4,636
Views
56
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Web Paper

Tacit knowledge and visual expertise in medical diagnostic reasoning: Implications for medical education

Pages e184-e188 | Published online: 03 Jul 2009

Abstract

Background: Much education—especially at the university level—has been criticized for having primarily dealt with explicit knowledge, i.e. those aspects of mental activities, which are verbal and conscious. Furthermore, research in medical diagnostic reasoning has been criticized for having focused on the specialty of intern medicine, while specialties with other skills, i.e. perceptive skills within pathology and radiology, have been ignored.

Aims: To show that the concept of tacit knowledge is important in medical education—at all levels and in medical diagnostic reasoning.

Methods: Describing how tacit knowledge according to Michael Polany, is experienced and expressed in day-to-day life, it is shown that there is a tacit dimension to all knowledge. Reviewing recent literature on medical diagnostic reasoning, it is shown that tacit knowledge is recognized in connection with concepts such as “non-analytical reasoning” and “dual process of reasoning.”

Conclusion: It is important that educators are trained in how explicit and implicit knowledge is attained and that tacit knowledge is included in educational programmes of all medical specialties.

Introduction

Recent reforms in Denmark have cut the time allotted for post-graduate education for surgeons, doctors and practicing physicians. Post-graduate training programmes and the time allowed between internship (basisuddannelse) and residency (introduktionsstilling) have been greatly acceleratedFootnote1.

These reforms have been initiated in Denmark in an attempt to solve logistical problems: there is a growing shortage of physicians and of specialists, particularly in psychiatry and oncology. Thus, the reform is based on the demands of the market rather than on any coherent pedagogical methodology, although it does borrow a few tools from the Can MEDS framework.

It is on this background that vital epistemological questions about the very nature of medical knowledge and its acquisition must be raised. If we are to maintain and improve standards of post-graduate clinical education in Denmark, we need to raise questions about the very nature of clinical learning and knowledge. That is the purpose of this article.

As a post-graduate clinical tutor in the field of pathology, I have been particularly interested in how novices in my field learn to see, to reason and to diagnose. With this in mind, I chose to study current literature in a reappraisal of the work of the late Hungarian-British philosopher Michael Polanyi. Polanyi was a physician as well as a chemist and philosopher, and was interested in the layers of knowledge which presuppose explicit skills, competencies and the ability to reason. He calls these layers of knowledge the tacit dimension of knowledge. Indeed, his particular contribution to the field of epistemology, in general, and medical education, in particular, is this very concept of tacit knowledge. The concept was introduced in Personal Knowledge (Polanyi Citation1958a) and in The Tacit Dimension (Polanyi Citation1966a). When applying his theory to a criticism of clinical education today we find that, generally speaking, the understanding of knowledge today has been reduced to that of epistemic knowledgeFootnote2, which has become the ideal for knowledge per se, especially within the sciences (Wackerhausen Citation1996). However, when focusing only on explicit, epistemic knowledge, other aspects of knowledge, particularly the dimension of tacit knowledge, are overlooked. As Polanyi (Polanyi Citation1966a) writes: “We can know more than we can tell.”

In the following, I will first attempt to present a short overview of Polanyi's theory of tacit knowledge in order to relate it to medical diagnostic reasoning and diagnostic skills in the field of pathology. Here, I will compare Polanyi's tacit dimension with complementary types of visual and diagnostic skills. Finally, I will raise questions about the implications of this theory of knowledge upon clinical education in general and post-graduate pathological education in particular.

Polanyi's theory of tacit knowledge

Tacit knowledge covers those aspects of human knowledge, skills and competencies, which lie outside the domain of rules and procedures and thus, can be extremely difficult to articulate. Furthermore tacit knowledge refers to the knowledge, which is a pre-condition for any skill or competence that is in focus for its performer.

One of the most convincing examples of tacit knowledge is facial recognition. “We know a person's face, and can recognize it among a thousand, indeed a million. Yet we usually cannot tell how we recognize a face we know, so most of this cannot be put into words.” (Polanyi Citation1966b). When you see a face you are not conscious about your knowledge of the individual features (eye, nose, mouth), but you see and recognize the face as a whole. Other examples of daily activities and tacit knowledge are: riding a bike, playing the piano, driving a car, hitting a nail with a hammer. You may know explicitly how turning of the handle bars or steering wheel change the direction of a bike or car, but you cannot simultaneously focus on this and at the same time orientate yourself in traffic. Similarly, you may know explicitly how to hold the handle of a hammer, but you cannot simultaneously focus on the handle and hit the nail correctly with the hammer. The master pianist can perform brilliantly, but if he begins to concentrate on the movements of his fingers instead of the music, he will not be able to play as a master.

