58,377
Views
207
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Commuting and wellbeing: a critical overview of the literature with implications for policy and future research

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon show all
Pages 5-34 | Received 10 Sep 2018, Accepted 08 Jul 2019, Published online: 01 Aug 2019

ABSTRACT

This review provides a critical overview of what has been learnt about commuting’s impact on subjective wellbeing (SWB). It is structured around a conceptual model which assumes commuting can affect SWB over three time horizons: (i) during the journey; (ii) immediately after the journey; and (iii) over the longer term. Our assessment of the evidence shows that mood is lower during the commute than other daily activities and stress can be induced by congestion, crowding and unpredictability. People who walk or cycle to work are generally more satisfied with their commute than those who travel by car and especially those who use public transport. Satisfaction decreases with duration of commute, regardless of mode used, and increases when travelling with company. After the journey, evidence shows that the commute experience “spills over” into how people feel and perform at work and home. However, a consistent link between commuting and life satisfaction overall has not been established. The evidence suggests that commuters are generally successful in trading off the drawbacks of longer and more arduous commute journeys against the benefits they bring in relation to overall life satisfaction, but further research is required to understand the decision making involved. The evidence review points to six areas that warrant policy action and research: (i) enhancing the commute experience; (ii) increasing commute satisfaction; (iii) reducing the impacts of long duration commutes; (iv) meeting commuter preferences; (v) recognising flexibility and constraints in commuting routines and (vi) accounting for SWB impacts of commuting in policy making and appraisal.

1. Introduction

The overall goal of public policies is to improve the welfare of the population. In transport, welfare has traditionally been assessed by considering objective impacts of the transport system such as travel times and costs, crashes, and environmental degradation. However, the subjective experience of transport, including how it contributes to overall happiness, is of growing interest (Mokhtarian, Citation2019). This has coincided with concern about the limits of GDP as a measure of economic performance and social progress and global interest in measuring and improving people’s wellbeing (OECD, Citation2011).

Wellbeing is a multidimensional concept that may be measured both objectively and subjectively. This review focuses on what we know about the impact of commuting on subjective wellbeing (SWB). SWB is aimed at capturing wellbeing as perceived by individuals based on the view that “people are the best judges of how their life is going” (OECD, Citation2011, p. 265). This does not dismiss the importance of objective dimensions of wellbeing (such as income and health). SWB is defined formally by the OECD as “Good mental states, including all of the various evaluations, positive and negative, that people make of their lives, and the affective reactions of people to their experiences” (OECD, Citation2013, p. 10). SWB can be measured (Tinkler & Hicks, Citation2011) in terms of evaluative wellbeing (how satisfied individuals are with different domains of their life and with life overall) and experiential wellbeing (how often individuals experience positive and negative emotions). Both of these are a type of hedonic wellbeing and relate to the presence of pleasure and absence of pain. SWB can also be measured in terms of eudaimonic wellbeing which relates to the achievement of a higher purpose or meaning in life.

Various papers have recently reviewed theoretical relationships between transport, personal travel, and wellbeing, and assessed what is known from the literature about the relationships (De Vos, Schwanen, Van Acker, & Witlox, Citation2013; Delbosc, Citation2012; Ettema, Gärling, Olsson, & Friman, Citation2010; Mokhtarian, Citation2019; Nordbakke & Schwanen, Citation2014; Reardon & Abdallah, Citation2013). These papers look at the role of travel in general and do not specifically focus on commuting, even though research on the subjective experience of commuting has a long tradition with studies on commuting stress dating back to the 1970s (e.g. Novaco, Stokols, Campbell, & Stokols, Citation1979).

A review of research on commuting and SWB is justified in its own right. The relationship between commuting and SWB is likely to be quite different from that of other travel (e.g. leisure travel) for many reasons. It is a regular, unavoidable activity which absorbs substantial personal time and resources and may be a dominant feature in people’s lives for many years. National Travel Survey data for England indicates that the average one-way commute duration is 31 min (DfT, Citation2018a) and one in seven commuters (14%) have commutes of 60 min or more (DfT, Citation2018b). Workers in England therefore spend an average of one hour per day commuting and one in seven spend at least two hours commuting. A comparison of one-way commuting times across Europe in 2015 suggests even longer commutes with a 53 min average for the UK and 42 min average across the European Union 28 countries (Eurostat, Citation2019).Footnote1 American Community Survey data for 2012–2016 indicates the average one-way commute is 26 min in the United States (USCB, Citation2017).

Studies of commuting and SWB have been conducted across various disciplines (e.g. economics, psychology, health, transport) and are highly heterogeneous in terms of the aspects of SWB considered, the characteristics of commuting considered, and the methodological approaches used. This presents a challenge in comparing and contrasting studies, critically evaluating findings and drawing conclusions. The authors of this paper took on the challenge following a symposium on “Commuting and Wellbeing” which was held in Bristol in June 2017 and brought together international researchers working in this field. We concluded that it would be valuable to the research community and to policy makers to review the diverse body of research, synthesise findings and identify implications for policy and future research.

We did not attempt to carry out a systematic review, as our goal was to understand the development of the field over time rather than summarise results from the set of studies that meet specific criteria. The review thus takes the form of a critical review (Grant & Booth, Citation2009) which aims at extensively researching the literature, identifying significant items and critically evaluating their contributions. It is written as a narrative, moving from one conceptual area to another, reflecting on the overall development of theoretical and empirical understanding.

The paper starts (in section 2) with a conceptual model of the relationship between commuting and SWB, identifying different areas that have been investigated in the literature. It then builds up a picture (in section 3) of what has been learnt to date, based on the expert knowledge of the authors in each area. It finishes with recommendations for policy and future research (in section 4).

2. Conceptualisation of the relationship between commuting and SWB

Our conceptualisation of the relationship between commuting and SWB is shown in . It is partly based on the conceptual model of the relationship between travel and wellbeing of Ettema et al. (Citation2010), which highlights that travel may affect different aspects of SWB, and of De Vos et al. (Citation2013), which differentiates impacts of travel in the short-term and long-term. However, our model applies specifically to commuting.

Figure 1. Conceptual model for the relationship between commuting and SWB.

Figure 1. Conceptual model for the relationship between commuting and SWB.

People’s commute journeys are influenced by their life situation (an objective factor as indicated by the rectangular outline) and personal traits (a subjective factor as indicated by the oval outline). The commute journey has potential objective impacts and subjective impacts on the commuter during their journey,Footnote2 after their journey and in the long-term. People’s wellbeing is also influenced by the broader interaction between their life situation and personal traits as shown by the arrows from these to the wellbeing box. Our review is organised with respect to this conceptualisation.

Commuting has objective effects on those that undertake it, for example, in terms of expenditure of time, money and physical effort and possible injuries sustained or exposure to pollutants. Our focus, however, is on how the commute journey is experienced subjectively.

The review starts with studies that have looked at affective experiences of commuting during travel (section 3.1). It then considers studies which have assessed satisfaction with the commute (section 3.2). Section 3.3 explores whether there are spill-over effects from the commute to other life domains. It is well established that physical health is a primary factor influencing overall SWB (e.g. Dolan, Peasgood, & White, Citation2008), hence in section 3.4 we summarise evidence for commuting impacts on physical health.

In section 3.5 we identify what has been learnt from studies that have focused directly on the relationship between commuting and overall SWB. These provide core evidence on whether there is any effect of commuting on people’s overall lives. In some cases, studies have sought to explain the mechanisms underlying observed relationships between commuting and overall SWB. Their findings are reported in section 3.6. The final area of consideration in section 3.7 is feedback effects between SWB and decisions that workers make relating to commuting, for example, whereby unhappy commuters alter how they make their journey to work or the journey itself (e.g. through a workplace or residential location change).

3. Evidence review

3.1. Affective experiences of commuting

Affective experiences of commuting refer to “feelings evoked by travelling, such as stress, excitement, pleasure, boredom and control” (Anable & Gatersleben, Citation2005, p. 164). According to Russell’s circumplex model of affect (Russell, Citation1980), an environment is automatically experienced in terms of two dimensions: valence (degree of pleasantness) and arousal (degree of intensity). For instance, “stress” is a combination of intense arousal and unpleasantness. Most research into affective experiences of commuting has focused on stress.

