846
Views
83
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

It’s not which school but which set you’re in that matters: the influence of ability grouping practices on student progress in mathematicsFootnote1

&
Pages 279-293 | Received 01 Jun 2002, Accepted 01 Jul 2003, Published online: 19 Oct 2010
 

Abstract

The mathematics achievement of a cohort of 955 students in 42 classes in six schools in London was followed over a 4‐year period, until they took their General Certificate of Secondary Education examinations (GCSEs) in the summer of 2000. All six schools were regarded by the Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted) as providing a good standard of education, and all were involved in teacher training partnerships with universities. Matched data on Key Stage 3 test scores and GCSE grades were available for 709 students, and these data were analysed in terms of the progress from Key Stage 3 test scores to GCSE grades. Although there were wide differences between schools in terms of overall GCSE grades, the average progress made by students was similar in all six schools. However, within each school, the progress made during Key Stage 4 varied greatly from set to set. Comparing students with the same Key Stage 3 scores, students placed in top sets averaged nearly half a GCSE grade higher than those in the other upper sets, who in turn averaged a third of a grade higher than those in lower sets, who in turn averaged around a third of a grade higher than those students placed in bottom sets. In the four schools that used formal whole‐class teaching, the difference in GCSE grades between top and bottom sets, taking Key Stage 3 scores into account, ranged from just over one grade at GCSE to nearly three grades. At the schools using small‐group and individualized teaching, the differences in value‐added between sets were not significant. In two of the schools, a significant proportion of working‐class students were placed into lower sets than would be indicated by their Key Stage 3 test scores.

Notes

* Corresponding author: ETS, Rosedale Road (ms 04‐R), Princeton, NJ 08541, USA. Email: ­[email protected]

Paper presented at the 27th annual conference of the British Educational Research Association, University of Leeds, September 2001.

In a box and whisker plot, the box represents the attainment of the middle half of the data, with the line indicating the value of the median. The whiskers extend far enough to include most of the remaining data (specifically, the whiskers extend far enough to encompass 99.5% of normally distributed data).

Our experience has been that it is difficult to collect reliable data on parental occupation ­without actually visiting classrooms and collecting the data ourselves. We asked students to provide information on the jobs done by parents or guardians, or, if they were out of work, what job they did when they last worked. An indication of the problematic nature of the data is provided by one incident when we collected information at Redwood School. A girl asked one of us (DW) for help as she didn’t know what to put for her father’s job. When asked, ‘What does your father do?’, the girl replied, ‘He’s a waiter, but when we were in Iran, he was a professor of history’.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Dylan Wiliam Footnote*

* Corresponding author: ETS, Rosedale Road (ms 04‐R), Princeton, NJ 08541, USA. Email: ­[email protected]

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

There are no offers available at the current time.

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.