Polanyi says that the physical body is the basis of our knowledge, intellectual as well as practical. “All knowing is personal knowing.” (Polanyi 1969b). By this he means that all knowledge is acquired by the knower by means of physical and mental processes. This is not the same as saying that personal knowledge is subjective. However, all knowledge, both intellectual and practical, is always attached to the knower. This is a criticism of the dogma of the separation of profession and person, because, according to Polanyi, to be professionally competent and knowledgeable, we have to act with our physical being, since a part of our professional knowledge is also embodied in our physical being.

No matter what type of activity we are engaged in, we act on the basis of tacit knowledge. It makes no difference whether we employ tangible tools or intangible capabilities, since both of these enable a skilled user to get things done. To learn to use a tool or a skill properly, we need to accommodate and embody it.

Tacit knowledge and medical diagnostic reasoning

If we accept that there is a tacit dimension to all knowledge and that we act on the basis of tacit knowledge, then we must consider how this affects medical education. In their excellent articles, Henry, Goldman and Tsoukas (Goldman Citation1990; Tsoukas Citation2002; Stephen Henry Citation2006) discuss the role of tacit knowledge in medical diagnostic reasoning. Henry argues that evidence-based medicine presupposes an inaccurate and deficient view of medical knowledge and that Michael Polanyi's theory of tacit knowledge both explains this deficiency and suggests remedies for it. According to Goldman, it is a dominant view that clinical judgment is a fully explicit process compared to the relatively neglected view that tacit knowledge plays a substantial role in the clinician's mental operations. Tsoukas argues that tacit knowledge has been greatly misunderstood. Tacit and explicit knowledge are not the two ends of a continuum, but two sides of the same coin. Even the most explicit kind of knowledge is presupposed by tacit knowledge

A most convincing example of tacit knowledge in the field of medicine is Polanyis own description of a student reading an X-ray of a chest: As a beginner, the student is cognitively distant from the diagnostic task, because he only thinks of basic, relevant radiological knowledge. Later “he will gradually forget about the ribs and begin to see the lungs and eventually … a rich panorama of significant details will be revealed to him: of physiological variations and pathological changes, of scars, of chronic infections and signs of acute disease. He has entered a new world” (Polanyi Citation1958c). The student has now interiorized the radiological knowledge and “a new world” has entered him. Polanyi calls this dialectical process “in-dwelling.”

Diagnostic skills in pathology

The core skill in the specialty of pathology is the diagnosis of illnesses (macroscopically and/or microscopically) based on morphological changes in tissues and cells. Pathologists have a lot in common with radiologists and dermatologists using vision as the most important diagnostic instrument. Performing visual diagnosis has a lot in common with face recognition: As mentioned above, when you see a face, you are not conscious about your knowledge of individual features—eye, nose, mouth—but you see and recognize the face as a whole. In the same manner when you as a pathologist see cancerous tissue in the microscope, you are not conscious about your knowledge of the individual features (the morphology of the cells, the cytoplasm, nuclei and nucleoli) but you see the cancerous tissue as a whole.

One of the essential studies by Crowley et al. (Citation2003) investigated 28 beginners, intermediates and expert pathologists. They were videotaped with “thinking aloud protocols” while diagnosing well-defined histological specimens. Rebecca Crowley points out that microscopic pathology is unusual because the diagnostician's eyes must move around in different slides, from area to area, and look at different regions in different magnifications. This makes the visual classification slow, at least for the novice. The pictures must be decoded and understood in small pieces. Rebecca Crowley deals with five categories during a microscopic investigation: 1) data examination, 2) data exploration and explanation, 3) data interpretation, 4) control processes and 5) operational processes. The length of time spent on the different categories depends on whether the pathologist is a novice or an expert. She refers to the Dreyfus and Dreyfus model (2002), which describes the general development from novice to expert—from the novice's perception of parts to the expert's recognition of patterns in any field. Pattern recognition, according to Crowley, consists of converting long sequences of specific features into shorter sequences of non-verbalized pattern recognition. The pathologist's diagnostic speed and accuracy increases, while the features are learned and put down in an ever-growing knowledge bank.

In a study by Krupinsky et al. (Citation2006), an eye-tracking device was used, assessing eye-movements of medical students, pathology residents and practicing pathologists, examining virtual slides on a digital display monitor. According to image processing theory, the extraction of feature details from complex images relies on two processes: global impression and focal search. Eye position recording data suggests that, as one gains more and more experience, more relevant information is handled during the initial global impression, reducing the extent and search of focal search. The study shows, that enough information is extracted from images in the initial global impression, to reduce the need for examining all tissues with focal search.