3.1.1. Commute stress

As early as the 1970s, researchers have documented the personal stress associated with commuting to work. The factors influencing commute stress are only partly clear. Early studies focused on impedance, defined as the difficulty commuters experience in moving from home to work and back (Schaeffer, Street, Singer, & Baum, Citation1988). Researchers initially measured impedance as travel distance or time (e.g. Novaco et al., Citation1979) but soon focused on travel speed to capture the effect of congestion (e.g. Schaeffer et al., Citation1988). Novaco, Stokols, and Milanesi (Citation1990) differentiated between physical impedance (e.g. speed) and subjective impedance measured as drivers’ perceptions about inability to avoid traffic, speed reductions due to traffic jams, exposure to traffic control devices and other characteristics of the commute.

It has been theorised that impedance contributes to stress through the mechanism of perceived control: higher impedance causes commuters to feel less control and thus more stress. One study found control to be “the most powerful predictor of commuting stress” (Sposato, Röderer, & Cervinka, Citation2012, p. 581). Control can be operationalised in a variety of ways. Schaeffer et al. (Citation1988), in comparing stress for commuters driving alone versus carpooling, differentiated between control over the internal environment of the car (e.g. controlling the radio) and control over the route taken to work. Lucas and Heady (Citation2002) showed for workers in Atlanta, Georgia, that flexi-time work schedules, which give commuters control over when they commute, are associated with reduced commute stress. The concept of choice is closely related to the concept of control (Kluger, Citation1998; White & Rotton, Citation1998), but some argue that choice has an ambiguous effect on stress, particularly if the choices available are not seen as favourable ones (Koslowsky, Kluger, & Reich, Citation2013). Others have operationalised control as the predictability or, conversely, the variability of the commute. While Novaco and Gonzalez (Citation2009) argued that variability is a moderator of the effect of impedance (i.e. variability magnifies the effect of impedance) on stress, Kluger (Citation1998, pp. 160–161) concluded that “commute variability may be the main commuting stressor”.

Most studies of commute stress have focused on car users, but studies of public transport users show some similarities and some differences. An early study showed that stress increased as crowding on trains in Stockholm increased (Lundberg, Citation1976). Another study of commuters to New York City showed that predictability is associated with reduced stress for rail commuters, as it is for driving, possibly because predictability offers a form of cognitive control in situations where commuters do not have behavioural control (Evans, Wener, & Phillips, Citation2002). A third study showed that improvements in service quality reduced stress for rail commuters to New York City by reducing travel times and increasing predictability (Wener, Evans, & Boately, Citation2005).

Recent studies have compared commute stress between users of different modes and found the lowest stress among those that walk or cycle to work and highest stress amongst those that drive (Gatersleben & Uzzell, Citation2007, for university employees at the University of Surrey, UK; Legrain, Eluru, & El-Geneidy, Citation2015, for university employees at McGill University, Canada). For pedestrians, it has been found that feelings of comfort and safety from traffic are associated with reduced commute stress (Legrain et al., Citation2015). It has also been found from a study in Rotterdam that the emotional state of active commuters is more sensitive to weather (temperatures, clouds, precipitation and wind) than other commuters (Böcker, Dijst, & Faber, Citation2016). The concept of bicycling level of traffic stress, measured as a function of road characteristics and traffic levels (Furth, Mekuria, & Nixon, Citation2016), is popular among transport planners but has not yet been validated to be an indicator of cycle commuting stress.

Commute stress also depends on personal characteristics. Women have been found to experience greater commute stress than men and some studies show that women are more sensitive to stress factors (Wener et al., Citation2005). The reasons for this gender effect have not been explored in depth, but it may be related to greater time urgency for women who often have more pressing responsibilities outside of work (e.g. childcare, housework) (Novaco, Kliewer, & Broquet, Citation1991).

The evidence to date on factors contributing to commute stress is convincing, although stronger evidence of causal relationships is needed. Most studies use cross-sectional, observational designs, although one study used an experimental design where college students were randomly allocated to use different commute modes and stress levels compared (White & Rotton, Citation1998). The studies are mostly from the United States and Europe, and their applicability to other parts of the world are uncertain.

3.1.2. Other affective responses to the commute experience

Whilst stress has been the main focus, some cross-sectional studies in the United States and Europe have explored other affective responses to the commute. One study of university employees at the University of Surrey, UK, obtained appraisals by commuters of the extent to which their journeys were stressful, exciting, boring, relaxing, pleasant and depressing (Gatersleben & Uzzell, Citation2007). Car users found their commutes to be relatively unpleasant and arousing, public transport users unpleasant and not arousing, cyclists pleasant and arousing and walkers pleasant and not arousing. Delays were the most important factors contributing to unpleasant experiences of car and public transport users. For walkers and cyclists it was traffic danger and quality of route provision.

Studies comparing commuting with other daily activities have found commuting to be the activity rated with the least positive affect scores and with one of the most negative affect scores (Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, Citation2004, for female, American workers; Mokhtarian, Papon, Goulard, & Diana, Citation2015, for French workers; Lancée, Veenhoven, & Burger, Citation2017, for Dutch workers). Analysis of data from the American Time Use Survey has shown that total affect (aggregated combination of positive and negative affect scores) is lower during work-related travel than other activities with this pattern more pronounced when commuting by bus and less pronounced when driving and cycling or interacting with another person while travelling (Morris & Guerra, Citation2015a). Increased commute duration is associated with lower total affect, particularly due to higher stress (Morris & Guerra, Citation2015b).

Some studies have focused on the factors that enable travellers to experience their journeys positively. They have used the concept of “liking” both for travel in general and for modes specifically (Ory & Mokhtarian, Citation2005). It has been shown for commuters in San Francisco that finding the commute less stressful is associated with greater liking of the commute (Ory et al., Citation2004). Greater understandings of positive affective experiences of commuting, their causal factors, and their relationships with commute stress are needed.

3.2. Satisfaction with the commute

Whereas affect during commuting concerns travellers’ emotional state, commute satisfaction has a broader definition. In particular, it is assumed that travel satisfaction can be regarded as a sub-domain of overall SWB (as per family life, working life, etc.), and commuting is a sub-domain in its own right given that it consumes a large amount of time for many workers (Ettema et al., Citation2010). Ettema et al. (Citation2011) proposed a measurement scale for travel satisfaction (which has frequently been applied in studies of commuting travel), which includes both cognitive and affective components. However, other authors measure travel satisfaction using only a cognitive measurement scale (e.g. Susilo & Cats, Citation2014), or as a composite of evaluations of specific aspects of the trip (e.g. St-Louis, Manaugh, van Lierop, & El-Geneidy, Citation2014). Some studies have measured satisfaction with a particular commute trip (e.g. today’s or yesterday’s commute: Mao, Ettema, & Dijst, Citation2016) and others with a typical commute trip (e.g. Olsson, Gärling, Ettema, Friman, & Fujii, Citation2013).

Given that commute satisfaction reflects commuters’ evaluations beyond their affective experiences implies that a broader set of factors influences commute satisfaction. First and foremost, travel mode has been found to be strongly associated with commute satisfaction. Commuters using active travel modes report the highest levels of commute satisfaction, whereas public transport users report the lowest levels (e.g. St-Louis et al., Citation2014, for university employees at McGill University, Canada; Friman, Gärling, Ettema, & Olsson, Citation2017, for urban commuters in Sweden; Ye & Titheridge, Citation2017, for workers in Xi’an, China). As most studies of commute satisfaction control for trip characteristics such as trip duration, this suggests that different travel modes have specific properties that make them more or less satisfying. For active travel, it is reported that the physical activity involved is associated with a more positive mood, which translates into higher satisfaction (Ekkekakis, Backhouse, Gray, & Lind, Citation2008). For car use, it is argued that aspects such as independence, mastery, joy and prestige play a role in the relatively high satisfaction with car use (Bergstad et al., Citation2011). For public transport commuting, two North American studies have found that rail commuters are more satisfied with their commute than bus commuters (Handy & Thigpen, Citation2019; St-Louis et al., Citation2014), but a study in Sweden found higher satisfaction among bus commuters than rail commuters (Ettema, Friman, Gärling, Olsson, & Fujii, Citation2012).

Longer commute durations are associated with reduced satisfaction with commutes made by all modes (Ettema et al., Citation2012; Ettema, Gärling, Olsson, Friman, & Moerdijk, Citation2013; Manaugh & El-Geneidy, Citation2013; Mao et al., Citation2016; St-Louis et al., Citation2014). For car commuters in the Netherlands, it has been found that congestion and perceived lack of safety are associated negatively with commute satisfaction (Ettema et al., Citation2013). For public transport users in Sweden, Ettema et al. (Citation2012) report that travelling in the peak, use of ICTs and engaging in relaxation and entertainment activities are negatively associated with commute satisfaction. Having company, however, is associated positively with commute satisfaction. The authors suggest that the use of ICTs and engaging in relaxation and entertainment activities can be interpreted as coping mechanisms for responding to negative de-activation during the commute. This highlights the caution required when interpreting results from cross-sectional studies.

