1,119
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Uncovering latent profiles of ICT self-concept among adults in Germany and their relation with gender

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 4-20 | Received 24 May 2022, Accepted 19 Oct 2022, Published online: 29 Nov 2022

ABSTRACT

Self-concept related to the use of information and communication technology (ICT-SC) is reflected in how people feel and behave when confronted with digital technologies. Although evidence from variable-centered analyses suggests a hierarchical and multidimensional structure of ICT-SC in heterogeneous populations, it is not yet known whether different profiles of general ICT-SC and specific ICT-SC domains (communicate, process and store, generate content, safe application, solve problems) exist. This study aims to extend previous research using person-centered analyses and to examine whether different profiles of ICT-SC can be identified in a heterogeneous adult population (18–69 years) from Germany and how these profiles relate to gender. Results of a latent profile analysis (German quota sample, N = 369) indicate a reliable three-profile solution. Profile I (n = 48) is characterised by rather low ICT-SC with relative profile strengths in the verbal-interactive domains (communicate, process and store). Profile II (n = 149) is characterised by low to average ICT-SC across ICT-SC domains. Profile III (n = 172) is characterised by high ICT-SC with profile strengths in the technical-analytical domains (safe application, solve problems). Gender did not correlate significantly with profile membership. We discuss the practical implications of the results for ICT-SC interventions and suggest directions for future research.

CRediT author statement

Isabelle Schmidt: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Software, Data curation, Writing-Original draft preparation, Formal analysis, Visualisation, Investigation, Supervision; Nathalie Schauffel: Conceptualisation, Methodology, Software, Writing-Original draft preparation, Formal analysis, Visualisation, Validation; Thomas Ellwart: Writing-Reviewing and Editing.

1. Introduction

In recent years, the use of information and communication technology (ICT) has become a daily practice in private, professional, and educational life. Individuals’ interest in and acceptance and use of ICT, as well as their perception of technostrain and computer anxiety depend on their competence self-beliefs, in particular their ICT-related self-concept (ICT-SC; Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012; Rubach and Lazarides Citation2019; Schauffel et al. Citation2021b).

ICT-SC is a multidimensional and hierarchically structured set of ICT-related competence self-beliefs (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b) comprising general ICT-SC and domain-specific ICT-SCs (i.e. communicate, process and store, generate content, safe application, and solve problems). It represents a new self-concept field that has received increasing research attention in the last decade (see, e.g. Christoph et al. Citation2015; Langheinrich, Schönfelder, and Bogner Citation2016; Schauffel and Ellwart Citation2021; Zylka et al. Citation2015). To investigate the antecedents and consequences of ICT-SC in more detail, knowledge about prevailing ICT-SC profiles with different configurations in terms of the level (i.e. high vs. low general ICT-SC) and shape (i.e. verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical ICT-SCs) of ICT-SC is essential.

Self-concept research has a long research tradition in educational science. A plethora of variable-centered studies in this field support a multidimensional structure of academic self-concept (i.e. individuals’ perceptions of their own academic abilities in general or in specific domains; see Arens et al. Citation2021; Brunner et al. Citation2010) and also of the recently introduced construct of ICT-SC (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b). Advanced person-centered analyses (e.g. latent profile analysis, LPA) complement traditional self-concept research because they enable researchers to identify distinct profiles of self-concept characterised by differences in level and shape and thus to detect heterogeneity in and provide detailed insight into structures of self-concept in populations (Marsh et al. Citation2009; Meeusen et al. Citation2018). The few studies that have investigated heterogeneity in the structure of academic self-concept (Franzen et al. Citation2022; Marsh et al. Citation2009; Saß and Kampa Citation2019) point to the existence of subgroups with different self-concept profiles. An even smaller number of studies have investigated profiles of self-perceived ICT competence (Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021; Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest Citation2021). The herein-focused self-perceived ICT competence is a concept similar to ICT-SC. ICT-SC describes the own mental representations and evaluations of competences in using ICT (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b) and belongs to the group of competence self-beliefs (Marsh et al. Citation2017). However, ICT-SC is a broader concept, as it includes aside from self-assessed ICT competence (‘I can’), also its evaluation (‘I am good at’), and mental representations of temporal aspects (‘I have always been good at’, ‘I quickly learn’). Also, ICT-SC is typically considered from a domain-specific perspective (i.e. competence domains), rather than focussing on single subcompetences. For an overview and in-depth comparison of different competence self-beliefs, including self-concept, we recommend Marsh et al. (Citation2017) (no specific focus on ICT). ICT-related competence self-beliefs are discussed in Peiffer et al. (Citation2020). These existing studies found different profiles of self-perceived ICT competence. However, the samples that they used were rather homogeneous (e.g. student teachers in the science, technology, engineering, and mathematics [STEM] fields). Hence, it is unclear if their findings regarding distinct profiles apply also to more heterogeneous samples, for example, adults with a broad range of ages and educational and professional backgrounds.

Investigating self-concept profiles in heterogeneous samples is highly relevant because self-concepts have the potential to influence vocational choices and thus professional career paths (see expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation; Eccles and Wigfield Citation2002). The fact that women are still underrepresented in degree programmes and jobs in the STEM fields worldwide (Blackburn Citation2017; World Economic Forum [WEF], Citation2021), especially in the IT sector, might be attributable to the fact that they have lower general ICT-SC than men (Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012). However, it might also be due to differences in domain-specific ICT-SCs or different configurations (i.e. ICT-SC profiles) of general and domain-specific ICT-SCs between women and men.

Thus, besides identifying different ICT-SC profiles, it is highly relevant to investigate whether gender differences in profile membership exist. Previous studies have indicated mean-level gender differences in general ICT-SC (Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012) and domain-specific ICT-SCs (Fraillon et al. Citation2014; Gómez-Trigueros and Yáñez de Aldecoa Citation2021). Furthermore, first evidence from academic self-concept research points to gender differences in profile membership (e.g. Franzen et al. Citation2022) corresponding to the math/language–gender stereotypes described in the literature (see, e.g. Morrissey et al. Citation2019), whereby women perceive themselves as more talented than men in verbal-like domains (e.g. native language) than in math-like domains (e.g. mathematics and physics), and vice versa. The math/verbal domains distinction of these gender stereotypes aligns well with ICT domains (i.e. verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical). Hence, similar findings might be expected for ICT-SC as for academic self-concept, although research is still lacking.

To pave the way for a nuanced understanding of hierarchical and multidimensional ICT-SC in heterogeneous populations, this study uses LPA with gender as a covariate to examine whether different profiles of ICT-SC exist in the adult population in Germany and whether and to what extent gender correlates with profile membership.

With this study, we contribute theoretically and practically to existing research. By applying person-centered analyses that account for interindividual variation in the structure of ICT-SC, not accounted for in variable-centered analyses, we extend theoretical knowledge of the structure of ICT-SC in heterogeneous populations (Marsh et al. Citation2009) and provide insight into whether math/language–gender stereotypes in competence perceptions apply also to domain-specific ICT-SCs (i.e. verbal-interactive domains vs. technical-analytical domains).

Practically, identifying distinct ICT-SC profiles and investigating whether and to what extent they covary with gender allows for customised, profile-specific ICT-SC interventions that address the support needs of a given population.

2. Theoretical background

2.1. Multidimensional structure of (ICT)-SC

Self-concept represents a set of self-perceptions of an individual’s abilities on a general level and in specific domains. Self-concept research is particularly prominent in educational science. A plethora of research in that field supports the hierarchical (e.g. students’ overall school self-concept) and multidimensional (e.g. students’ subject-specific self-concepts) structure of academic self-concept (Arens et al. Citation2021; Brunner et al. Citation2010) and its influence on performance, motivation, performance-related behaviour, and vocational choices (for a summary, see Marsh et al. Citation2017).

