills on procrastination (IPS) via SSE was significant (β = −0.390, 95% bias-corrected CI [−0.541, −0.268], boot SE = 0.108, p < .001), which represents a large effect, k2 = .40.

As for the control variables in both models, study discipline was a significant predictor of academic procrastination when measured by APS (β = 0.410, p = .03), indicating more procrastination among social science students, but non-significant measured by IPS. Gender was significant in both models when predicting procrastination (p < .001) and Study Self-Efficacy (p < .001), indicating more procrastination and higher self-efficacy among males.

In summary, Study 3 repeated the results of Studies 1 and 2, indicating support for the conceptual model suggesting that study self-efficacy mediates the study skill habit – procrastination relation. In Study 3, results were similar using an academic procrastination scale (APS) and a trait procrastination measure (IPS) as outcome variables, indicating support for the appropriateness of using IPS as a measure of academic procrastination in Studies 1 and 2. However, note the higher mean scores for the IPS scale compared to the APS. This probably reflects the fact that APS scores focus on academic tasks specifically.

General Discussion

Academic skills are important for academic performance. Unfortunately, many students do not possess the sufficient levels of academic skills and competencies needed for efficient academic work, negatively affecting their academic performance as well as the likelihood of completing their studies (Richardson et al., Citation2012; Robbins et al., Citation2004). The logical remedy for this problem would be to provide explicit training in study-related skills, but universities instead often rely on advising students on study habits believed to be of utility in the study situation. However, research (e.g., Foerst et al., Citation2017) has demonstrated a discrepancy between students’ knowledge of SRL strategies and their actual use of such strategies: Even if students possess knowledge and skills of useful study strategies, they do not necessarily put this knowledge into action. One key factor for translating study skills into action is study self-efficacy (Klassen et al., Citation2008), the beliefs students have in their ability to plan and implement student activities successfully (Bandura, Citation1997; Pajares & Valiante, Citation1997; Zimmerman, Citation1990).

The current research explored these issues in the context of procrastination. First, we document that low study skills (in the present studies, low adherence to recommended study habits compiled in a Study Skill Habits index, SSH) were associated with increased procrastination. In three studies, we observed moderate negative correlations (−.38–−.49) between these measures. This result follows predictably from the assumption that low study skills make academic work appear difficult, boring, or even aversive. As difficult, boring, and aversive tasks are well-documented predictors of procrastination (Grunschel et al., Citation2013; Klingsieck et al., Citation2013; Schraw et al., Citation2007; Steel, Citation2007), the negative relation between the Study Skill Habits measure and procrastination is consistent with prior research.

As a remedy to this situation, study skills training, or – as in the present paper – adherence to recommended study habits, may be introduced. However, study habits are not automatically translated into good study performance, as study self-efficacy may be vital in translating knowledge of efficient study habits into action (e.g., Bandura, Citation1997; Pajares & Valiante, Citation1997; Schunk, Citation2012; Zimmerman, Citation1990). Using SEM, we tested a model proposing that study self-efficacy mediates the observed study habits – procrastination relation. The sample included in Study 1 was relatively homogeneous, whereas participants in Studies 2 and 3 varied in experience and study fields. All three studies indicated support for the model; Studies 1 and 3 indicated full mediation, whereas Study 2 indicated partial mediation. Thus, these results indicate that although Study Skill Habits index is negatively related to procrastination, one key factor in this relationship is study self-efficacy. To our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate that the study habits → procrastination relation is dependent on study self-efficacy beliefs.

One implication of the present results is that study skills training, as well as advice on recommended study strategies, should be accompanied by measures to increase study self-efficacy. Just sharing information on effective study habits is not enough. However, whereas academic skills are relatively easy to train, efficacy beliefs in the academic context are not easily trainable. Unfortunately, academic self-efficacy is related to preceding academic achievement (e.g., Bartimote-Aufflick et al., Citation2016; Diseth, Citation2011), making a negative academic history an effective detrimental factor for student performance. A negative academic history may reduce or even neutralize efforts to enhance study skills. Low self-efficacy also negatively affects ambition, motivation, and persistence (Bandura, Citation1997), putting students with low self-efficacy in an unfavorable situation compared to their student fellows. Fortunately, intervention studies indicate that educational programs may enhance self-efficacy (e.g., Van Dinther et al., Citation2011). These authors reviewed studies that demonstrate positive effects of intervention efforts over different study types and domains. Interventions based on social cognitive theory demonstrated the best results, and enactive mastery experiences seemed to be important for success (Bandura, Citation1997). Also, combined self-efficacy sources are reported as effective in increasing student self-efficacy (Van Dinther et al., Citation2011, p. 105). Bartimote-Aufflick et al. (Citation2016, p. 1930) suggest specific strategies that may enhance study self-efficacy.