Some studies have investigated the role of built environment characteristics. Mostly, the residential location is used as a proxy of the environment in which commuting takes place, although in fact most of the commuting will take place beyond this area. As a result, outcomes are often inconclusive. Ettema et al. (Citation2012) did not find significant differences between Swedish cities of different size. Mao et al. (Citation2016) found that in denser areas of Beijing, using the subway or bicycle are associated with higher commute satisfaction. Ye and Titheridge (Citation2017) did not find significant associations between access to public transport, green areas or car-oriented design and commute satisfaction in the Chinese city of Xi’an. Other studies have investigated whether perceptions of the travel route and the surrounding landscape influence travel satisfaction. Böcker et al. (Citation2016) found that the percentage of green space on the route is positively associated with commute satisfaction for cycle commuters in Rotterdam.

Mao et al. (Citation2016) found that commuters in Beijing with more flexibility in their mode choice had higher commute satisfaction, presumably because they can choose their preferred travel mode. Surprisingly, this study also found that those without any flexibility in mode choice had a relatively high commute satisfaction, which was attributed to less experience with competing modes and processes of rationalisation. Handy and Thigpen (Citation2019) also found higher satisfaction among those with mode constraints in the context of Davis, California.

Some studies have shown that commute satisfaction is linked to travel-related attitudes. According to De Vos, Mokhtarian, Schwanen, Van Acker, and Witlox (Citation2016) and St-Louis et al. (Citation2014), a positive stance towards a certain travel mode has positive implications for travel satisfaction when using that mode. For instance, a liking of one’s usual commute mode is associated with higher commute satisfaction (Handy & Thigpen, Citation2019). Ye and Titheridge (Citation2019) found lower income commuters in Xi’an had lower levels of commuting satisfaction and this is related to a mismatch between commuting mode choice and travel attitudes. Besides travel-related attitudes, other types of attitudes might also impact travel satisfaction. Manaugh and El-Geneidy (Citation2013) suggest that satisfaction with walking trips is more likely for people who value exercise and who are environmentally aware. It has also been shown that people with a positive stance towards travel in general are more satisfied with trips compared to people who dislike travel (De Vos & Witlox, Citation2016; Ye & Titheridge, Citation2017). On the other hand, it is also plausible that travel satisfaction affects travel-related attitudes; a satisfying trip with a certain travel mode might result in a more positive stance towards the used mode. In a conceptual paper, De Vos (Citation2019) argues that travel satisfaction might influence attitudes (and also travel mode choice) more than vice versa. However, the effect of commute satisfaction on attitudes has not yet been empirically analysed.

Nearly all the results reported above have been based on cross-sectional studies and hence there can be doubts about direction of causality. A longitudinal evaluation of an e-cycling stimulation programme in the province of North-Brabant, the Netherlands, found that the commute satisfaction of car commuters increases over time after taking up e-cycling to work (De Kruijf, Ettema, & Dijst, Citation2019). Residential or job moves resulting in shorter commute distances and more active travel have been found to result in higher levels of commute satisfaction (De Vos, Ettema, & Witlox, Citation2019, for movers to Ghent, Belgium, and Schneider & Willman, Citation2019, for employees at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee). This shows that changes to commuting can influence commute satisfaction.

The role of various contextual factors that potentially could impact on commute satisfaction and be subject to influence by policy interventions remains unexplored. For car commuting, this includes road design and, parking availability. For public transport factors such as seat availability, vehicle functionalities (e.g. Wi-Fi) and design of stations merit investigation. For active travel modes, more insight is needed on the impact of landscape, road design, surface quality and workplace facilities. This research should extend to emerging new technological and organisational innovations in transportation such as electric cars, automated vehicles, bike sharing, car sharing and ride hailing platforms.

It should be noted that travel can also have a direct impact on eudaimonic wellbeing – since feelings of security, confidence and autonomy can be affected by how people (perceive) travel. Singleton (Citation2019) refers to these eudaimonic aspects of travel as travel eudaimonia. We are not aware of empirical research that has considered eudaimonic aspects of commuting.

3.3. Commuting spill-over effects to other life domains

This section first considers objective effects of commuting on time spent on other activities and then subjective effects on mood subsequent to the commute and satisfaction with other life domains.

There is evidence from different countries on how time spent commuting affects time allocation to other activities. A study using the American Time Use Survey found longer commute durations are associated with less time spent with spouse, children and friends for men and less time spent with friends for women (Christian, Citation2012). An analysis of data for car commuters from Statistics Canada’s General Social Survey showed long commute durations associated with reduced time spent in physically active leisure and social leisure, but not work (Hilbrecht, Smale, & Mock, Citation2014). In contrast, results obtained from the China Family Panel Studies showed that longer duration commutes are associated with reduced time working and sleeping, but no difference in time spent caring for family, in physical activity and social activity (Nie & Sousa-Poza, Citation2018). A study of how commuting affects social capital in southern Sweden found commuting by car associated with reduced social participation and less trust compared with active commuting, and the strength of these associations increased with duration of the commute (Mattisson, Håkansson, & Jakobsson, Citation2015).

Another potential effect of commuting is a spill-over of mood to subsequent activities. An analysis of American Time Use Survey data found that longer commute durations are associated with lower positive affect at work, but no difference in sense of meaning during work (an indicator of eudaimonic wellbeing) (Morris & Zhou, Citation2018). A study in Sweden asked commuters to report on their smartphones their mood before and directly after their commute and later at the workplace (Friman, Olsson, Ståhl, Ettema, & Gärling, Citation2017). Analysis of the data showed that longer duration commutes are associated with worsened mood later in the workplace, although not immediately after the commute. Two studies of commuters in Montreal (Canada) have compared mood at work of users of different modes. A survey of McGill University staff and students found that cyclists are more likely to be energised when they arrive at work than users of other modes (Loong, van Lierop, & El-Geneidy, Citation2017). A comparison of IT workers arriving to work by car, public transport and cycling, however, found no difference in mood between the different mode users in the initial period of working (Brutus, Javadian, & Panaccio, Citation2017).

Looking at impacts of the commute on people’s daily lives more broadly, analysis of data from public health surveys in southern Sweden showed that car and public transport commutes exceeding 30 min are associated with increased everyday stress, lower vitality and perceived poor sleep quality (Hansson, Mattisson, Björk, Östergren, & Jakobsson, Citation2011). There is one example of a longitudinal study which has evaluated the impact of an intervention and found that commuters who switched to an improved train service in New York experienced reduced commute stress and also reduced job strain but no change in stress at home (Wener et al., Citation2005).

Some studies have considered the relationship between commuting and satisfaction with different life domains. For example, two studies have shown that longer commute durations are associated with decreased satisfaction with social contacts (Delmelle, Haslauer, & Prinz, Citation2013, for workers in Vienna; Kroesen, Citation2014, for workers in Netherlands), while a study of British workers found longer commute durations are associated with lower leisure time satisfaction for men but not for women (Wheatley, Citation2014). When workers are satisfied with their commutes it has been found they have greater satisfaction with their jobs (Abou-Zeid & Ben-Akiva, Citation2011, for an international sample of commuters) and with work-family balance (Denstedli, Julsrud, & Christiansen, Citation2017, for knowledge workers in Oslo).

In summary, the evidence suggests that longer commutes are associated with reduced time spent in social and leisure activities (at least in a North American and European context) and this is felt by commuters in terms of satisfaction with social/leisure participation and work-family balance. Studies also indicate spill-over effects from the commute to mood at work and job satisfaction. As previously, the evidence is almost entirely based on cross-sectional survey data and caution is required in inferring causal relationships.

3.4. Commuting and physical health

Physical health is a primary factor influencing overall SWB (e.g. Dolan et al., Citation2008), hence the impact of commuting on physical health is an important potential pathway for commuting affecting overall SWB. First, higher levels of physical activity have been observed amongst commuters who walk (Audrey, Procter, & Cooper, Citation2014), cycle (Donaire-Gonzalez, de Nazelle, & Cole-Hunter, Citation2015) or use public transport (MacDonald, Stokes, & Cohen, Citation2010), when compared to drivers (Wanner, Götschi, Martin-Diener, Kahlmeier, & Martin, Citation2012). It is important to understand if increased (or decreased) physical activity from mode changes are offset by corresponding decreased (or increased) physical activity in other activity domains. A longitudinal study exploring this in English towns identified a modest, positive relationship between change in the amount of active travel for work and change in overall physical activity (Sahlqvist, Goodman, Cooper, & Ogilvie, Citation2013).