With increasing digitalisation, ICT-SC has entered scientific discussion (e.g. Christoph et al. Citation2015; Langheinrich, Schönfelder, and Bogner Citation2016; Schauffel and Ellwart Citation2021; Zylka et al. Citation2015). ICT-SC can be defined as individuals’ own mental representations and evaluations of their competence in using ICT (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b). Previous research indicates that like academic self-concept (see Arens et al. Citation2021; Brunner et al. Citation2010), ICT-SC is also hierarchical and multidimensionally structured: A general self-concept related to the use of ICT is located on a global level, and five domain-specific self-concepts (e.g. ICT-SC related to communication or safe application) are located on a lower hierarchical level (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b). This structure has been modelled in a factor analytical approach as an incomplete bifactor model called the nested Marsh/Shavelson model (see Brunner et al. Citation2010). A special feature of this model is that it allows to clearly separate the level and shape of an individual’s self-concept profile. This is achieved by including a direct measure of general ICT-SC in the model. General self-concept (‘level’) is comprised of items that measure general self-concept directly and is, in addition, also shaped by the shared variance of all domain-specific ICT-SC items (i.e. the variance that can be attributed to the ‘level’ of ICT-SC). Thus, the domain-specific ICT-SCs contain only variance unique to the respective domains and depict the shape of an individual’s profile (see , lower part).

Figure 1. Latent profiles of ICT self-concept and gender as a covariate.

Notes: Factor scores of the five domain-specific ICT self-concept (ICT-SCs) and general ICT-SC based on the nested Marsh/Shavelson model provided the input for the latent profile analysis. ICT-SC = self-concept related to information and communication technology; H1 = hypothesis 1; H2 = hypothesis 2; SCCO = ICT-SC domain communicate; SCPS = ICT-SC domain process and store; SCGE = ICT-SC domain generate content; SCSA = ICT-SC domain safe application; SCSP = ICT-SC domain solve problems; SCGL1-5 = items measuring general ICT-SC; ICTSCg = g-factor general ICT-SC.

Figure 1. Latent profiles of ICT self-concept and gender as a covariate.Notes: Factor scores of the five domain-specific ICT self-concept (ICT-SCs) and general ICT-SC based on the nested Marsh/Shavelson model provided the input for the latent profile analysis. ICT-SC = self-concept related to information and communication technology; H1 = hypothesis 1; H2 = hypothesis 2; SCCO = ICT-SC domain communicate; SCPS = ICT-SC domain process and store; SCGE = ICT-SC domain generate content; SCSA = ICT-SC domain safe application; SCSP = ICT-SC domain solve problems; SCGL1-5 = items measuring general ICT-SC; ICTSCg = g-factor general ICT-SC.

The five domains of ICT-SC correspond to the five areas of digital competence described in the earliest version of the European Digital Competence Framework for Citizens (DigComp; Ferrari Citation2013) and in the updated versions, DigComp 2.0 (Vuorikari et al. Citation2016) and DigComp 2.1 (Carretero, Vuorikari, and Punie Citation2017). Following Schauffel et al. (Citation2021b), the ICT-SC domain communicate refers to individuals’ evaluations of their own competences ‘related to communicating with relevant others through the use of ICT’ (p. 4); the ICT-SC domain process and store represents individuals’ evaluations of their own competences ‘related to processing and storing digital data, information, and content’ (p. 4); the ICT-SC domain generate content refers to individuals’ evaluations of their own competences ‘related to creating digital data, information, and content’ (p. 4); and the ICT-SC domain safe application represents individuals’ evaluations of their own competences ‘related to protecting digital data, information, and content as well as an entire system’ (p. 4). Schauffel et al. (Citation2021b) noted that the domain safe application ‘also refers to the responsible use of ICT’ (p. 4). And finally, the ICT-SC domain solve problems represents individuals’ evaluations of their own competences ‘related to successfully solving emerging technical problems of ICT’ and ‘to solving content-related tasks, challenges, and problems using ICT’ (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b, p. 4).

2.2. The role of dimensional comparisons in the development of (ICT)-SC profiles

Self-concepts are formed by experiences, interactions with the environment, feedback from significant others (Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton Citation1976), and comparison processes within different frames of reference (see Marsh Citation1986; Wolff et al. Citation2019). For the development of the multidimensional structure of self-concept, comparison processes within an internal frame of reference (i.e. dimensional comparisons) are crucial (e.g. Marsh Citation1986). To explain the multidimensional structure of academic self-concept, dimensional comparison theory (DCT, Möller and Marsh Citation2013) was developed and validated for academic self-concept (Marsh et al. Citation2017). DCT posits that individuals use an internal frame of reference to compare their performance in one self-concept domain (e.g. math) with their performance in another self-concept domain (e.g. their native language). Dimensional comparisons can result in contrast or assimilation effects, depending on the perceived dissimilarity of, or distance between the target domains. In DCT, competence domains are arranged on a continuum ranging from verbal to math-like (Marsh, Byrne, and Shavelson Citation1988). In the academic context, the verbal–math continuumFootnote1 refers to the self-concept related to school subjects ranging from native and foreign languages to STEM subjects. Contrast effects are more likely to occur between domains that are expected to be located at the endpoints of a competence continuum (i.e. native language and math). Contrast effects result in a lower self-concept in the competence domain in which the individual performs less well (e.g. in terms of grades) and in higher self-concept in the competence domain in which the individual performs better (Möller and Marsh Citation2013). By contrast, assimilation effects are more likely to occur between domains in which individuals are expected to have related or similar competences – that is, domains that are located adjacent to each other on a competence continuum (e.g. STEM subjects). Assimilation effects lead to positive correlations between domain-specific self-concepts.

DCT thus offers a theoretical explanation of why individuals perceive strengths and weaknesses in their academic self-concept (i.e. perceiving themselves as more competent in the verbal domain than in the mathematical domain, or vice versa). Because the ICT-SC domains show substantive similarities with competence domains in the academic context, it can be expected that the continuum of verbal and mathematical competences in the academic context will be reflected in the ICT context and that DCT should therefore also be applicable to ICT-SC. The verbal-interactive ICT-SC facet communicate is similar to the verbal domains of academic self-concept, whereas the technical-analytical ICT-SC facets solve problems and safe application align with the mathematical academic competence domains. As the ICT-SC facets process and store and generate content integrate to some extent both verbal and technical-analytical abilities, they might fall near the middle of the competence continuum.

The complex pattern of dimensional comparison processes in self-concept development suggests an idiographic process of self-concept structure and thus implies distinct profiles of ICT-SC configurations. The idiosyncratic pattern of assimilation and contrast effects can contribute to explaining why some self-concept profiles are rather heterogeneous (i.e. differences between self-concept domains) and others homogeneous (i.e. uniform self-concept levels across domains, Marsh et al. Citation2009).

For example, a rather heterogeneous ICT-SC profile could be characterised by high general ICT-SC and high ICT-SC in the competence domain communicate but low ICT-SC in the competence domain safe application. Exemplarily, a homogeneous ICT-SC profile could be characterised by high or low levels of ICT-SC across the five competence domains. However, person-centered approaches to investigating self-concepts are still scarce. Most evidence stems from variable-centered approaches such as factor analysis, correlation, or regression analysis (see Arens et al. Citation2021). These methods focus on the relationship between variables and assume homogeneity across individuals (i.e. comparable variable relations across all individuals) but disregard potential individual differences in ICT-SC configurations.

To investigate self-concept profiles, person-centered analyses such as latent class analysis (LCA, for categorical variables), latent profile analysis (LPA, for continuous variables), or latent transition analysis (for longitudinal data) are necessary (Hickendorff et al. Citation2018). These person-centered approaches consider heterogeneity in variable interactions (i.e. distinct profiles) among individuals and can thus detect similar patterns of self-concept profiles clustering individuals into subgroups. Hence, person-centered analyses complement variable-centered ones (Marsh et al. Citation2009; Robins et al. Citation1996).

2.3. Research status of (ICT)-SC profiles

LPA is especially useful in ICT-SC research, as assumptions derived from DCT (Möller and Marsh Citation2013) suggest the existence of qualitatively distinct ICT-SC profiles. In line with these models, self-concept configuration should differ not only according to the general level of ICT-SC (i.e. quantitative differences such as high, medium, or low ICT-SC) but also regarding the shape of the five ICT-SC competence domains (i.e. qualitative differences such as high ICT-SC in the domain communicate but low ICT-SC in the domain safe application). Previous research investigating academic self-concept has revealed different profiles. For instance, applying LPA in a sample of upper secondary school students, Marsh et al. (Citation2009) investigated profile levels (high vs. low) and profile shapes (mathematical vs. verbal self-concept profile) and found distinct self-concept profiles. Similarly, Saß and Kampa (Citation2019) identified four distinct self-concept profiles characterised by high math self-concept, high verbal self-concept, low overall self-concept, and high overall self-concept, respectively. Marsh et al. (Citation2009) and Saß and Kampa (Citation2019) also included technical-analytical self-concept domains (e.g. math, computers, problem solving). They found distinct configurations across the profiles – for instance, a profile with either high self-concept in technical-analytical self-concept domains and low verbal self-concept, or vice versa. In a longitudinal study, Franzen et al. (Citation2022) found evidence for similar distinct profiles in a student sample that were stable over time.