Increasing the probability that students, in fact, have mastery experiences is important. Such self-efficacy training should also be specific and closely related to the nature of the learning tasks, how they are framed, and focus on positive habit formation and strategies for which self-efficacy beliefs are important (Bandura, Citation1986). Skills need to be practiced in the proper context in order to be mastered, and teaching students how to implement different study skills should therefore be an integrated part of the various subjects students learn (Purdie & Hattie, Citation1999). Accordingly, when educators plan to train students in study skills, such training should be closely related to specific study programs (Weinstein et al., Citation2000), and skills training should ensure feedback and mastery experiences, thereby building self-efficacy beliefs.

Limitations and Further Research

The relationships between study skills, study self-efficacy, and procrastination are complex, and the model tested in the present studies (see ) is one of several possible. For example, efficacy beliefs may themselves affect the use of study strategies (Diseth, Citation2011; Phan, Citation2011). Also, procrastination has been demonstrated to be negatively related to academic performance (Kim & Seo, Citation2015; see also Footnote 1 in the present paper), with procrastination measure, performance indicator, type of data (self-report vs. external observation), and demographic profile of the study sample as important moderator variables. The potential role of study self-efficacy was not examined in the Kim and Seo paper, but other research (e.g., Balkis, Citation2011) has demonstrated study self-efficacy as a moderator variable. The results of the present paper indicate that study self-efficacy should receive increased attention as a moderator or mediator variable in studies examining performance and performance-related factors in the academic context. Furthermore, our model is also a simplification, as (academic) procrastination, self-efficacy, and academic skills are complex constructs related to other factors important to student work, including value, motivation, and metacognition (e.g., Bartimote-Aufflick et al., Citation2016; Cerino, Citation2014; Steel & Klingsieck, Citation2016).

Some additional limitations of the present studies should be noted. First, the Study Self-Efficacy scale used included items adapted from a general self-efficacy scale, modified to specifically tap study habit self-efficacy. While the internal consistency was satisfactory in Studies 2 and 3, the low Cronbach alpha in Study 1 indicates that the results of this study should be interpreted with caution. Second, the Study Skill Habits measure used in the present studies is a simplified proxy of study skills. Although this measure correlated predictably with study performance (self-reported grades), it should not be seen as an alternative to scales addressing study skills. On the positive side, our measure is probably an ecologically valid measure of students’ willingness to practice recommended study habits, which is the operational definition of “study skills” as practiced by many universities. Third, as most of the measures used in the present studies have not been assessed for measurement invariance (cf. Brown, Citation2015; Gregorich, Citation2006), results should be interpreted with some caution. In the present studies, threats to measurement invariance include study field differences and differences due to levels study experience. For example, it is possible that the understanding of items addressing study self-efficacy may depend on study experience and/or study field, so that a given item (e.g., “When I get an assignment to work with, I have a hard time finding a solution”) is understood differently depending on these variables. Future research should address this issue.

The possibility that study skills training itself may increase study self-efficacy (e.g., Wernersbach et al., Citation2014) should also be explored. As noted, universities and high schools should train students in basic study skills, ensuring that skills training is accompanied by mastery experiences. Such training requires repeated sessions of active student participation and feedback for success experiences that can help establish new habits as well as an understanding of when and why they are used (McCabe, Citation2011; Verplanken, Citation2006). Note that reliance on the use of advice on study habits only does not secure such a deeper understanding. Future research should explore appropriate interventions, preferably in close concert with specific study programs. In these efforts, the situational and contextual factors in academic student life should be taken into account. Universities often arrange academic environments as “procrastination friendly,” especially for beginning students in open study programs (Svartdal et al., Citation2020). A large degree of individual freedom for the student, long deadlines, and ample opportunities to divert attention from academic tasks to more tempting alternatives easily induce procrastination, maybe especially so in students low in study skills and/or academic self-efficacy. Future studies should examine the role of such variables and the possibility of arranging academic life with less situational and contextual opportunities to procrastinate.