Other determinants of health, alongside those arising from physical activity, should be considered to fully understand the impact of commuting on physical health. Whilst commuting duration, unsurprisingly, is positively associated with inhalation of air pollutants amongst active commuters, one analysis has estimated that the physical activity benefits, compared to staying at home, exceeded the potential harm from air pollution for people who cycle up to 3.5 h daily (Tainio et al., Citation2016). Longer commuting durations have been shown to be related to fatigue symptoms (Kageyama, Nishikido, Kobayashi, Kurokawa, & Kaneko, Citation1998, for male workers in Tokyo) and poor sleep (Walsleben et al., Citation1999, for rail commuters in New York), which can induce cardiovascular abnormalities and dysfunction related to the onset of heart disease.

Turning to objective indicators of physical health itself, various cross-sectional (e.g. Flint & Cummins, Citation2016) and longitudinal (e.g. Martin, Panter, Suhrcke, & Ogilvie, Citation2015) studies have found public transport users and active commuters reporting lower Body Mass Index (BMI) and/or body fat (indicators of overweight and obesity) than car commuters. However, other studies have shown mixed results on the relationship between commute distance or duration and BMI (Hoehner, Barlow, Allen, & Schootman, Citation2012; Kroesen, Citation2014; Künn-Nelen, Citation2015). Further studies have also examined waist circumference, systolic and diastolic blood pressure and musculoskeletal disorders (Koslowsky et al., Citation2013). Typically effect sizes observed in these studies are of small (clinical) significance and the longer-term impacts are under-researched.

Results on the relationship between commuting and self-reported measures of physical health have not produced clear results. Lower frequency of sickness absence has been observed amongst employees in the Netherlands who cycled to work, particularly those cycling longer distances (Hendriksen, Simons, Garre, & Hildebrandt, Citation2010). A study of commuting in Cambridge (UK) found more time spent in active commuting associated with better physical wellbeing (Humphreys, Goodman, & Ogilvie, Citation2013), but changes over time in active commuting not associated with changes in physical wellbeing (Mytton, Panter, & Ogilvie, Citation2016). Two studies have examined the relationship between commute duration and perceived health based on repeated observations from panel data. Künn-Nelen (Citation2015) found from 1991–2008 data for British workers a small negative association between commute duration and self-reported health and more substantial negative association with health satisfaction, both of which are more pronounced for car commuters and for women. Clark, Chatterjee, Martin, and Davis (Citation2019) found no association between commute duration and self-reported health for workers in England from data for 2009/2010–2013/2014. They found no longitudinal association between commute mode and self-reported health but did find a positive cross-sectional association between cycling to work and self-reported health and a negative cross-sectional association for bus commuting. This can be interpreted as showing that people with better health cycle to work and people with worse health use the bus – it does not suggest a causal effect of commute mode on health.

In summary, considerable heterogeneity exists across studies in this area, particularly in study designs and measures of physical health. Evidence suggests that active commuting may decrease overweight/obesity to a small degree, but not that it makes a substantial difference to perceived health. Nevertheless, in section 3.6.1, we assess whether there is any evidence that physical health effects of commuting have an impact on overall SWB.

3.5. Relationship between commuting and overall SWB

This section and the next one (section 3.6) present findings from studies that have directly examined the relationship between commuting and overall SWB. Some studies have used cross-sectional data while others have used panel data. provides summary details of the studies reported in sections 3.5 and 3.6 to allow the reader to compare characteristics of the studies.

Table 1. Studies of the relationship between commuting and overall SWB.

3.5.1. Cross-sectional studies

Cross-sectional studies have consistently found a negative relationship between commute duration and SWB, including studies in Sweden (Hansson et al., Citation2011), United States (Choi, Coughlin, & D’Ambrosio, Citation2013), Canada (Hilbrecht et al., Citation2014), Great Britain (ONS, Citation2014), and China (Nie & Sousa-Poza, Citation2018). One of these studies reported that the largest negative association occurs for one-way commutes between 61 and 90 min (ONS, Citation2014). Possible explanations for this are that those with commutes exceeding 90 min travel to work less frequently than other workers or may have more comfortable travelling conditions. No association between use of different commute modes and SWB was found for Chinese commuters (Nie & Sousa-Poza, Citation2018). Walking to work and commuting by bus have been found to be associated with lower SWB compared to driving for British workers (ONS, Citation2014).

Studies in specific locations can help to understand how context affects the relationship between commuting and SWB. Walking to work is associated with higher life satisfaction in London (after controlling for commute distance), indicating that walking may be beneficial in large cities (Chng, White, Abraham, & Skippon, Citation2016). However, walking was not found to be associated with better mental health. Humphreys et al. (Citation2013) also found no association between the amount of time spent actively commuting and mental wellbeing for commuters in Cambridge (UK). Cycling to work in Sydney is associated with higher self-rated quality of life (which can be regarded as an indicator of SWB) compared to other forms of commuting (Crane, Rissel, Greaves, & Gebel, Citation2016). This was argued to be due to the moderately intense physical activity involved in cycling.

3.5.2. Panel studies

With concern that associations between commuting and SWB based on cross-sectional data may be spurious, researchers have used panel data to control for potential confounders. They have analysed the data using fixed-effects regression modelling which identifies the within-individual relationship between commuting behaviour and SWB based on multiple observations per individual.

The first example of such a study used eight waves of data from the German Socio-Economic Panel (GSOEP) to find that longer duration commutes are associated with lower life satisfaction (Stutzer & Frey, Citation2008). An 18 min increase in commute duration is associated with a lower life satisfaction equivalent of one-eighth of the effect of being unemployed. The relationship holds for different commute modes. The possibility that partners of long duration commuters receive a compensatory benefit was tested and rejected. The authors referred to their finding as the “commuting paradox”, since economic theory would suggest that people with longer commutes will not have lower than average SWB, since they would be compensated by better jobs or housing. Their explanation was that people incorrectly estimate the effects of commuting and their ability to adapt to it.

In contrast, longer duration commutes are not found to be associated with lower life satisfaction based on a fixed-effects regression analysis of British Household Panel Survey (BHPS) data (Dickerson, Hole, & Munford, Citation2014). A previous analysis of the same BHPS data set found that longer duration commutes are associated with worse mental health for women but not men (Roberts, Hodgson, & Dolan, Citation2011). This was interpreted as showing that women’s greater household responsibilities meant longer commute durations were unfavourable to them.

Another study used BHPS data and fixed-effects regression modelling with the primary aim of assessing whether there are differences in SWB associated with commute mode (Martin, Goryakin, & Suhrcke, Citation2014). It found walking and bus commuting are associated with better mental health than commuting by car and the longer the duration of the walk to work the larger the improvement in mental health (with the opposite effect for car commute duration). The relationship between active commuting and mental wellbeing has been assessed for commuters in Cambridge based on repeated surveys in 2009 and 2012. Those who maintained cycling to work reported improved mental wellbeing scores over time compared to those who did not cycle to work, although there was no significant association with mental wellbeing for those who started cycling to work or for those maintaining or starting walking to work (Mytton et al., Citation2016).

Three recent studies have explored how the relationship between commuting and SWB differs depending on the measure of SWB considered. With fixed-effects regression modelling on six waves of Understanding Society data for workers in England it was found that longer commute durations are not associated with decreased life satisfaction, although they are associated with increased strain, worse mental health, reduced job satisfaction and reduced leisure time satisfaction (Clark et al., Citation2019). Only limited differences in SWB were found for using different commute modes. Walking to work is associated with decreased strain in people’s lives and increased leisure time satisfaction. Lorenz (Citation2018) analysed the relationship between commuting distance (rather than duration) and various measures of SWB based on GSOEP data. She found no association between commute distance and overall SWB, whether experiential or evaluative. Ingenfeld, Wolbring, and Bless (Citation2018) analysed GSOEP data over a longer period and found a negative association between commute distance and life satisfaction when commute distance is specified as a continuous variable but that the negative association is only strongly significant for commute distances over 80 kms when commute distance is specified as a categorical variable. This suggests greater attention should be given to non-linear effects in future research.