In the context of ICT use, two empirical studies (Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest Citation2021; Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021) examined profiles of self-perceived competence in the five areas of ICT competence proposed in DigComp (Ferrari Citation2013). Schulze Heuling and Wild (Citation2021) used LPA in a sample of students in vocational education and training and found four profiles that differed mainly in the level of self-perceived ICT competence (i.e. high vs. low ICT competence). In particular, one profile showed a weak ICT competence profile across all five competence areas, with extremely low ICT competence in safety, a rather technical-analytical competence area.

In another study (Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest Citation2021), a similar pattern of results was described in a sample comprising engineering students and student science teachers. The authors found four ICT competence profiles (high, rather high, rather low, low), differing mainly in the level of ICT competence domains. The majority of the sample members were assigned to the profiles with high or rather high ICT competence (27% and 44%, respectively).

In sum, there is first evidence for the existence of distinct academic self-concept profiles and ICT-SC profiles in specific samples (i.e. engineering students and student science teachers; students in vocational education and training). Whether previous findings can be transferred to more heterogeneous populations remains an open question. Comparable latent profile analyses for heterogeneous populations do not exist, to our knowledge, but would be valuable since ICT use became an essential demand of individuals’ private (e.g. e-government, e-health) and professional lives, across ages and beyond typically IT-related professions (e.g. nurses: Duffy Citation2012; digital farming: Shamshiri et al. Citation2018). Moreover, the studies by Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest (Citation2021) and Schulze Heuling and Wild (Citation2021) did not aim to assess ICT-SC specifically but rather self-reported ICT competence. Furthermore, they did not consider the hierarchical structure of self-concepts (see ) that implies a distinction between general ICT-SC and domain-specific ICT-SCs and thus allows to capture the level and shape of the profile in a manner consistent with the theory. To achieve this, it is necessary to include a measure of general ICT-SC alongside measures of domain-specific ICT-SCs and to use an incomplete bifactor model to model the structure of ICT-SC (see Section 2.1). Investigating the shape of an ICT-SC profile separately from its general level can lead to a clearer shape of the profile because domain-specific ICT-SCs otherwise contain shared variance that can be attributed to general ICT-SC (see Schmidt et al. Citation2017).

2.4. Gender as a covariate of (ICT-)SC profile membership

When assuming different profiles of ICT-SC, the question arises as to what factors might correspond to profile membership. Here, gender, as a person characteristic, should be carefully examined. Men typically report higher self-concepts than women in more technical-analytical domains (e.g. computers: Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012; mathematics: Mejía-Rodríguez, Luyten, and Meelissen Citation2021), whereas women typically report higher self-concepts than men in more verbal-interactive domains (e.g. Esnaola et al. Citation2020). Because self-concepts (profiles) have the power to impact both vocational choices and performance (e.g. Saß and Kampa Citation2019), and thus individuals’ professional careers (see also expectancy-value theory; Eccles and Wigfield Citation2002), self-concept differences might explain why women are still underrepresented in more technical-analytical occupational fields (Blackburn Citation2017; Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012). Here, evidence from person-centered analyses would complement evidence from variable-centered analyses. Confirming – or refuting – the gender-specificity of ICT-SC profiles would allow conclusions to be drawn regarding the customised design and promotion of self-concept interventions by educators, institutions, and human resource developers.

Results from previous studies using variable-centered analyses point to gender differences in general ICT-SC and domain-specific ICT-SCs in favour of men (Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012; Marsh et al. Citation2009; Sáinz and Eccles Citation2012; Schauffel et al. Citation2021b) that correspond to career motivation (Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012). Further, longitudinal research in a secondary school context suggests that the more positive computer-related self-concept of male students increases over time, whereas the lower computer-related self-concept of female students decreases over time (Sáinz and Eccles Citation2012).

Previous studies investigating academic self-concept profiles using person-centered analyses have found evidence for gender differences in profile membership. Recent studies by Saß and Kampa (Citation2019) and Franzen et al. (Citation2022) revealed that males are overrepresented in high math profiles and high/moderate overall profiles, whereas females are overrepresented in high verbal profiles. Given the substantive similarity of ICT domains and academic self-concept domains (i.e. verbal vs. mathematical; verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical), the findings for academic self-concept should be transferable to ICT-SC. However, research investigating this has been lacking to date.

3. Present research

In this study, we investigate latent ICT-SC profiles (see , bold lines) and their covariation with gender. We aim to contribute to a better understanding of the structure of ICT-SC in heterogeneous adult populations.

Integrating and reflecting on previous research findings that revealed distinct profiles of academic self-concept (e.g. Franzen et al. Citation2022) and self-perceived ICT competence in homogeneous samples (e.g. Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021), and assuming the applicability of DCT (Marsh, Parker, and Craven Citation2015; Möller and Marsh Citation2013) to ICT-SC, we hypothesise:

Hypothesis 1 (H1): Latent ICT-SC profiles showing differences in shape (verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical ICT-SC) and level (high vs. low general ICT-SC) exist within the adult population.

Further, transferring to the context of ICT use results from previous research in academic settings that point to gender differences in profile membership, we examine gender as a person-related covariate of profile membership (see , dashed line). We hypothesise:

Hypothesis 2 (H2): Gender correlates with ICT-SC profile membership, with women being more likely than men to belong to profiles characterised by low general ICT-SC and by strengths in verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains, and men being more likely than women to belong to profiles characterised by high general ICT-SC and by strengths in technical-analytical ICT-SC domains.

4. Method

4.1. Sample and design

The data on which the present study is based were collected in August 2020 from a quota sample that reflected the heterogeneity of the adult population in Germany with regard to age, gender, and educational attainment. The quotas were based on the German Microcensus 2011. The data is publicly available in a data repository (see Schauffel et al. Citation2021a). Data were collected via computer-assisted self-administered interviewing (CASI) in the course of the quality investigation of several measures. The person in charge of the survey (Isabelle Schmidt) ensured that the study meets ethical and legal requirements. Participation was voluntary, anonymous, and informed consent was given. Informed consent was assured by for example explaining the aims, overall purpose, and methods of the study. No vulnerable participants (e.g. children) were surveyed. Research participants were not subjected to harm in any way; no harm in physical or psychological terms can be expected due to the content of the survey. The study follows the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR). A planned missingness three-form design (Graham, Hofer, and Piccinin Citation1994) was used in which all respondents answered items within a common block (X), and each respondent answered only two of the three partial blocks (A, B, and C). Due to the planned missingness design and the placement of the items investigated in the present study in one of the partial blocks, the final research sample comprised N = 369Footnote2,Footnote3 individuals aged between 18 and 69 years (M = 43.11, SD = 14.88), with 48.8% men, and heterogeneous educational attainment levels (low: 35.7%, medium: 31.2%, high: 33.1%).Footnote4

4.2. Measures

ICT-SC: We measured ICT-SC on a general and a domain-specific level using the original German-language version of the 25-item scale ICT-SC25g (Schauffel et al. Citation2021b). Because the ICT-SC25g contains both general and domain-specific items, it is suitable for adequately capturing the multidimensional and hierarchical structure of ICT-SC. Five items measure general ICT-SC (e.g. ‘I am good at using digital systems’; ω = .95), and four items each measure the five domain-specific ICT-SCs communicate (e.g. ‘It is easy for me to spread information through digital systems’; ω = .94), process and store (e.g. ‘I quickly learn how and where digital data, information, and content have to be stored’; ω = .95), generate content (e.g. ‘I can create digital data, information, and content on my own; ω = .94), safe application (e.g. ‘I can protect digital systems through safety measures’; ω = .92), and solve problems (e.g. ‘I can restore the functionality of digital systems in case of problems without the help of others’; ω = .95). Item stems (e.g. ‘I can’, ‘I quickly learn’) are consistent across the five domain-specific ICT-SCs. Each item was rated on a six-point Likert-type scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (6). The full item wording of the ICT-SC25g in German and in its English version (ICT-SC25e) is publicly available (see Schauffel et al. Citation2021b).