Disclosure Statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Notes

1 Although not part of the present study, we note that the correlation between self-reported grades and procrastination (IPS) confirmed to prior research (Kim & Seo, Citation2015), with correlations across the three studies at r = −.22, −.35, and −.29. The correlations between study self-efficacy and grades were r = .51, .65, and .48.

2 Scales measuring academic procrastination may include no or very few items addressing academic tasks. For example, the GPS – probably the most used scale to measure procrastination (see Svartdal & Steel, Citation2017) – has 20 items, and comes in two versions: One general, and a student version which includes 4 unique “academic procrastination” items. Thus, the general and student versions have 16 non-academic items in common. Similarly, an often used student procrastination scale, the Tuckman scale (35 items, often reduced to a 16-item scale based on the top loading items from the complete scale) has no items that specifically address academic procrastination (item 7 mentions studying, though: “I put the necessary time into even boring tasks, like studying”). These observations indicate that academic and general procrastination are very similar constructs, and that a valid assessment of academic procrastination is possible using a general procrastination scale focusing on implemental delay, like the IPS.

References

  • Balkis, M. (2011). Academic efficacy as a mediator and moderator variable in the relationship between academic procrastination and academic achievement. Egitim Arastirmalari – Eurasian Journal of Educational Research, 45, 1–16. http://acikerisim.pau.edu.tr:8080/xmlui/handle/11499/5762
  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice Hall.
  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. WH Freeman.
  • Bartimote-Aufflick, K., Bridgeman, A., Walker, R., Sharma, M., & Smith, L. (2016). The study, evaluation, and improvement of university student self-efficacy. Studies in Higher Education, 41(11), 1918–1942. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2014.999319
  • Brown, T. A. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research (2nd Edn). The Guilford Press.
  • Brown, T., & Murdolo, Y. (2016). Approaches to study across four year-levels of undergraduate occupational therapy students: Similar or different? British Journal of Occupational Therapy, 79(12), 752–761. https://doi.org/10.1177/0308022616662482
  • Cerino, E. S. (2014). Relationships between academic motivation, self-efficacy, and academic procrastination. Psi Chi Journal of Psychological Research, 19(4), 156–163. https://doi.org/10.24839/2164-8204.JN19.4.156
  • Diamantopoulos, A., & Siguaw, J. A. (2006). Formative versus reflective indicators in organizational measure development: A comparison and empirical illustration. British Journal of Management, 17(4), 263–282. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8551.2006.00500.x
  • Diseth, A. (2011). Self-efficacy, goal orientations and learning strategies as mediators between preceding and subsequent academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(2), 191–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.003
  • Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2015). Practice tests, spaced practice, and successive relearning: Tips for classroom use and for guiding students’ learning. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 1(1), 72–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000024
  • Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., & Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: Promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266
  • Ekuni, R., de Souza, B. M. N., Agarwal, P. K., & Pompeia, S. (2020). A conceptual replication of survey research on study strategies in a diverse, non-WEIRD student population. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000191
  • Ferrari, J. R., Parker, J. T., & Ware, C. B. (1992). Academic procrastination: Personality correlates with Myers-Briggs types, self-efficacy, and academic locus of control. Journal of social Behavior and personality, 7(3), 495–502.
  • Foerst, N. M., Klug, J., Jöstl, G., Spiel, C., & Schober, B. (2017). Knowledge vs. Action: Discrepancies in university students’ knowledge about and self-reported use of self-regulated learning strategies. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1288. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01288
  • Goffe, W. L., & Kauper, D. (2014). A survey of principles instructors: Why lecture prevails. The Journal of Economic Education, 45(4), 360–375. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220485.2014.946547
  • Gore, P. A. (2006). Academic self-efficacy as a predictor of college outcomes: Two incremental validity studies. Journal of Career Assessment, 14(1), 92–115. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072705281367
  • Grau, J., & Minguillon, J. (2013). When procrastination leads to dropping out: Analysing students at risk. eLearn Center Research Paper Series, 63–74.
  • Gregorich, S. E. (2006). Do self-report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the confirmatory factor analysis framework. Medical Care, 44(11 Suppl. 3), S78–S94. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.mlr.0000245454.12228.8f
  • Gröpel, P., & Steel, P. (2008). A mega-trial investigation of goal setting, interest enhancement, and energy on procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(5), 406–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.015
  • Grunschel, C., Patrzek, J., & Fries, S. (2013). Exploring reasons and consequences of academic procrastination: An interview study. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28(3), 841–861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0143-4
  • Haycock, L. A., McCarthy, P., & Skay, C. L. (1998). Procrastination in college students: The role of self-efficacy and anxiety. Journal of Counseling & Development, 76(3), 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1998.tb02548.x
  • Honicke, T., & Broadbent, J. (2016). The influence of academic self-efficacy on academic performance: A systematic review. Educational Research Review, 17, 63–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2015.11.002
  • Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6(1), 53–60. https://academic-publishing.org/index.php/ejbrm/article/view/1224