The lack of a negative within-individual association between commute duration and life satisfaction was interpreted by Clark et al. (Citation2019) as arising because workers are acting rationally and only take on longer commutes if there are compensating benefits (income and satisfactory housing/employment) which contribute to life goals. With their data, they noted that those workers who moved from short commutes (up to 15 min one-way) to long commutes (over 45 min one-way) increased their income more than those who continued to have a short commute (Chatterjee, Clark, Martin, & Davis, Citation2017). They also found that those workers who persisted with long commutes had consistently lower life satisfaction than other workers. This could be due to unobserved factors unrelated to the commute (e.g. being more pessimistic), or quite plausibly due to these workers accepting the situation and being unwilling or unable to change it. This would support the commuting paradox hypothesis. Further investigation is warranted about why long duration commuters persist with their commutes.

3.6. Mechanisms underlying relationship between commuting and overall SWB

The results reported in section 3.5 identify relationships between commuting and overall SWB without revealing explanations why they might arise. In this section we assess evidence on mechanisms responsible for these relationships.

Two studies have considered how time spent commuting affects time spent on other activities and hence overall SWB. The first for Canadian car commuters found that a negative association between commute duration and life satisfaction was mediated by reduced time spent in physically active leisure and greater experience of traffic congestion but not time spent for social leisure (Hilbrecht et al., Citation2014). The second for Chinese workers found no evidence for mediation via a reduction in time for caring for family, work, physical activity and social activity but evidence to support part mediation via a reduction in time spent sleeping (Nie & Sousa-Poza, Citation2018).

The impact of commuting on overall SWB through its effect on satisfaction with different life domains has been another focus of investigation. Satisfaction with the commute has been shown to be positively associated with affect balance (experiential wellbeing) and life satisfaction (evaluative wellbeing) for Swedish commuters (Olsson et al., Citation2013). Given that satisfaction with the commute is positively associated with walking or cycling to work and negatively associated with commute duration this implies active commuting and shorter duration commutes are beneficial not only to domain-specific commute satisfaction but to overall SWB. These results are based on cross-sectional data and we cannot be certain about the direction of causality. It is conceivable that happier and healthier people are more likely to report greater satisfaction with daily activities and to take up active commuting. The relationship could also be bi-directional where happier people take up active commuting and this further increases their happiness. As noted in section 3.2, however, the recent longitudinal study by De Kruijf et al. (Citation2019) found that the commute satisfaction of car commuters increases over time after taking up e-cycling and this represents some evidence of the main direction of causality theorised in .

A path analysis based on cross-sectional data for car and bicycle commuters in the Netherlands found that increased commute duration is associated with decreased satisfaction with social contacts which in turn has a negative association with happiness (Kroesen, Citation2014). However there was no association between commute duration and other tested mediators, namely: BMI, perceived health and job satisfaction. A path analysis for one wave of Understanding Society data for workers in England revealed that longer commute journeys are associated with decreased leisure time satisfaction, decreased job satisfaction and increased strain (Chatterjee et al., Citation2017). These factors in turn are associated with reduced life satisfaction. Leisure time satisfaction is the most dominant of the factors and accounts for 80% of the negative association between commute duration and life satisfaction. In neither of these studies is there an indication that longer commuter durations impact on overall SWB via worse physical health.

The role of stress was investigated by Ruger, Pfaff, Weishaar, and Wiernik (Citation2017) who found that perceived stress has a mediating role in the negative association between commute duration and health-related quality of life based on a survey of expatriate workers of the German Foreign Office. The mediating role is particularly prominent among parents, suggesting reduced time availability from long commutes is particularly felt by this group.

The results reported above suggest longer commute durations influence overall SWB negatively via decreased satisfaction with social participation/leisure time, loss of sleep and increased stress. They suggest active commuting influences overall SWB positively via greater satisfaction with commuting. It is recommended that further investigations of mediating relationships are carried out based on longitudinal data to provide stronger evidence on causality.

3.7. Feedback effects between SWB and decisions that workers make relating to commuting

The possibility that commuters respond to SWB effects of commuting by altering how they make their journey to work or the journey itself (e.g. through a workplace or residential location change) needs to be considered. Recent studies suggest that people choose to live in a neighbourhood which enables them to have satisfying trips (Cao & Ettema, Citation2014, for residents of Minneapolis-Saint Paul; De Vos & Witlox, Citation2016, for residents of Ghent, Belgium). This can be achieved by living in neighbourhoods facilitating use of a preferred travel mode, but also by living in a neighbourhood that permits a preferred commute length (De Vos & Witlox, Citation2017). Analysis of panel data for workers in England shows that those with commutes over 45 min one-way (who tend to have lower life satisfaction than other workers) have an increased likelihood of changing jobs by the following year of around 25% (Chatterjee et al., Citation2017). People might also change their travel choices (e.g. mode choice, departure time) in response to dissatisfying commute trips. However, due to the mostly fixed work and house locations and working hours, changing travel choices (e.g. not travelling by car or travelling outside peak hours) might often not be feasible. In general, more research is needed on how commuters respond to dissatisfying commutes.

4. Policy and research recommendations

4.1. Summary of the evidence

The body of evidence reviewed in section 3 suggests commuting has an impact on multiple dimensions of SWB, both during and after the journey to work. During the journey, stress can be induced by a lack of control, associated with congestion, crowding and unpredictability, and mood is found to be generally lower than during other daily activities. People who walk or cycle to work are generally more satisfied with their commute than those who travel by car and especially those who use public transport. Satisfaction decreases with duration of commute, regardless of mode used, and increases when travelling with company. After the journey, there is evidence that the commute experience “spills over” into how people feel and perform at work and home. However, a consistent link between commuting and life satisfaction overall has not been established. The evidence suggests that commuters are generally successful in trading off the drawbacks of longer and more arduous commute journeys against the benefits they bring in relation to overall life satisfaction, but further research is required to understand the decision making involved.

4.2. Policy context for acting upon the evidence

There is a fertile policy environment for utilising the growing evidence on the links between commuting and SWB given the high prominence of the wellbeing agenda in many countries at present. One important dimension of the wellbeing agenda is measurement. For example, the OECD’s Better Life Index measures wellbeing in each OECD country across 11 domains – housing; income; jobs; community; education; environment; civic engagement; health; life satisfaction; safety; and work-life balance. At the national level, wellbeing measurement programmes, such as those in the UK, Canada and Australia, have dashboards of wellbeing indicators with domains similar to that of the OECD (Kroll, Citation2011). However, while transport is linked in various ways to many of these domains of wellbeing (Delbosc, Citation2012; Reardon & Abdallah, Citation2013), including through the commute, transport is largely absent from these indicator sets. For example, in the UK Measuring National Wellbeing Programme, the only transport specific indicator relates to accessibility to services; measured as the average minimum travel time by public transport or walking to eight main services (including hospitals and schools).

One of the ways in which the wellbeing agenda differs from others before it, such as the sustainability agenda, is its emphasis on subjective indicators. Advocates for the use of SWB in policy argue that the practice of asking people directly about their wellbeing, rather than relying on objective proxies, democratises the basis upon which policy is made. A key concern, however, with using subjective indicators is the possibility of the “happy poor” where those who are in disadvantaged positions adapt to their circumstances and therefore subjectively may be happy and consider themselves satisfied with their life, while objectively are experiencing poor wellbeing (for example, below average life expectancy and housing conditions) and thus there is a risk of undermining legitimate claims for state intervention to support these groups. Conversely, there are risks that those with the highest wellbeing by objective measures will be the least satisfied subjectively – the “worried well” – and resources are directed from the people who need them most in objective terms. The current policy consensus is that subjective indicators should be used as a complement to, rather than replacement for, existing objective data on wellbeing (Bache, Reardon, & Anand, Citation2016).

There is therefore potential for evidence on wellbeing and its links to the commute to inform government policy in many ways. For example, the UK Airports Commission undertook a Quality of Life Assessment as part of its assessment of future airport capacity needs. The Commission assessed the links between SWB and four aviation factors (proximity to airports, aviation noise, working in airports and being at airports). It argued that “the ability to value the impact of airports on subjective wellbeing … provides an important potential input to understanding the scale of any mitigation that might be required before an airport scheme is attractive, especially in terms of non-market impacts” (Airports Commission, Citation2014, p. 50). It follows, therefore, that evidence on the links between commuting and SWB could also be used to inform policy interventions. For example, understanding more the links between commuting, SWB and productivity at work, could lead to work-placed interventions that help to mitigate the impact of the commute; not only for the benefit of employers but for individuals and society too.

4.3. Policy and research recommendations

We now consider recommendations for policy actions and research priorities which follow on from our understanding of the evidence base. These recommendations were collaboratively developed by the authors. summarises the recommendations, which are organised by six policy aims. The policy actions have been partly informed by the literature reviewed and partly based on the personal knowledge of the authors. Many of them have been applied in parts of the world, for other aims than improving the SWB of commuters, but we hope this review may provide an added impetus to adopt them.