Gender: We assessed the hypothesised covariate, gender (0 = male; 1 = female), with a single item.

4.3. Data analysis

The data analyses were conducted in Mplus Version 8.4 (Muthén and Muthén Citation1998–2017). For all models, we used a maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors (MLR), which is robust to non-normality of data (Enders Citation2010).

In a first step, we used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to specify the incomplete bifactor model of ICT-SC (see ). We evaluated model fit using the following cut-off criteria for good model fit proposed by Hu and Bentler (Citation1999): comparative fit index (CFI ≥ .95), root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA ≤ .06), and standardised root-mean-square residual (SRMR ≤ .08). Ensuring comparability of the measure across our main covariate gender, we conducted measurement invariance analysis across gender (male vs. female). Following a step-up approach (Putnick and Bornstein Citation2016), we tested increasing levels of measurement invariance against each other (i.e. configural, metric, scalar, strict), using multigroup CFAs. We accept the higher level of measurement invariance if CFI decreases less than .010 and RMSEA increases less than .015 (Chen Citation2007).

In the next step, we saved the factor scores to run the LPA (Morin et al. Citation2016). The factor scores for the five domain-specific ICT-SCs and general ICT-SC provided the input for the LPA. This procedure represents an appropriate alternative to the use of fully latent mixture models, given the limited sample size (N = 369) and model complexity, as the factor scores preserve the nature of the underlying measurement structure (Gillet et al. Citation2021; Morin et al. Citation2016), here the incomplete bifactor model.

To conduct the LPA, we used the three-step BCH (Bolck-Croon-Hagenaars) approach (Asparouhov and Muthén Citation2014), which is one of the currently recommended methods for LPA with covariates (see Ferguson et al. Citation2020). First, we ran iterative LPA models for solutions in the range from K = 1 to K = 4. In the models, we constrained the variances across classes to equality (Ferguson et al. Citation2020; Tein, Coxe, and Cham Citation2013). To address potential local maxima issues, we followed the recommendation of Spurk et al. (Citation2020) that ‘the best log-likelihood value should be replicated in at least two final-stage solutions’ (p. 10). To identify the optimal number of profiles, we used the following fit performance scores: the Akaike information criterion (AIC), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), sample-size adjusted BIC (SABIC), and entropy. Entropy, a measure of classification uncertainty, can range from 0 to 1, with higher values indicating a higher degree of classification certainty (e.g. Tein, Coxe, and Cham Citation2013). Values > .80 indicate low entropy (Tein, Coxe, and Cham Citation2013). When comparing AIC, BIC, and SABIC values across models, the model with the lower values indicates the better model fit. Further, we used bootstrap likelihood ratio test (BLRT) scores, which provide p values indicating whether the fit of K classes is significantly better than that of K − 1 classes. We chose the BLRT because compared with other tests and indexes, it has been found to be the most consistent indicator for the correct number of classes in a population given the disadvantage of a multiplied computation time (Nylund, Asparouhov, and Muthén Citation2007). As latent profiles that comprise less than 5% of the total number of respondents are likely to be spurious (e.g. Masyn Citation2013), we checked these carefully. To identify the optimal number of profiles, we evaluated not only the meaningfulness of profile solutions in terms of statistical salience but also whether the profile solutions reflected theoretical assumptions (see Ferguson et al. Citation2020).

In the second step, we used the respondents’ profile probabilities to specify their probability of membership of each latent profile. We describe the final profile solution by the profiles’ levels and shapes. We determine profile separation by effect sizes of mean differences between profiles on the LPA indicators (Nylund, Asparouhov, and Muthén Citation2007), following the conventions by Cohen (Citation1988, i.e. small: |d| ≥ 0.20, medium: |d| ≥ 0.50, large: |d| ≥ 0.80). Following Marsh et al. (Citation2009), we examine within-profile differences between ICT-SC in the verbal-interactive (i.e. communicate) and technical-analytical (i.e. solve problems) domains, as an indicator of profile shape.

In the third and final step, we included gender as a predictor variable in the model. In doing so, we fixed the profile memberships according to the previous step and used profile memberships in a multinomial logistic regression as dependent variables. The code for the conducted LPA is available as electronic supplementary material (see ESM 2).

5. Results

Descriptive statistics (i.e. means, standard deviations, range, skewness, and kurtosis) for all variables used in the analysis for the total sample, and separately by gender, are displayed in .

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the ICT self-concept items for the whole sample and separately by gender.

Results of the CFA showed a good model fit of the incomplete bifactor model, χ2 (df) = 521.155 (245), p < .001; CFI = .957; RMSEA = .055; SRMR = .025. Factor correlations between domain-specific ICT-SCs are depicted in . The pattern of correlations showed that the correlations between the ICT-domains communicate (corresponding to the verbal-interactive pole of the ICT-SC continuum) and safe application and solve problems (corresponding to the technical-analytical pole of the ICT-SC continuum) were lowest (r = .405/.406). Results from the multigroup CFA (see ) supported strict measurement invariance (i.e. equal pattern, factor loading, intercepts, residual variances) across gender (male vs. female). Thus, the ICT-SC25 measures ICT-SC comparably across gender.

Table 2. Latent intercorrelations of the ICT self-concept domains.

Table 3. Measurement invariance testing of the ICT-SC25g across gender (male vs. female) based on the NMS model.

shows the results of the LPA. Fit summary of the best log-likelihood solutions was replicated at least twice. Results showed that AIC, BIC, and SABIC values continuously decreased with the increasing number of latent profiles (K). Entropy was lowest (i.e. the entropy value was highest) at K = 4. According to the BLRT test, models with a higher number of profiles fit the data significantly better. However, the model with K = 4, which had the best performance indices, included one latent group that comprised less than 5% of the cases (n = 1, which is not reliable). Therefore, we excluded that model and accepted the K = 3 solution because it showed low entropy (.843) and the three ICT-SC profiles lend themselves very well to theoretical interpretation.

Table 4. Results of the latent profile analysis.

Model results of the final three-class solution are depicted in . Profile I, which we called the ‘shallow users’ (n = 48), is characterised by low ICT-SC, with the expected profile strengths in the verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains. Profile II, the ‘hesitant users’ (n = 149), is characterised by rather low/below average to average ICT-SC across domains. Profile III, the ‘reflective users’ (n = 172), is characterised by high ICT-SC across domains and strengths in the technical-analytical domains. Between- and within-profile differences indicate both level and shape differences between the ICT-SC profiles.

Table 5. Three-class solution: Profile descriptions and model results including within-profile differences and between-profile effect sizes.

While between-profile differences in the general ICT-SC are negligible to small (|d| = 0.126 to |d| = 0.320), profile differences related to the verbal-interactive ICT-SC communicate need to be considered as small to large (|d| = 0.459 to |d| = 1.352). In technical-analytical competence domains, the three profiles show large differences (|d| ≥ 1.659). Varying effect sizes indicate profile level and shape differences.

Within-profile differences between ICT-SCs at the end-poles of the verbal-interactive–technical-analytical continuum further underline the existence of shape differences in ICT-SC profiles. Profile I shows the most pronounced shape with relative strengths in the verbal-interactive ICT-SC and weaknesses in the technical-analytical ICT-SC domains (ΔMSCCO-SCSP = 1.066). Profile III shows slight strengths in technical-analytical ICT-SC (ΔMSCCO-SCSP = −0.270). The shape of Profile II is homogeneous with uniform ICT-SC levels across domains (ΔMSCCO-SCSP = −0.024). Profiles of the final solution LPA model are visualised in .

Figure 2. Profiles of ICT self-concept according to the three-profile model.