  • Hu, L.-t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
  • Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 28(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-011-0097-y
  • Jairam, D. (2019). First-year seminar focused on study skills: An ill-suited attempt to improve student retention. Journal of Further and Higher Education, 44(4), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877x.2019.1582757
  • Jarvis, C. B., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, P. M. (2003). A critical review of construct indicators and measurement model misspecification in marketing and consumer research. Journal of Consumer Research, 30(2), 199–218. https://doi.org/10.1086/376806
  • Jung, K. R. (2013). The mediational effect of academic self-discipline (ASD) between academic self-efficacy (ASE) and college GPA [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. University of Minnesota. http://hdl.handle.net/11299/157688
  • Kenny, D. A. (2017). MedPower: An interactive tool for the estimation of power in tests of mediation [Computer software]. https://davidakenny.shinyapps.io/PowerMed/
  • Khan, M. J., & Rasheed, S. (2019). Moderating role of learning strategies between meta-cognitive awareness and study habits among university students. Pakistan Journal of Psychological Research: PJPR, 34(Spring 2019), 215–231. https://doi.org/10.33824/PJPR.2019.34.1.12
  • Kim, K. R., & Seo, E. H. (2015). The relationship between procrastination and academic performance: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 82, 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.038
  • Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., & Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of undergraduates: Low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procrastination. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 33(4), 915–931. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.07.001
  • Klingsieck, K. B., Grund, A., Schmid, S., & Fries, S. (2013). Why students procrastinate: A qualitative approach. Journal of College Student Development, 54(4), 397–412. https://doi.org/10.1353/csd.2013.0060
  • Kreber, C., Castleden, H., Erfani, N., & Wright, T. (2005). Self-regulated learning about university teaching: An exploratory study. Teaching in Higher Education, 10(1), 75–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/1356251052000305543
  • MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130–149. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130
  • MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., & Williams, J. (2004). Confidence limits for the indirect effect: Distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(1), 99–128. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2821115/. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4
  • Mandap, C. M. (2016). Examining the differences in procrastination tendencies among university students. International Journal of Education and Research, 4(4), 431–436. http://ww.w.ijern.com/journal/2016/April-2016/35.pdf
  • McCabe, J. (2011). Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates. Memory & Cognition, 39(3), 462–476. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-010-0035-2
  • McCloskey, J., & Scielzo, S. A. (2015). Finally!: The development and validation of the academic procrastination scale. Manuscript submitted for publication. https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Shannon-Scielzo/publication/273259879_Finally_The_Development_and_Validation_of_the_Academic_Procrastination_Scale/links/54fcfb3d0cf20700c5e9c735/Finally-The-Development-and-Validation-of-the-Academic-Procrastination-Scale.pdf
  • Nordby, K., Klingsieck, K., & Svartdal, F. (2017). Do procrastination-friendly environments make students delay unnecessarily? Social Psychology of Education, 20(3), 491–512. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9386-x
  • Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1997). Influence of self-efficacy on elementary students’ writing. The Journal of Educational Research, 90(6), 353–360. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1997.10544593
  • Phan, H. P. (2011). Interrelations between self-efficacy and learning approaches: A developmental approach. Educational Psychology, 31(2), 225–246. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2010.545050
  • Phan, H. P. (2013). Theoretical constructs that explain and enhance learning: A longitudinal examination. Higher Education Research & Development, 32(6), 1007–1021. https://doi.org/10.1080/07294360.2013.806445
  • Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 33–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33
  • Prat-Sala, M., & Redford, P. (2010). The interplay between motivation, self-efficacy, and approaches to studying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(2), 283–305. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709909X480563
  • Preacher, K. J., & Kelley, K. (2011). Effect size measures for mediation models: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psychological Methods, 16(2), 93–115. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022658
  • Purdie, N., & Hattie, J. (1999). The relationship between study skills and learning outcomes: A meta-analysis. Australian Journal of Education, 43(1), 72–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/000494419904300106
  • Pychyl, T. A., Morin, R. W., & Salmon, B. R. (2000). Procrastination and the planning fallacy: An examination of the study habits of university students. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15(5), 135.
  • Richardson, J. T. (2010). Perceived academic quality and approaches to studying in higher education: Evidence from danish students of occupational therapy. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 54(2), 189–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831003637972
  • Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 353–387. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026838
  • Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130(2), 261–288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261
  • Roberts, N., & Thatcher, J. (2009). Conceptualizing and testing formative constructs: Tutorial and annotated example. ACM Sigmis Database: The Database for Advances in Information Systems, 40(3), 9–39. https://doi.org/10.1145/1592401.1592405