Table 2. Policy and research recommendations.

The first policy aim is “Enhancing the commute experience” and draws upon the findings reported in section 3.1. Policy actions are identified to avoid commute stress by increasing the predictability and control of commute journeys and to increase positive affect by providing an environment which enables commuting to be enjoyable. Research recommendations focus on getting better knowledge on how features of the commute influence affect and evaluating how interventions can influence this.

The second policy aim is “Increasing commute satisfaction” which builds on the first policy aim but concerns overall satisfaction with the commute and draws upon the findings reported in section 3.2. Policy actions focus on measures to increase active commuting (for which there is strong evidence of higher commute satisfaction) and measures to reduce dissatisfaction associated with public transport use (since public transport is unavoidable for many commuters). Research recommendations focus on understanding differences amongst the working population in commute satisfaction and how this is affected by their commute contexts (design and environmental factors) and also evaluating how changes in people’s lives and the transport system influence this. It is also suggested that cost-effectiveness analyses be carried out to better understand which policy actions would deliver desired outcomes for the lowest possible cost, as well as to help potentially understand the return on public (or private) investment.

The third policy aim is “Reducing negative wellbeing impacts of long duration commutes” which draws upon the findings reported in sections 3.3–3.6. Policy actions focus on reducing the need for long duration commutes, reducing commute journey times across different transport modes and making journey times feel shorter. Research is recommended to understand in what circumstances long duration commutes are most damaging to SWB and whether benefits connected to jobs distant from where people live can be maintained when people live closer to their work. It is also important to understand longer term effects of commute dissatisfaction on SWB as virtually no evidence is available on this. Evaluations would be valuable to assess the outcomes of interventions aimed at reducing long duration commuting. There is a question of who bears responsibility for this issue. If workers choose situations involving long commutes should they not be the ones that bear the costs (SWB impacts) of this? This needs careful consideration of whether long commutes are a result of personal preferences or failures of markets and planning and whether they are unevenly distributed across the population and thus potentially contributing to inequality (in terms of accessibility, job prospects and so on), and thus whether they are an appropriate sphere for public intervention.

The fourth policy aim is “Meeting commuter preferences” which draws upon the findings reported in sections 3.2 and 3.7. This has policy recommendations to stimulate initiatives which offer the public more options regarding housing, jobs and commuting. Research is recommended to better understand how travel-related attitudes influence commuting satisfaction and to consider the competing issues that people face regarding their home, job and commuting choices and how they resolve them. This would enable better understanding of preferences and how they can be met.

The fifth policy aim is “Recognising flexibility and constraints in commuting routines” which draws upon the findings reported in sections 3.2 and 3.5. Studies have tended to assume that commuting is a repeated activity one working day to the next, but we know that commuting is changing as a result of more flexible working practices and greater provision of information to commuters on travel conditions and options. For example, while the average commute duration has increased from 27 min to 31 min in England between 2002 and 2017 (DfT, Citation2018a), this has been counteracted by the trend for workers to travel to their workplace less often (379 commute trips per year on average for those full-time employed in England in 2002 and 331 in 2017 [DfT, Citation2018c]). Furthermore, the proportion of people in employment working from home has increased (DfT, Citation2016). Policy recommendations focus on further increasing commuting flexibility. However, there will remain workers who have less discretion about how often and when they travel to work and these are likely to be those from lower socio-economic groups who also experience lower SWB for other reasons than commuting. It is important that the impacts of commuting on SWB are considered across the social gradient. Regarding research, we recommend in-depth studies investigating how commuting flexibility and constraints affect SWB and investigating the barriers which prevent workers from modifying their commutes.

The sixth and final policy aim is “Accounting for wellbeing impacts of commuting in policy making”. The systems used to appraise transport investment decisions consider the decision utility of travellers (for example, based on their willingness to trade off time for money) but we argue that experienced utility in the form of SWB impacts is neglected and may not closely coincide with decision utility. We advocate that procedures are developed to account for SWB impacts of transport and other public policy interventions that affect commuting and that research supports this by collecting the necessary evidence. We also advocate that SWB related to travel (i.e. commute satisfaction) is more routinely included in monitoring of the wellbeing and quality of life of the workforce and communities.

We finish with some observations about the policy process itself. The evidence presented in this paper is robust enough to highlight some key links between commuting and SWB (even if stronger evidence is needed to demonstrate cause and effect and many aspects need further research). Our view is that the evidence is not strong enough to be directly applied in project appraisal currently but can be incorporated into evidence-informed policy making. We believe the evidence is strong enough to provide a clear narrative (see section 4.1) to policy makers, the business sector and the public of the links between commuting and SWB. In turn, policy needs to use more measures of objective wellbeing and subjective wellbeing in project and performance evaluation in order to close the gaps in the evidence base and strengthen it going forward.

To conclude, the research reviewed in this paper has made an important contribution in enabling SWB, an outcome of transportation that has been largely neglected, to be brought into decision making and specific measures to be taken which can improve people’s lives. It offers a helpful new perspective on and impetus for transport policies and interventions, beyond the traditional goals of facilitating movement and managing “objective” negative externalities such as pollution and injuries.

Acknowledgements

This paper was written following a symposium on “Commuting and Wellbeing” which was held in Bristol on Friday 23rd June 2017 to bring together researchers working in this area. The symposium was financially supported by the Commuting & Wellbeing study which was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) (Grant Number ES/N012429/1). The project was led by Dr Kiron Chatterjee at the University of the West of England (UWE Bristol) and ran for eighteen months from February 2016 to July 2017.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council [grant number ES/N012429/1].

Notes

1 This data was collected by the European Foundation for the Improvement of Living and Working Conditions (EuroFound) and the methodology used is not described. Our view is that the National Travel Survey results are more reliable.

2 Potentially, commuting might elicit affective responses before the commute, for example, in preparation or anticipation of travel but this has not been investigated in studies to date.