Notes: The depicted values are z-scores. Profile I (n = 48) = the ‘shallow users’, with rather low ICT self-concept (ICT-SC) and relative strengths in the verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains; Profile II (n = 149) = the ‘hesitant users’, with rather low/below average to average ICT-SC across domains; Profile III (n = 172) = the ‘reflective users’, with high ICT-SC across domains and strengths in the technical-analytical ICT-SC domains; ICTSCg = general ICT-SC; SCCO = ICT-SC domain communicate; SCPS = ICT-SC domain process and store; SCGE = ICT-SC domain generate content; SCSA = ICT-SC domain safe application; SCSP = ICT-SC domain solve problems.

Figure 2. Profiles of ICT self-concept according to the three-profile model.Notes: The depicted values are z-scores. Profile I (n = 48) = the ‘shallow users’, with rather low ICT self-concept (ICT-SC) and relative strengths in the verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains; Profile II (n = 149) = the ‘hesitant users’, with rather low/below average to average ICT-SC across domains; Profile III (n = 172) = the ‘reflective users’, with high ICT-SC across domains and strengths in the technical-analytical ICT-SC domains; ICTSCg = general ICT-SC; SCCO = ICT-SC domain communicate; SCPS = ICT-SC domain process and store; SCGE = ICT-SC domain generate content; SCSA = ICT-SC domain safe application; SCSP = ICT-SC domain solve problems.

The probability of being classified into a particular profile did not significantly depend on gender. However, the pattern of results is descriptively in line with our hypothesis (H2). Results of the covariate analysis with changing reference groups are shown in . Descriptively, these results show that women were less likely to belong to Profile III (the ‘reflective users’: high ICT-SC across domains and strengths in technical-analytical ICT-SC domains) or Profile II (the ‘hesitant users’: below average to average ICT-SC across domains) than to Profile I (the ‘shallow users’: low ICT-SC with relative strengths in verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains). Further, women were descriptively more likely to belong to Profile I than to Profiles II or III.Footnote5

Table 6. Results of the covariate analysis with gender for the three-profile model.

6. Discussion

In this study, we examined whether different profiles of ICT-SC existed in a heterogeneous population of adults in Germany and whether gender correlated with ICT-SC profile membership. Drawing on findings from academic self-concept research and first evidence from self-concept research in the context of ICT, we hypothesised, first (H1), that distinct latent ICT-SC profiles showing differences in level (i.e. high vs. low ICT-SC) and shape (i.e. verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical strengths) existed in the adult population. We hypothesised, second (H2), that women would be more likely than men to belong to profiles characterised by low general ICT-SC and strengths in verbal-interactive domains, and that men would be more likely than women to belong to profiles characterised by high general ICT-SC and strengths in technical-analytical domains.

The results are partly in line with our hypotheses. As expected (H1), we found distinct profiles of ICT-SC characterised by level and shape differences: Profile I, the ‘shallow users’ (n = 48), characterised by low ICT-SC and strengths in the verbal-interactive domains; Profile II, the ‘hesitant users’ (n = 149), characterised by rather low/below average to average ICT-SC across domains; Profile III, the ‘reflective users’ (n = 172), characterised by high ICT-SC across domains and particular strengths in the technical-analytical domains. Contrary to our expectations (H2), gender did not significantly covary with profile membership.

6.1. Profile solutions

Although our results are partly consistent with those of previous studies investigating ICT-SC profiles (e.g. Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest Citation2021), we found a different number of profiles and slightly different profile configurations. Whereas we identified three ICT-SC profiles, previous studies have proposed four profiles of ICT-SC (e.g. Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021) or academic self-concept (e.g. Franzen et al. Citation2022; Saß and Kampa Citation2019). Similar to the findings of Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest (Citation2021) and Schulze Heuling and Wild (Citation2021), we found that a large proportion of our sample belonged to a profile characterised by rather high ICT-SC (Profile III: the ‘reflective users’, n = 172). However, in contrast to Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest (Citation2021), we did not find a profile with strengths in the domain generate content. Instead, we found a profile characterised by rather low ICT-SC with relative strengths in the expected verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains (i.e. the ‘shallow users’). Further, in contrast to our findings, Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest (Citation2021) did not identify a profile with rather low/below average to average ICT-SC across domains.

The divergence between the present results and those of previous studies might be due mainly to two reasons. First, we included both general ICT-SC and domain-specific ICT-SCs in our analysis, whereas previous studies used only domain-specific ICT-SC measures (e.g. Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021). More specifically, we used factor scores from an incomplete bifactor model (the nested Marsh/Shavelson model, Brunner et al. Citation2010), a special feature of which is that it allows the level (high vs. low general ICT-SC) and shape (verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical ICT-SCs) of an individual’s ICT-SC profile to be clearly separated. This might have contributed to the more pronounced shapes of the profiles identified in our study.

The second possible reason for the divergence between our results and those of previous profile analysis studies of ICT-SC might be that those studies used more homogeneous (i.e. student) samples (e.g. Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest Citation2021), whose members typically have above-average ICT competences across domains – in contrast to the members of the heterogeneous sample used in our study.

Furthermore, Schulze Heuling, Wild, and Vest (Citation2021) found a profile with strengths in the ICT-SC domain generate content, which is plausible because 58% of the members of their study sample were student science teachers, and the generation of digital content (e.g. teaching material) is a typical task for teachers, who frequently use ICT.

Regarding the separation of ICT-SC profiles, our results show that Profile I to Profile III are (rather) close to each other regarding general ICT-SC and the ICT-SC domain communicate while the profiles are clearly separated from each other regarding ICT-SC in process and store, generate content, and especially regarding the technical-analytical ICT-SC domains (i.e. safe application, solve problems). Contentwise, this is an interesting and new insight into the structure and distribution of ICT-SC (profiles) alongside the verbal-interactive–technical-analytical competence continuum in the German adult population, which was the central goal of the present study. Methodwise, it should be taken into account that besides the sample size and the number of LPA indicators also class separation influences LPA performance (Tein, Coxe, and Cham Citation2013). Tein, Coxe, and Cham (Citation2013) argue that ‘[i]ndicators should have a certain degree of distance between latent classes’ (p. 3) to detect the correct number of latent classes with sufficient power and to ‘consider discarding indicators that have small inter-class distances‘ (p. 12). However, such an approach would have been contrary to the main goal of our study, which explicitly focused on the examination of ICT-SC profiles including general ICT-SC and domain-specific ICT-SCs along the entire ICT competence continuum (Carretero, Vuorikari, and Punie Citation2017), ranging from verbal-interactive to technical-analytical ICT-SC domains. Further, Tein, Coxe, and Cham (Citation2013) also stated that ‘[i]n real world situations, the indicators are not likely to be so uniform in nature. Some indicators will be very good at distinguishing between classes and others will not’ (p. 12). As the vast majority of inter-class separation is large, varying class separation should not have interfered with the correct number of class extraction. Methodological articles dealing exclusively with this aspect, however, do not yet exist to the authors’ knowledge as ‘[LPA] is an active area of research and continues to evolve’ (Weller, Bowen, and Faubert Citation2020, p. 287).

In conclusion, the distinct ICT-SC profiles characterised by level and shape differences found in our study support previous findings and extend them to a heterogeneous sample of the adult population in Germany.

6.2. Gender as a covariate of profile membership

Based on previous research on academic self-concept showing that gender correlates with profile membership (see Franzen et al. Citation2022; Marsh et al. Citation2009; Saß and Kampa Citation2019), we expected that gender would covary with ICT-SC profile membership. In particular, we hypothesised that women would be more likely than men to belong to profiles characterised by low general ICT-SC and strengths in verbal-interactive ICT-SC domains and that men would be more likely than women to belong to profiles characterised by high general ICT-SC and strengths in technical-analytical ICT-SC domains.

Contrary to our expectations, gender did not significantly covary with profile membership in our study, although the values pointed descriptively in the assumed direction. Thus, although in most previous variable-centered analyses, females have reported on average lower general ICT-SC (e.g. Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012) and lower domain-specific ICT-SC (e.g. Fraillon et al. Citation2014), profile membership of women and men did not vary significantly in our study.