The diagnostic process of “global impression” and “focal search” is confirmed by a recent study by Kundell et al. (Citation2007). In this study, they recorded the eye-position of nine radiologists, in the spectrum of novice to expert, who interpreted 20 normal and 20 abnormal mammograms. The results support the hypothesis that a difference exists, in the mechanism of image perception, from relatively slow search-to-find mode to a relatively fast holistic mode. The holistic mode seems to be more highly developed in the most proficient observers

Crowley, Krupinsky and Kundell describe a dual way of arriving at a visual diagnosis: The novice more often uses “focal search” or “the search-to-find mode” compared to the expert's use of “global expression” or “the holistic mode of search”. The expert, however, apparently cross-checks his/her diagnosis by using the “search-to-find-mode”. This dual way of visual diagnosis has much in common with the current discussion among cognitive scientists concerning the dual process of reasoning (Kahnemann 2002; Evans Citation2003; Citation2008): System 1 is an intuitive mode in which judgments and decisions are made automatically and rapidly, this corresponds to the holistic mode of search. System 2 is a controlled mode, which is deliberate and slower corresponding to the search-to-find mode. In a recent review, Karen Eva (Eva Citation2004) emphasizes the importance of non-analytical reasoning strategies as well as analytical reasoning strategies in medical diagnostic reasoning. Geoff Norman states that non-analytical reasoning strategies are a central component diagnostic expertise of all levels (Norman Citation2007).

The authors use different descriptive words concerning the novice's and expert's way of diagnostic resoning. Their theories have all in common the distinction between cognitive processes that are fast, automatic and unconscious and those that are slow, deliberate and conscious. shows the key words of explicit and implicit reasoning according to the authors mentioned above.

Table 1.  Key words describing the novice's and expert's preferred way of reasoning

Educational implications

When novices are learning from experts, it is not enough that the novice is presented to new knowledge through literature, courses and clinical guidelines (i.e. explicit knowledge). If such were the cases, he could learn to be a doctor without leaving the classroom. Clinical knowledge must be experienced and embodied (indwelled) as in Polanyi's description of the student's reading of an X-ray or in the pathology students’ study of a slide. Only when the novice has entered “the new world” and “a new world” has entered the novice, it is possible for her/him to fully attain the requisite competencies and to become an expert. This double process of indwelling, however, takes time and practice, as Martin Talbot points out (Talbot Citation2004) in his “critique of accelerated higher specialist training”. At what point in the doctor's career does “the new world” become second nature (embodied)? How much time, how much observation and how much practice is “enough”? Is 1 introductory year of a specialty sufficient to assess if a person is suitable for continuing on to 4 years of specialist training in a particular field? Is 4 years of specialist training sufficient to attain the requisite competencies of the field? If specialist training is accelerated too much, it may inhibit the process of indwelling, a prerequisite for progressing from novice to expert.

It is important that educators and trainees are aware of the elements of the tacit dimension (associative and holistic, pattern recognition, non-analytical reasoning strategies) and include this awareness in their educational planning, as suggested by Leron (Leron and Hazzan Citation2006). It is also important to learn how to use the analytical and non-analytical models of clinical reasoning. It is suggested that clinical teachers should stress the importance of both forms of reasoning, enabling the students to marshal reasoning processes in a flexible and context-specific manner (Eva Citation2004). Some even emphasizes the importance of explicitly empowering students to utilize multiple diagnostic strategies including non-analytical approaches (Ark et al. Citation2007). Geoff Norman concludes that teachers should recognize non-analytical reasoning as being a central component of diagnostic expertise at all levels (Norman Citation2007).

Looking at the spectrum of medical specialties, non-analytical reasoning seems to be more conspicuous in the visual specialties of pathology, radiology and dermatology as compared to the more technical specialties of surgery or anesthesiology. This is due to the fact that pathologists and radiologists rely primarily on their own (embodied) expertise. As a consequence, it is extremely important that novices of visual specialties are given enough time to be able to build up an ever-growing knowledge bank (Crowley et al. Citation2003). In terms of educational outcomes: if the competencies are not attained, either the level of competencies is too high or the amount of time available is too short. One introductory year in the specialty of pathology may be too little time to assess if a trainee is suitable for continuing on to 4 years of specialist training. How are we to determine how much time is reasonable and feasible, and at the same time allow for individual cognitive differences and learning styles?

Finally, Polanyis identification and description of tacit knowledge is recognized in recent research of medical knowledge and diagnostic reasoning (analytical/non-analytical reasoning strategies) as well as in recent cognitive research (dual process theories of judgment and decision making). There is a level of tacit, experiential learning, which trainees must attain before they can be considered to be competent. (Talbot Citation2004). However, it can be difficult to formally assess levels of tacit knowledge, as they can be only tested “in action.” If the educational reforms do not take these questions into account, the standards of post-graduate medical education may be seriously lowered.