References

  • Abou-Zeid, M., & Ben-Akiva, M. (2011). The effect of social comparisons on commute well-being. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 45(4), 345–361.
  • Airports Commission. (2014). Quality of life assessment. London: Price Waterhouse Coopers.
  • Anable, J., & Gatersleben, B. (2005). All work and no play? The role of instrumental and affective factors in work and leisure journeys by different travel modes. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 39(2–3), 163–181.
  • Audrey, S., Procter, S., & Cooper, A. (2014). The contribution of walking to work to adult physical activity levels: A cross sectional study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 11(1), 37.
  • Bache, I., Reardon, L., & Anand, P. (2016). Wellbeing as a wicked problem: Navigating the arguments for the role of government. Journal of Happiness Studies, 17, 893–912.
  • Bergstad, C. J., Gamble, A., Hagman, O., Polk, M., Gärling, T., & Olsson, L. E. (2011). Affective–symbolic and instrumental–independence psychological motives mediating effects of socio-demographic variables on daily car use. Journal of Transport Geography, 19(1), 33–38.
  • Böcker, L., Dijst, M., & Faber, J. (2016). Weather, transport mode choices and emotional travel experiences. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 94, 360–373.
  • Brutus, S., Javadian, R., & Panaccio, A. J. (2017). Cycling, car, or public transit: A study of stress and mood upon arrival at work. International Journal of Workplace Health Management, 10(1), 13–24.
  • Cao, X., & Ettema, D. (2014). Satisfaction with travel and residential self-selection: How do preferences moderate the impact of the Hiawatha Light Rail Transit line? Journal of Transport and Land Use, 7(3), 93–108.
  • Chatterjee, K., Clark, B., Martin, A., & Davis, A. (2017). The commuting and wellbeing study: Understanding the impact of commuting on people’s lives. Bristol: UWE Bristol. Retrieved from https://www.travelbehaviour.com/outputs-commuting-wellbeing/
  • Chng, S., White, M., Abraham, C., & Skippon, S. (2016). Commuting and wellbeing in London: The roles of commute mode and local public transport connectivity. Preventive Medicine, 88, 182–188.
  • Choi, J., Coughlin, J., & D’Ambrosio, L. (2013). Travel time and subjective well-being. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, 2357, 100–108.
  • Christian, T. (2012). Automobile commuting duration and the quantity of time spent with spouse, children, and friends. Preventive Medicine, 55, 215–218.
  • Clark, B., Chatterjee, K., Martin, A., & Davis, A. (2019). How commuting affects subjective wellbeing. Transportation. First online 11 March 2019. doi: 10.1007/s11116-019-09983-9
  • Crane, M., Rissel, C., Greaves, S., & Gebel, K. (2016). Correcting bias in self-rated quality of life: An application of anchoring vignettes and ordinal regression models to better understand QoL differences across commuting modes. Quality of Life Research, 25(2), 257–266.
  • De Kruijf, J., Ettema, D., & Dijst, M. (2019). A longitudinal evaluation of satisfaction with e-cycling in daily commuting in the Netherlands. Travel Behaviour and Society, 16, 192–200.
  • Delbosc, A. (2012). The role of well-being in transport policy. Transport Policy, 23, 25–33.
  • Delmelle, E., Haslauer, E., & Prinz, T. (2013). Social satisfaction, commuting and neighbourhoods. Journal of Transport Geography, 30, 110–116.
  • Denstedli, J. M., Julsrud, T. E., & Christiansen, P. (2017). Urban commuting – A threat to the work-family balance? Journal of Transport Geography, 61, 87–94.
  • De Vos, J. (2019). Satisfaction-induced travel behaviour. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 63, 12–21.
  • De Vos, J., Ettema, D., & Witlox, F. (2019). Effects of changing travel patterns on travel satisfaction: A focus on recently relocated residents. Travel Behaviour and Society, 16, 42–49.
  • De Vos, J., Mokhtarian, P. L., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., & Witlox, F. (2016). Travel mode choice and travel satisfaction: Bridging the gap between decision utility and experienced utility. Transportation, 43(5), 771–796.
  • De Vos, J., Schwanen, T., Van Acker, V., & Witlox, F. (2013). Travel and subjective well-being: A focus on findings, methods and future research needs. Transport Reviews, 33(4), 421–442.
  • De Vos, J., & Witlox, F. (2016). Do people live in urban neighbourhoods because they do not like to travel? Analysing an alternative residential self-selection hypothesis. Travel Behaviour and Society, 4, 29–39.
  • De Vos, J., & Witlox, F. (2017). Travel satisfaction revisited. On the pivotal role of travel satisfaction in conceptualising a travel behaviour process. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 106, 364–373.
  • DfT. (2016). Table NTS0804. Workplace and working at home, England, since 2002. UK: Department for Transport. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/statistical-data-sets/nts08-availability-and-distance-from-key-local-services
  • DfT. (2018a). Table NTS0403. Average number of trips, miles and time spent travelling by trip purpose. England, UK: Department for Transport. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/statistical-data-sets/nts04-purpose-of-trips
  • DfT. (2018b). Table TSGB0110. Time taken to travel to work by region of workplace. UK: Department for Transport. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/statistical-data-sets/tsgb01-modal-comparisons
  • DfT. (2018c). Table NTS0411. Trips and distance by commuters by employment status: England, 2002 to 2017. UK: Department for Transport. Retrieved from https://www.gov.uk/government/statistical-data-sets/nts04-purpose-of-trips
  • Dickerson, A., Hole, A., & Munford, L. (2014). The relationship between well-being and commuting revisited: Does the choice of methodology matter? Regional Science and Urban Economics, 49, 321–329.
  • Dolan, P., Peasgood, T., & White, M. (2008). Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Psychology, 29(1), 94–122.
  • Donaire-Gonzalez, D., de Nazelle, A., & Cole-Hunter, T. (2015). The added benefit of bicycle commuting on the regular amount of physical activity performed. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 49(6), 842–849.
  • Ekkekakis, P., Backhouse, S. H., Gray, C., & Lind, E. (2008). Walking is popular among adults but is it pleasant? A framework for clarifying the link between walking and affect as illustrated in two studies. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9(3), 246–264.
  • Ettema, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., Olsson, L. E., & Fujii, S. (2012). How in-vehicle activities affect work commuters’ satisfaction with public transport. Journal of Transport Geography, 24, 215–222.
  • Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Eriksson, L., Friman, M., Olsson, L. E., & Fujii, S. (2011). Satisfaction with travel and subjective well-being: Development and test of a measurement tool. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 14(3), 167–175.
  • Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Olsson, L. E., & Friman, M. (2010). Out-of-home activities, daily travel, and subjective well-being. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 44(9), 723–732.
  • Ettema, D., Gärling, T., Olsson, L. E., Friman, M., & Moerdijk, S. (2013). The road to happiness: Measuring Dutch car drivers’ satisfaction with travel. Transport Policy, 27, 171–178.
  • Eurostat. (2019). Mean duration of commuting time one-way between work and home by sex and age (source: Eurofound). Luxembourg: Eurostat. Retrieved from https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/about/overview/how-to-find-us
  • Evans, G. W., Wener, R. E., & Phillips, D. (2002). The morning rush hour: Predictability and commuter stress. Environment and Behavior, 34(4), 521–530.
  • Flint, E., & Cummins, S. (2016). Active commuting and obesity in mid-life: Cross-sectional, observational evidence from UK Biobank. The Lancet Diabetes & Endocrinology, 4(5), 420–435.
  • Friman, M., Gärling, T., Ettema, D., & Olsson, L. E. (2017). How does travel affect emotional well-being and life satisfaction? Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 106, 170–180.
  • Friman, M., Olsson, L. E., Ståhl, M., Ettema, D., & Gärling, T. (2017). Travel and residual emotional well-being. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 49, 159–176.
  • Furth, P. G., Mekuria, M. C., & Nixon, H. (2016). Network connectivity for low-stress bicycling. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, 2587, 41–49.
  • Gatersleben, B., & Uzzell, D. (2007). Affective appraisals of the daily commute: Comparing perceptions of drivers, cyclists, walkers, and users of public transport. Environment and Behavior, 39(3), 416–431.
  • Grant, M. J., & Booth, A. (2009). A typology of reviews: An analysis of 14 review types and associated methodologies. Health Information and Libraries Journal, 26(2), 91–108.
  • Handy, S., & Thigpen, C. (2019). Commute quality and its implications for commute satisfaction: Exploring the role of mode, location, and other factors. Travel Behaviour and Society, 16, 241–248.
  • Hansson, E., Mattisson, K., Björk, J., Östergren, P. O., & Jakobsson, K. (2011). Relationship between commuting and health outcomes in a cross-sectional population survey in southern Sweden. BMC Public Health, 11(1), 834.
  • Hendriksen, I. J., Simons, M., Garre, F. G., & Hildebrandt, V. H. (2010). The association between commuter cycling and sickness absence. Preventive Medicine, 51(2), 132–135.
  • Hilbrecht, M., Smale, B., & Mock, S. E. (2014). Highway to health? Commute time and well-being among Canadian adults. World Leisure Journal, 56, 151–163.
  • Hoehner, C. M., Barlow, C. E., Allen, P., & Schootman, M. (2012). Commuting distance, cardiorespiratory fitness, and metabolic risk. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 42(6), 571–578.
  • Humphreys, D. K., Goodman, A., & Ogilvie, D. (2013). Associations between active commuting and physical and mental wellbeing. Preventive Medicine, 57(2), 135–139.
  • Ingenfeld, J., Wolbring, T., & Bless, H. (2018). Commuting and life satisfaction revisited: Evidence on a non-linear relationship. Journal of Happiness Studies. First online 18 December 2018. doi: 10.1007/s10902-018-0064-2
  • Kageyama, T., Nishikido, N., Kobayashi, T., Kurokawa, Y., & Kaneko, M. (1998). Long commuting time, extensive overtime, and sympathodominant state assessed in terms of short-term heart rate variability among male white-collar workers in the Tokyo megalopolis. Industrial Health, 36(3), 209–217.
  • Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306(5702), 1776–1780.
  • Kluger, A. (1998). Commute variability and strain. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19(2), 147–165.
  • Koslowsky, M., Kluger, A., & Reich, M. (2013). Commuting stress: Causes, effects, and methods of coping. New York: Springer.
  • Kroesen, M. (2014). Assessing mediators in the relationship between commute time and subjective wellbeing: Structural equation analysis. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, 2452, 114–123.
  • Kroll, C. (2011). Measuring progress and well-being: Achievements and challenges of a new global movement. Berlin: International Policy Analysis.
  • Künn-Nelen, A. (2015). Does commuting affect health? Health Economics, 25(8), 984–1004.
  • Lancée, S., Veenhoven, R., & Burger, M. (2017). Mood during commute in the Netherlands: What way of travel feels best for what kind of people? Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 104, 195–208.
  • Legrain, A., Eluru, N., & El-Geneidy, A. (2015). Am stressed, must travel: The relationship between mode choice and commuting stress. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 34, 141–151.
  • Loong, C., van Lierop, D., & El-Geneidy, A. (2017). On time and ready to go: An analysis of commuters’ punctuality and energy levels at work or school. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 45, 1–13.
  • Lorenz, O. (2018). Does commuting matter to subjective well-being? Journal of Transport Geography, 66, 180–199.
  • Lucas, J. L., & Heady, R. B. (2002). Flextime commuters and their driver stress, feelings of time urgency, and commute satisfaction. Journal of Business and Psychology, 16(4), 565–571.
  • Lundberg, U. (1976). Urban commuting: Crowdedness and catecholamine excretion. Journal of Human Stress, 2(3), 26–32.
  • MacDonald, J., Stokes, R., & Cohen, D. (2010). The effect of light rail transit on Body Mass Index and physical activity. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 39(2), 105–112.
  • Manaugh, K., & El-Geneidy, A. M. (2013). Does distance matter? Exploring the links among values, motivations, home location, and satisfaction in walking trips. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 50, 198–208.
  • Mao, Z., Ettema, D., & Dijst, M. (2016). Commuting trip satisfaction in Beijing: Exploring the influence of multimodal behavior and modal flexibility. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 94, 592–603.
  • Martin, A., Goryakin, Y., & Suhrcke, M. (2014). Does active commuting improve psychological wellbeing? Longitudinal evidence from eighteen waves of the British Household Panel Survey. Preventive Medicine, 69, 296–303.
  • Martin, A., Panter, J., Suhrcke, M., & Ogilvie, D. (2015). Impact of changes in mode of travel to work on changes in body mass index: Evidence from the British Household Panel Survey. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 69(8), 753–761.
  • Mattisson, K., Håkansson, C., & Jakobsson, K. (2015). Relationships between commuting and social capital among men and women in southern Sweden. Environment and Behavior, 47(7), 734–753.
  • Mokhtarian, P. L. (2019). Subjective well-being and travel: Retrospect and prospect. Transportation, 46(2), 493–513.
  • Mokhtarian, P. L., Papon, F., Goulard, M., & Diana, M. (2015). What makes travel pleasant and/or tiring? An investigation based on the French National Travel Survey. Transportation, 42(6), 1103–1128.
  • Morris, E. A., & Guerra, E. (2015a). Mood and mode: Does how we travel affect how we feel? Transportation, 42(1), 25–43.
  • Morris, E. A., & Guerra, E. (2015b). Are we there yet? Trip duration and mood during travel. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 33, 38–47.
  • Morris, E. A., & Zhou, Y. (2018). Are long commutes short on benefits? Commute duration and various manifestations of well-being. Travel Behaviour and Society, 11, 101–110.
  • Mytton, O. T., Panter, J., & Ogilvie, D. (2016). Longitudinal associations of active commuting with wellbeing and sickness absence. Preventive Medicine, 84, 19–26.
  • Nie, P., & Sousa-Poza, A. (2018). Commute time and subjective well-being in urban China. China Economic Review, 48, 188–204.
  • Nordbakke, S., & Schwanen, T. (2014). Well-being and mobility: A theoretical framework and literature review focusing on older people. Mobilities, 9(1), 104–129.
  • Novaco, R. W., & Gonzalez, O. I. (2009). Commuting and well-being. Technology and Well-Being, 3, 174–174.
  • Novaco, R. W., Kliewer, W., & Broquet, A. (1991). Home environment consequences of commute travel impedance. American Journal of Community Psychology, 19, 881–909.
  • Novaco, R. W., Stokols, D., Campbell, J., & Stokols, J. (1979). Transportation, stress, and community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 7(4), 361–380.
  • Novaco, R. W., Stokols, D., & Milanesi, L. (1990). Objective and subjective dimensions of travel impedance as determinants of commuting stress. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(2), 231–257.
  • OECD. (2011). How’s life? Measuring well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264121164-en
  • OECD. (2013). OECD guidelines on measuring subjective well-being. Paris: OECD Publishing. doi: 10.1787/9789264191655-en
  • Olsson, L. E., Gärling, T., Ettema, D., Friman, M., & Fujii, S. (2013). Happiness and satisfaction with work commute. Social Indicators Research, 111(1), 255–263.
  • ONS. (2014). Commuting and personal wellbeing. UK: Office for National Statistics. Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/dcp171766_351954.pdf
  • Ory, D. T., & Mokhtarian, P. L. (2005). When is getting there half the fun? Modeling the liking for travel. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 39(2), 97–123.
  • Ory, D. T., Mokhtarian, P. L., Redmond, L. S., Salomon, I., Collantes, G. O., & Choo, S. (2004). When is commuting desirable to the individual? Growth and Change, 35(3), 334–359.
  • Reardon, L., & Abdallah, S. (2013). Well-being and transport: Taking stock and looking forward. Transport Reviews, 33(6), 634–657.
  • Roberts, J., Hodgson, R., & Dolan, P. (2011). “It’s driving her mad”: Gender differences in the effects of commuting on psychological health. Journal of Health Economics, 30, 1064–1076.
  • Ruger, H., Pfaff, S., Weishaar, H., & Wiernik, B. M. (2017). Does perceived stress mediate the relationship between commuting and health-related quality of life? Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 50, 100–108.
  • Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178.
  • Sahlqvist, S., Goodman, A., Cooper, A., & Ogilvie, D. (2013). Change in active travel and changes in recreational and total physical activity in adults: Longitudinal findings from the iConnect study. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 10(1), 28.
  • Schaeffer, M. H., Street, S. W., Singer, J. E., & Baum, A. (1988). Effects of control on the stress reactions of commuters. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18(11), 944–957.
  • Schneider, R. J., & Willman, J. L. (2019). Move closer and get active: How to make urban university commutes more satisfying. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 60, 462–473.
  • Singleton, P. A. (2019). Walking (and cycling) to well-being: Modal and other determinants of subjective well-being during the commute. Travel Behaviour and Society, 16, 249–261.
  • Sposato, R. G., Röderer, K., & Cervinka, R. (2012). The influence of control and related variables on commuting stress. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 15(5), 581–587.
  • St-Louis, E., Manaugh, K., van Lierop, D., & El-Geneidy, A. (2014). The happy commuter: A comparison of commuter satisfaction across modes. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 26, 160–170.
  • Stutzer, A., & Frey, B. (2008). Stress that doesn’t pay: The commuting paradox. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 110(2), 339–366.
  • Susilo, Y. O., & Cats, O. (2014). Exploring key determinants of travel satisfaction for multi-modal trips by different traveler groups. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 67, 366–380.
  • Tainio, M., de Nazelle, A. J., Götschi, T., Kahlmeier, S., Rojas-Rueda, D., Nieuwenhuijsen, M. J., … Woodcock, J. (2016). Can air pollution negate the health benefits of cycling and walking? Preventive Medicine, 87, 233–236.
  • Tinkler, L., & Hicks, S. (2011). Measuring subjective well-being. Newport: Office for National Statistics. Retrieved from http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20160105231554/http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/user-guidance/well-being/publications/previous-publications/index.html
  • USCB. (2017). Average one-way commuting time by metropolitan areas. Washington, DC: United States Census Bureau. Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/library/visualizations/interactive/travel-time.html
  • Walsleben, J. A., Norman, R. G., Novak, R. D., O’Malley, E. B., Rapoport, D. M., & Strohl, K. P. (1999). Sleep habits of Long Island rail road commuters. Sleep, 22(6), 728–734.
  • Wanner, M., Götschi, T., Martin-Diener, E., Kahlmeier, S., & Martin, B. W. (2012). Active transport, physical activity, and body weight in adults: A systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 42(5), 493–502.
  • Watts, L., & Lyons, G. (2010). Travel remedy kit: Interventions into train lines and passenger times. In M. Büscher, J. Urry, & K. Witchger (Eds.), Mobile methods (pp. 189–213). Abingdon: Routledge.
  • Wener, R., Evans, G., & Boately, P. (2005). Commuting stress: Psychophysiological effects of a trip and spillover into the workplace. Transportation Research Record: Journal of the Transportation Research Board, 1924, 112–117.
  • Wheatley, D. (2014). Travel-to-work and subjective well-being: A study of UK dual career households. Journal of Transport Geography, 39, 187–196.
  • White, S. M., & Rotton, J. (1998). Type of commute, behavioral after effects, and cardiovascular activity - A field experiment. Environment and Behavior, 30(6), 763–780.
  • Ye, R., & Titheridge, H. (2017). Satisfaction with the commute: The role of travel mode choice, built environment and attitudes. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment, 52, 535–547.
  • Ye, R., & Titheridge, H. (2019). The determinants of commuting satisfaction in low-income population: A case study of Xi’an, China. Travel Behaviour and Society, 16, 272–283.