There are two likely explanations for the latter finding. First, we based our hypothesis on results from studies investigating academic self-concept and the application of assumptions of the DCT to ICT-SC. In particular, we assumed that the verbal–math continuum in the context of academic self-concept would correspond to the verbal-interactive–technical-analytical continuum in the context of ICT-SC. However, our results suggest a less pronounced continuum with domains being more closely related compared with those in the context of academic self-concept. We found larger correlations between ICT-SC domains compared with those found between verbal and mathematical domains in studies on academic self-concept (e.g. Brunner et al. Citation2010; Schmidt et al. Citation2017). For instance, using an incomplete bifactor model in a student sample, Brunner et al. (Citation2010) found smaller and also negative correlations between verbal and mathematical academic self-concept domains. Hence, further investigations are needed to clarify the applicability of DCT to ICT-SC.

Second, the data for the present study were collected in August 2020, almost six months after the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic. Due to the pandemic-related lockdown and a nationwide obligation to work from home (Maurer, Bach, and Oertel Citation2022), social activities (e.g. gatherings, sports, or concerts) and work activities had shifted online. Thus, more ICT functions (corresponding to the five ICT-SC competence domains) had to be used than before the crisis. With this externally driven increase in ICT use, gender-specific interests and gender-specific preferences for specific functions of ICT may have faded into the background, thereby resulting in our finding of no gender differences in profile membership.

Theoretically, the absence of an association between ICT-SC profile membership and gender helps to challenge gender stereotypes in competence perception patterns (i.e. verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical) in ICT use (e.g. Comunello et al. Citation2017; Janneck, Vincent-Höper, and Ehrhardt Citation2012; Sáinz and Eccles Citation2012). Our results suggest that women do not perceive themselves to be generally more competent than men in verbal-interactive ICT domains compared with technical-analytical ICT domains, and vice versa.

At this point, it must be pointed out that the identified profiles and gender-related findings cannot be transferred to other samples without further ado. Especially in less digitised countries (for digital competitiveness ranking see Institute for Management Development Citation2021), gender might play a crucial role in ICT-SC profile membership, displaying a gender-related digital divide (i.e. inequalities in ICT access, use, and outcomes, Scheerder, van Deursen, and van Dijk Citation2017). However, recent results from variable-centered analyses comprising university teachers from different Latin American countries (e.g. Argentina, Peru, Venezuela) show that at least among this highly educated sample no gender difference in teachers’ self-concept related to the use of digital content creation tools was found (e.g. Antón-Sancho et al. Citation2021).

6.3. Practical implications

The empirical evidence of three distinct ICT-SC profiles within the adult population in Germany found in the present study has important implications for decision-makers in politics, adult education, and human resource management.

Previous research has shown that ICT-SC is related to other motivational variables (e.g. Schauffel et al. Citation2021b), therefore high ICT-SC is a desirable developmental goal.

It is, thus, first of all, positive feedback that there was no significant effect of gender on profile membership, however, future research should examine the role of occupational activities (e.g. care work, occupational sector). ICT-SC has an important role in bridging the digital divide, as ICT-related competence self-beliefs are antecedents of ICT acceptance and use motivation (e.g. Rizun and Strzelecki Citation2020; Schauffel and Ellwart Citation2021; Venkatesh Citation2000). Furthermore, it is positive feedback that although there seem to exist different ICT-SC profiles, only approximately 13% of the study participants belonged to Profile I, characterised by rather low ICT-SC. Across profiles, similar ICT-SCs in the general use of ICT and communication with others via ICT speak for the ability of the adult population in Germany to participate in everyday life via digital systems. However, the profiles also differed significantly, especially concerning technical-analytical ICT competences and the generation of digital content. This is where practical interventions should start.

ICT-SC is not static, but rather it changes depending on individual experiences and ICT-related interactions (see Shavelson, Hubner, and Stanton Citation1976). From self-concept research, it is well known that self-concepts can be strengthened by interventions. A meta-analysis conducted by O’Mara et al. (Citation2006) confirmed the effectiveness of self-concept interventions. The identified ICT-SC profiles showing differences in level and shape suggest a modular conception of ICT-SC interventions. Individuals differ in the configurations of their ICT-SC profiles. Thus, to address a broad range of people, ICT-SC interventions should enable individuals to choose single modules, based on their individual needs. Concerning the structure of the intervention modules, our results suggest that differentiation between verbal-interactive and technical-analytical training elements would be helpful. The shape of the profile of individuals with low ICT-SC (i.e. the ‘shallow users’) – who are most likely to require an ICT-SC intervention – is characterised by a fracture between the verbal-interactive and technical-analytical competence domains. Because competence beliefs regarding ICT (e.g. ICT-SC) and performance-based ICT competence are positively correlated (e.g. Gnambs Citation2021; Siddiq et al. Citation2016), the level differences across the three ICT-SC profiles underline the need for ICT-SC interventions starting at different ICT competence levels.

For providers of interventions (e.g. educational institutions), our results offer two practical implications. First, because gender is not a significant determinant of profile membership, the gender-specific design and promotion of ICT-SC interventions are neither advisable nor necessary. Second, to ensure the fit of the intervention to the needs of individuals, advertisements for ICT-SC interventions should use keywords that address profile characteristics (e.g. focus on ICT-mediated communication, build on technical-analytical strengths, basic competence across topics) and directly name the modules and competence domains that will be focused on.

For the communication and justification of ICT-SC intervention needs, the identified profiles are more easily to articulate to human resource managers and decision makers than findings from variable-centered analyses, being cognitively more easily understood (Morin et al. Citation2011).

6.4. Limitations and future research

Our study has some limitations. First, due to the rather small sample size, we did not implement a fully latent modelling approach but used factor scores based on the NMS model as the input of the LPA. Although this approach is frequently used in applied LPA research (e.g. Morin et al. Citation2016), future research using a substantially larger sample size could enhance the methodological approach by modelling the NMS directly in the LPA and thus control for measurement errors more sufficiently.

Second, the present results were discussed against the background of existing literature (e.g. Schulze Heuling and Wild Citation2021). However, no multigroup analyses of profile similarity across samples and also countries were included in the present study.Footnote6 Here, Morin et al. (Citation2016) provide a rigorous guideline on how to compare the similarity/generalizability of profile solutions across samples with six steps (i.e. number of profiles, means, variability, profile size, relations with predictors and outcomes). Nevertheless, the results provide a first step to increasing the attention on ICT-SC profiles alongside a verbal-interactive–technical-analytical competence continuum on which future research can be based.

Third, we did not include a multidimensional ICT competence test in our study, because no such test is available to our knowledge. Thus, we were unable to directly examine the effect of dimensional comparisons on domain-specific ICT-SCs. Usually, the effect of dimensional comparisons is reflected in higher correlations between domain-specific performance indicators of distal domains (i.e. verbal vs. mathematical or verbal-interactive vs. technical-analytical) than those between the self-concepts in the corresponding domains (see Möller and Marsh Citation2013). Future research is needed to investigate the applicability of DCT to ICT-SC.

Fourth, our study investigated only one variable – gender – as a covariate of profile membership. Other interesting covariates are the occupational sector in which individuals are employed or the vocational track to which they belong. One would expect different types of ICT-SC profiles depending on the degree of digitalisation of and the ICT functions required in the respective occupational sectors or vocational tracks. A nuanced understanding of whether and to what extent individuals’ ICT-SC profiles match with the ICT competences that are needed to succeed in a particular sector (i.e. person–environment fit; see Pasca Citation2014) might help student advisors or human resource developers to identify training needs and understand individuals’ career decisions (e.g. job preferences: Ribaud and Saliou Citation2015, virtual leadership: Roy Citation2012), and thus might help to predict individuals’ career paths.

Furthermore, the stability of ICT-SC profiles over time, especially if the ICT-related context changes, remains an open question. Focusing on the work context, it would be beneficial to examine how professional experiences (e.g. digitalisation of work processes, switching to a job in a different sector) impact membership of ICT-SC profiles. Future longitudinal studies applying latent transition analysis are needed here.

7. Conclusion

The present study contributes to the understanding of the structure of ICT-SC in heterogeneous adult populations. We found evidence for different profiles of ICT-SC characterised by shape and level differences. Our finding that profile membership was independent of gender challenges gender stereotypes that women perceive themselves to be more competent than men in verbal domains (e.g. verbal-interactive ICT-SC) than in math-like domains (e.g. technical-analytical ICT-SC), and vice versa. Practically, this finding implies that interventions to promote general and domain-specific ICT-SC do not have to be gender-specific, but rather should focus on individuals’ ICT-SC profiles.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset of the article is available in the GESIS SowiDataNet | datorium repository. https://doi.org/10.7802/2343.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (71.4 KB)

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (38.4 KB)

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (20.6 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

The research received no research funding. The Article Processing Charges (APCs) were funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) [Project No. 491156185].