Acknowledgements

Ruth Mach-Zagal, Department of Education and Development, Roskide Hospital, is greatly acknowledged for the inspiration and constructive criticism about the concepts of tacit knowledge

Notes

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Peter Johan Heiberg Engel

PETER ENGEL is a consultant pathologist at Roskilde Sygehus. He is also a post-graduate lecturer in Pathology at the University of Copenhagen, with special interests in medical diagnostic reasoning, visual expertise and tacit knowledge. This manuscript is based on a speech “Tacit Knowledge in the Hidden Specialty (of Pathology)” presented at the Center of Educational Development in Health Sciences, University of Copenhagen, June 14, 2006.

Notes

1. The duration of post-graduate medical training in Denmark has for several reasons often lasted 8–10 years. In 2003, a reform by the National Board of Health accelerated higher specialist training (Danish ministry of Health Citation2000): formal post-graduate training programmes have been accelerated and must define learning objectives and assessment methods based on the CanMEDS framework (CanMEDS Citation2000). Requirements of post-graduate education of today include an 18-month internship (in 2008 it is further reduced to 12 months), a 1-year introductory training in a specialty and a further 4–5-year specialty training as a resident.

2. “Epistemic knowledge.” Wackerhausen (Citation1996) bases this concept on Aristotle's concept, where episteme is the true, universal and definitive scientific knowledge, techne is the skills and crafts, which are connected to the making of things and phronesis is connected to ethically correct actions among human beings. According to Aristotle, all three kinds of knowledge are to be considered as a whole and are equally important in understanding the nature of man and his society.

References

  • Ark TK, et al. The benefits of flexibility. The pedagogical value of instructions to adopt multifaceted diagnostic reasoning strategies. Med Teach 2007; 4: 281–287
  • CanMEDS. Extract from the CanMEDS 2000. Project Societal Needs Working Group Report. Royal College of Physicians and Surgeons of Canada. Med Teach 2000; 22: 527–532
  • Crowley RS, et al. Development of visual diagnostic expertise in pathology: An information-processing study. JAMIA 2003; 10: 39–51
  • Danish Ministry of Health. The future specialist. Report from the Specialist Commission. Report No. 1385., Statens Information, Copenhagen 2000
  • Dreyfus H, Dreyfus S. Apprenticeship and experts learning (Mesterlære og eksperters læring). Apprenticeship. Learning in social context. (Mesterlære. Læring som social praksis), N Klaus, K Steinar. Hans Reitzels Publishing House, Denmark 2002; 54–76
  • Eva KW. What every teacher needs to know about clinical reasoning. Med Educ 2004; 39: 98–106
  • Evans J StBT. In two minds: Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Trends Cogn Sci 2003; 7: 454–459
  • Evans J StBT. Dual processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 2008; 59: 255–278
  • Goldman GM. The tacit dimension of clinical judgment. Yale J Biol Med 1990; 63: 47–61
  • Henry SG. Recognizing tacit knowledge in Medical Epistemology. Theor Med Bioeth 2006; 27: 87–213
  • Kahneman D. Maps of bounded rationality: A perspective on intuitive judgment and choice. Am Psychol 2002; 58: 697–720, (Review)
  • Krupinsky EA, et al. Eye-movement study and human performance using telepathology slides. Implications for medical education and differences with experience. Hum Pathol 2006; 37: 1543–1556
  • Kundell HL, et al. Holistic component of image perception in mammogram interpretation—gaze-tracking study. Radiology 2007; 242: 396–402
  • Leron U, Hazzan O. The rationality debate: Application of cognitive psychology to mathematics education. Educ Stud Math 2006; 62: 105–126
  • Norman GR. Non-analytical models of clinical reasoning: The role of experience. Med Educ 2007; 41: 1140–1145
  • Polanyi M. Towards a post-critical philosophy (Reprinted 2002). Personel knowledge. Routledge, Taylor and Francis Group, London 1958a
  • Polanyi M. Personal knowledge 1958b; 55
  • Polanyi M. Personal knowledge 1958c; 101
  • Polanyi M. The tacit dimension (Reprinted 1983). Doubleday & Company Inc., Glouchester 1966a; 4
  • Polanyi M. The tacit dimension 1966b; 4
  • Talbot M. Good wine may need to mature: A critique of accelerated higher specialist training. Evidence from cognitive neuroscience. Med Educ 2004; 38: 399–408
  • Tsoukas H. Do we really understand tacit knowledge? Presented to Knowledge Economy and Society Seminar. LSE Department of Information Systems. 14 June, 2002, 2002
  • Wackerhausen S. Tacit knowledge and education in education of the 1990ies (Tavs Viden og pædagogik i 90’ernes pædagogik), S Hegstrup. Psika Publishing House, Denmark 1996; 203–230

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.