  • Rozental, A., Forsell, E., Svensson, A., Andersson, G., & Carlbring, P. (2015). Internet-based cognitive—behavior therapy for procrastination: A randomized controlled trial.. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83(4), 808–824. https://doi.org/10.1037/ccp0000023
  • Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004). Procrastination in academic settings: General introduction. In H. C. Schouwenburg, C. H. Lay, T. A. Pychyl, & J. R. Ferrari (Eds.), Counseling the procrastinator in academic settings (pp. 3–17). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10808-001
  • Schraw, G., Wadkins, T., & Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: A grounded theory of academic procrastination. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(1), 12–25. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.12
  • Schunk, D. H. (2012). Social cognitive theory. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, T. Urdan, C. B. McCormick, G. M. Sinatra, & J. Sweller (Eds.), APA educational psychology handbook, Vol. 1. Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 101–123). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13273-005
  • Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs, 1(1), 35–37. http://userpage.fu-berlin.de/~health/engscal.htm
  • Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychological Bulletin, 133(1), 65–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65
  • Steel, P. (2010). Arousal, avoidant and decisional procrastinators: Do they exist? Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 926–934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.025
  • Steel, P., & Ferrari, J. (2013). Sex, education and procrastination: An epidemiological study of procrastinators’ characteristics from a global sample. European Journal of Personality, 27(1), 51–58. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1851
  • Steel, P., & Klingsieck, K. B. (2016). Academic procrastination: Psychological antecedents revisited. Australian Psychologist, 51(1), 36–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/ap.12173
  • Steel, P., Svartdal, F., Thundiyil, T., & Brothen, T. (2018). Examining procrastination across multiple goal stages: A longitudinal study of temporal motivation theory. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00327
  • Stewart, M., Stott, T., & Nuttall, A. M. (2016). Study goals and procrastination tendencies at different stages of the undergraduate degree. Studies in Higher Education, 41(11), 2028–2043. https://doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2015.1005590
  • Svartdal, F. (2017). Measuring procrastination: Psychometric properties of the Norwegian versions of the Irrational Procrastination Scale (IPS) and the pure procrastination scale (PPS). Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 61(1), 18–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/00313831.2015.1066439
  • Svartdal, F., Dahl, T. I., Gamst-Klaussen, T., Koppenborg, M., & Klingsieck, K. B. (2020). How study environments foster academic procrastination: Overview and recommendations. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 540910. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.540910
  • Svartdal, F., & Steel, P. (2017). Irrational delay revisited: Examining five procrastination scales in a global sample. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1927. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01927
  • Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8(6), 454–458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x
  • Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the procrastination scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51(2), 473–480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164491512022
  • Van Dinther, M., Dochy, F., & Segers, M. (2011). Factors affecting students’ self-efficacy in higher education. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 95–108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2010.10.003
  • Van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(6), 1401–1418. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00358-6
  • Verplanken, B. (2006). Beyond frequency: Habit as mental construct. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45(3), 639–656. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X49122
  • Washington, J. A. (2004). The relationship between procrastination and depression among graduate and professional students across academic programs: Implications for counseling. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.
  • Weinstein, C. E., Husman, J., & Dierking, D. R. (2000). Self-regulation interventions with a focus on learning strategies. In Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 727–747). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50051-2
  • Wernersbach, B. M., Crowley, S. L., Bates, S. C., & Rosenthal, C. (2014). Study skills course impact on academic self-efficacy. Journal of Developmental Education, 14–33. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24614030
  • Wieman, C., & Gilbert, S. (2015). Taking a scientific approach to science education, part I-research. https://sei.ubc.ca/bitstream/seima/2184/1/Wieman-Gilbert_ScienceEd-pt1_Microbe_2015.pdf
  • Wolters, C. A. (2003). Understanding procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 179. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.179
  • Yockey, R. D. (2016). Validation of the short form of the academic procrastination scale. Psychological Reports, 118(1), 171–179. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294115626825
  • Zarick, L. M., & Stonebraker, R. (2009). I’ll do it tomorrow: The logic of procrastination. College Teaching, 57(4), 211–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/87567550903218687
  • Zeegers, P. (2004). Student learning in higher education: A path analysis of academic achievement in science. Higher Education Research & Development, 23(1), 35–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/0729436032000168487
  • Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An overview. Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 3–17. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2501_2
  • Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. American Educational Research Journal, 29(3), 663–676. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312029003663