Notes

1 In existing research, the term ‘math-verbal continuum’ instead of ‘verbal-math continuum’ is predominant (e.g., Arens et al. Citation2020). However, to increase the readability of our article, we use the term verbal-math continuum, in concordance with the verbal-interactive–technical-analytical continuum presented in this article.

2 The final research sample refers to screened data only. Data screening (e.g., Mahalanobis distance, ipsative variance, implausible response time) led to the exclusion of 36 participants with potentially invalid cases (Ninitial = 405). Electronic supplemental material 1 (ESM 1) displays how the final sample conforms to the quotas.

3 Supplementary a posteriori power analysis (i.e. Monte Carlo simulation) supported that the sample size is sufficient to identify multiple classes.

4 Education levels (from low to high) are defined as follows: ohne Bildungsabschluss/Hauptschule [no educational qualification/ lower secondary leaving certificate], mittlerer Schulabschluss [intermediate school leaving certificate], (Fach-)Hochschulreife [higher education entrance qualification].

5 We performed a supplementary analysis with a retested sample (n = 177, age: 18–69 years, M = 44.40, SD = 15.36; educational attainment: 34.5 % low, 33.3% medium, 32.2% high) which was assessed two to three weeks after the initial survey (Mdn = 14 days), to provide some initial form of robustness evidence (see ESM 3). LPA with the retest sample resulted in a descriptively similar three-profile solution as with the study sample concerning the percentage of profile membership as well as the level and shape characteristics. Again, gender did not significantly predict profile membership.

6 In supplementary analysis, initital empirical robustness analysis using a retest sample were included (see footnote 5).