Appendix

Study Skill Habits (SSH). English/Norwegian

  1. I test myself in the material I read/Jeg tester meg selv i det stoffet jeg leser

  2. I reread material I have read before/Jeg leser om igjen ting jeg har lest før

  3. Before each lecture I prepare myself by getting acquainted with the material/Før hver forelesning forbereder jeg meg ved å gjøre med kjent med stoffet

  4. I am active in seminars and study groups/Jeg er aktiv på seminarer og forelesninger

  5. I practice understanding difficult technical terms by explaining them to myself or others/Jeg trener på å forstå vanskelige begreper ved å forklare for meg selv eller andre

Study Efficacy Scale. English/Norwegian. (* = added in Studies 2 and 3)

  1. When I get an assignment to work with, I have a hard time finding a solution/Når jeg får en studieoppgave å jobbe med, sliter jeg med å finne løsning

  2. I have little faith in my abilities to study effectively/Jeg har liten tiltro til mine evner til å studere effektivt

  3. It is difficult for me to follow the study curriculum when something unexpected happens/Det er vanskelig for meg å følge leseplanen når noe uventet skjer

  4. * I am capable of learning this year’s course content/Jeg er i stand til å lære det som blir undervist i år

  5. * When I’ve decided to do something important to me, I keep working at it even when it is harder than I anticipated./Når jeg har bestemt meg for å gjennomføre noe som er viktig for meg, så fortsetter jeg å prøve, selv om det er vanskeligere enn jeg trodde

  6. * I am certain that I can achieve the academic goals I have set for myself/Jeg er sikker på at jeg klarer å oppnå de akademiske målene jeg har satt for meg selv

Six-item Version of Irrational Procrastination Scale. English/Norwegian

  1. I put things off so long that my well-being or efficiency unnecessarily suffers/Jeg utsetter ting så lenge at det går ut over velvære og effektivitet

  2. My life would be better if I did some activities or tasks earlier/Livet mitt ville vært bedre om jeg hadde gjort ting tidligere

  3. When I should be doing one thing, I will do another/Når jeg burde gjøre noe, gjør jeg gjerne noe annet i stedet

  4. At the end of the day, I know I could have spent the time better/Når jeg ser tilbake på dagen, vet jeg at jeg kunne utnyttet tiden bedre

  5. I delay tasks beyond what is reasonable/Jeg venter med å gjøre ting mer enn hva som er fornuftig

  6. I procrastinate/Jeg utsetter ting

Appendix: Study 1

Appendix: Study 2

Appendix, Study 3 (APS)

Appendix, Study 3 (IPS)

Table A1. Sensitivity analysis—model estimates (n = 85).

Table A2. Sensitivity analysis—model estimates (n = 483).

Table A3. Standardized model estimates (n = 180).