References

  • Antón-Sancho, Á, D. Vergara, V. E. Lamas-Álvarez, and P. Fernández-Arias. 2021. “Digital Content Creation Tools: American University Teachers’ Perception.” Applied Sciences 11 (24), Article 11649. doi:10.3390/app112411649
  • Arens, A. K., F. Helm, F. Wolff, and J. Möller. 2020. “Social and Dimensional Comparisons in the Formation of German Students’ Language Self-Concepts.” The Journal of Experimental Education 88 (2): 245–264. doi:10.1080/00220973.2019.1635069.
  • Arens, A. K., M. Jansen, F. Preckel, I. Schmidt, and M. Brunner. 2021. “The Structure of Academic Self-Concept: A Methodological Review and Empirical Illustration of Central Models.” Review of Educational Research 91 (1): 34–72. doi:10.3102/0034654320972186.
  • Asparouhov, T., and B. Muthén. 2014. “Auxiliary Variables in Mixture Modeling: Three-Step Approaches Using Mplus.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 21 (3): 329–341. doi:10.1080/10705511.2014.915181.
  • Blackburn, H. 2017. “The Status of Women in STEM in Higher Education: A Review of the Literature 2007–2017.” Science & Technology Libraries 36 (3): 235–273. doi:10.1080/0194262X.2017.1371658.
  • Brunner, M., U. Keller, C. Dierendonck, M. Reichert, S. Ugen, A. Fischbach, and R. Martin. 2010. “The Structure of Academic Self-Concepts Revisited: The Nested Marsh/Shavelson Model.” Journal of Educational Psychology 102 (4): 964–981. doi:10.1037/a0019644.
  • Carretero, S., R. Vuorikari, and Y. Punie. 2017. DigComp 2.1: The Digital Competence Framework for Citizens with Eight Proficiency Levels and Examples of use. Luxembourg: European Union. doi:10.2760/38842.
  • Chen, F. F. 2007. “Sensitivity of Goodness of Fit Indexes to Lack of Measurement Invariance.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 14 (3): 464–504. doi:10.1080/10705510701301834.
  • Christoph, G., F. Goldhammer, J. Zylka, and J. Hartig. 2015. “Adolescents’ Computer Performance: The Role of Self-Concept and Motivational Aspects.” Computers & Education 81: 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.004.
  • Cohen, J. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.
  • Comunello, F., M. Fernández Ardèvol, S. Mulargia, and F. Belotti. 2017. “Women, Youth and Everything Else: Age-Based and Gendered Stereotypes in Relation to Digital Technology among Elderly Italian Mobile Phone Users.” Media, Culture & Society 39 (6): 798–815. doi:10.1177/0163443716674363.
  • Duffy, M. 2012. “Tablet Technology for Nurses.” The American Journal of Nursing 112 (9): 59–64. doi:10.1097/01.NAJ.0000418927.60847.44.
  • Eccles, J. S., and A. Wigfield. 2002. “Motivational Beliefs, Values, and Goals.” Annual Review of Psychology 53: 109–132. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153.
  • Enders, C. K., ed. 2010. Applied Missing Data Analysis. Guilford Press.
  • Esnaola, I., A. Sesé, I. Antonio-Agirre, and L. Azpiazu. 2020. “The Development of Multiple Self-Concept Dimensions During Adolescence.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 30 (S1): 100–114. doi:10.1111/jora.12451.
  • Ferguson, S. L., G. Moore, E. W. & Hull, and D. M. 2020. “Finding Latent Groups in Observed Data: A Primer on Latent Profile Analysis in Mplus for Applied Researchers.” International Journal of Behavioral Development 44 (5): 458–468. doi:10.1177/0165025419881721.
  • Ferrari, A. 2013. DIGCOMP: A Framework for Developing and Understanding Digital Competence in Europe. Luxembourg: European Union. doi:10.2788/52966.
  • Fraillon, J., J. Ainley, W. Schulz, T. Friedman, and E. Gebhardt. 2014. Preparing for Life in a Digital Age [IEA International Computer and Information Literacy Study International Report]. Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-14222-7
  • Franzen, P., A. K. Arens, S. Greiff, and C. Niepel. 2022. “Student Profiles of Self-Concept and Interest in Four Domains: A Latent Transition Analysis.” Learning and Individual Differences 95. Article 102139. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2022.102139
  • Gillet, N., A. J. S. Morin, R. Mokounkolo, C. Réveillère, and E. Fouquereau. 2021. “A Person-Centered Perspective on the Factors Associated with the Work Recovery Process.” Anxiety, Stress, and Coping 34 (5): 571–596. doi:10.1080/10615806.2020.1866174.
  • Gnambs, T. 2021. “The Development of Gender Differences in Information and Communication Technology (ICT) Literacy in Middle Adolescence.” Computers in Human Behavior 114. Article 106533. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2020.106533
  • Gómez-Trigueros, I. M., and C. Yáñez de Aldecoa. 2021. “The Digital Gender Gap in Teacher Education: The TPACK Framework for the 21st Century.” European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 11 (4): 1333–1349. doi:10.3390/ejihpe11040097.
  • Graham, J. W., S. M. Hofer, and A. M. Piccinin. 1994. “Analysis with Missing Data in Drug Prevention Research.” In Advances in Data Analysis for Prevention Intervention Research, edited by L. M. Collin and L. A. Seitz, 13–63. Rockville: National Institute on Drug Abuse. https://archives.drugabuse.gov/sites/default/files/monograph142.pdf.
  • Hickendorff, M., P. A. Edelsbrunner, J. McMullen, M. Schneider, and K. Trezise. 2018. “Informative Tools for Characterizing Individual Differences in Learning: Latent Class, Latent Profile, and Latent Transition Analysis.” Learning and Individual Differences 66: 4–15. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.001.
  • Hu, L. T., and P. M. Bentler. 1999. “Cutoff Criteria for fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis: Conventional Criteria Versus new Alternatives.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 6 (1): 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118.
  • Institute for Management Development. 2021. IMD World Digital Competitiveness Ranking 2021. https://www.imd.org/centers/world-competitiveness-center/rankings/world-digital-competitiveness/.
  • Janneck, M., S. Vincent-Höper, and J. Ehrhardt. 2012. “Das computerbezogene Selbstkonzept: Eine gendersensitive Studie [The Computer-Related Self-Concept: A Gender-Sensitive Study].” In Mensch & Computer 2012: interaktiv informiert – allgegenwärtig und allumfassend!?, edited by H. Reiterer and O. Deusson, 243–252. München: Oldenbourg Wissenschaftsverlag.
  • Langheinrich, J., M. Schönfelder, and F. X. Bogner. 2016. “Measuring the Computer-Related Self-Concept.” Journal of Educational Computing Research 54 (3): 352–370. doi:10.1177/0735633115621066.
  • Marsh, H. W. 1986. “Verbal and Math Self-Concepts: An Internal/External Frame of Reference Model.” American Educational Research Journal 23 (1): 129–149. doi:10.3102/00028312023001129.
  • Marsh, H. W., B. M. Byrne, and R. J. Shavelson. 1988. “A Multifaceted Academic Self-Concept: Its Hierarchical Structure and its Relation to Academic Achievement.” Journal of Educational Psychology 80 (3): 366–380. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.366.
  • Marsh, H. W., O. Lüdtke, U. Trautwein, and A. J. S. Morin. 2009. “Classical Latent Profile Analysis of Academic Self-Concept Dimensions: Synergy of Person- and Variable-Centered Approaches to Theoretical Models of Self-Concept.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 16 (2): 191–225. doi:10.1080/10705510902751010.
  • Marsh, H. W., A. J. Martin, A. S. Yeung, and R. G. Craven. 2017. “Competence Self-Perceptions.” In Handbook of Competence and Motivation: Theory and Application, edited by A. J. Elliot, C. S. Dweck, and D. S. Yeager, 85–115. New York: The Guilford Press.
  • Marsh, H. W., P. D. Parker, and R. G. Craven. 2015. “Dimensional Comparison Theory: An Extension of the Internal/External Frame of Reference Model.” In Self-concept, Motivation and Identity: Underpinning Success with Research and Practice, edited by F. Guay, H. W. Marsh, D. M. McInerney, and R. G. Craven, 115–150. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.
  • Masyn, K. E. 2013. “Latent Class Analysis and Finite Mixture Modeling.” In The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods, edited by T. Little, Vol. 2, 551–611. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Maurer, M., N. Bach, and S. Oertel. 2022. “Forced to go Virtual. Working-from-Home Arrangements and Their Effect on Team Communication During COVID-19 Lockdown.” German Journal of Human Resource Management: Zeitschrift Für Personalforschung 36 (3): 238–269. doi:10.1177/23970022221083698.
  • Meeusen, C., B. Meuleman, K. Abts, and R. Bergh. 2018. “Comparing a Variable-Centered and a Person-Centered Approach to the Structure of Prejudice.” Social Psychological and Personality Science 9 (6): 645–655. doi:10.1177/1948550617720273.
  • Mejía-Rodríguez, A. M., H. Luyten, and M. R. M. Meelissen. 2021. “Gender Differences in Mathematics Self-Concept Across the World: An Exploration of Student and Parent Data of TIMSS 2015.” International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education 19 (6): 1229–1250. doi:10.1007/s10763-020-10100-x.
  • Möller, J., and H. W. Marsh. 2013. “Dimensional Comparison Theory.” Psychological Review 120 (3): 544–560. doi:10.1037/a0032459.
  • Morin, A. J. S., J.-S. Boudrias, H. W. Marsh, I. Madore, and P. Desrumaux. 2016. “Further Reflections on Disentangling Shape and Level Effects in Person-Centered Analyses: An Illustration Exploring the Dimensionality of Psychological Health.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 23 (3): 438–454. doi:10.1080/10705511.2015.1116077.
  • Morin, A. J., J. P. Meyer, J. Creusier, and F. Biétry. 2016. “Multiple-group Analysis of Similarity in Latent Profile Solutions.” Organizational Research Methods 19 (2): 231–254. doi:10.1177/1094428115621148.
  • Morin, A. J. S., J. Morizot, J.-S. Boudrias, and I. Madore. 2011. “A Multifoci Person-Centered Perspective on Workplace Affective Commitment: A Latent Profile/Factor Mixture Analysis.” Organizational Research Methods 14 (1): 58–90. doi:10.1177/1094428109356476.
  • Morrissey, K. R., D. Hallett, A. Bakhtiar, and C. L. Fitzpatrick. 2019. “Implicit Math-Gender Stereotype Present in Adults but not in 8th Grade.” Journal of Adolescence 74: 173–182. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.06.003.
  • Muthén, B. O., and L. K. Muthén. 1998–2017. Mplus (Version 8) [Computer software]. Muthén & Muthén. https://www.statmodel.com.
  • Nylund, K. L., T. Asparouhov, and B. O. Muthén. 2007. “Deciding on the Number of Classes in Latent Class Analysis and Growth Mixture Modeling: A Monte Carlo Simulation Study.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 14 (4): 535–569. doi:10.1080/10705510701575396.
  • O’Mara, A. J., H. W. Marsh, R. G. Craven, and R. L. Debus. 2006. “Do Self-Concept Interventions Make a Difference? A Synergistic Blend of Construct Validation and Meta-Analysis.” Educational Psychologist 41 (3): 181–206. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_4.
  • Pasca, R. 2014. “Person-environment fit Theory.” In Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, edited by A. C. Michalos, 4776–4778. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. doi:10.1007/978-94-007-0753-5_2155.
  • Peiffer, H., I. Schmidt, T. Ellwart, and A.-S. Ulfert. 2020. “Digital Competences in the Workplace: Theory, Terminology, and Training.” In Vocational Education and Training in the age of Digitization: Challenges and Opportunities, edited by E. Wuttke, J. Seifried, and H. Niegemann, 157–181. Leverkusen Opladen: Barbara Budrich Publishers. doi:10.2307/j.ctv18dvv1c.11.
  • Putnick, D. L., and M. H. Bornstein. 2016. “Measurement Invariance Conventions and Reporting: The State of the Art and Future Directions for Psychological Research.” Developmental Review 41: 71–90. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2016.06.004.
  • Ribaud, V., and P. Saliou. 2015. “Relating ICT Competencies with Personality Types.” In Systems, Software and Services Process Improvement, edited by R. V. O’Connor, M. Umay Akkaya, K. Kemaneci, M. Yilmaz, A. Poth, R. Messnarz, and R. V. O’Connor, 295–302. Ankara: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-24647-5_24.
  • Rizun, M., and A. Strzelecki. 2020. “Students’acceptance of the COVID-19 Impact on Shifting Higher Education to Distance Learning in Poland.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17 (18). Article 6468. doi:10.3390/ijerph17186468.
  • Robins, R. W., O. P. John, A. Caspi, T. E. Moffitt, and M. Stouthamer-Loeber. 1996. “Resilient, Overcontrolled, and Undercontrolled Boys: Three Replicable Personality Types.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 (1): 157–171. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.1.157.
  • Roy, S. R. 2012. “Digital Mastery.” International Journal of E-Collaboration 8 (3): 56–66. doi:10.4018/jec.2012070104.
  • Rubach, C., and R. Lazarides. 2019. “Eine Skala zur Selbsteinschätzung digitaler Kompetenzen bei Lehramtsstudierenden [A Digital Literacy Self-Assessment Scale for Student Teachers].” Zeitschrift Für Bildungsforschung 9 (3): 345–374. doi:10.1007/S35834-019-00248-0.
  • Sáinz, M., and J. Eccles. 2012. “Self-Concept of Computer and Math Ability: Gender Implications Across Time and Within ICT Studies.” Journal of Vocational Behavior 80 (2): 486–499. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2011.08.005.
  • Saß, S., and N. Kampa. 2019. “Self-Concept Profiles in Lower Secondary Level – an Explanation for Gender Differences in Science Course Selection?” Frontiers in Psychology 10. Article 836. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00836.
  • Schauffel, N., and T. Ellwart. 2021. “Forced Virtuality During COVID-19: A Multigroup Perspective on Technology Acceptance of Public Digital Services.” Zeitschrift für Arbeits- und Organisationspsychologie A&O 65 (4): 244–257. doi:10.1026/0932-4089/a000366.
  • Schauffel, N., I. Schmidt, H. Peiffer, and T. Ellwart. 2021a. Data and Code: Ict-SC25g and ICT-SC25e – Scale Development and Validation [Dataset]. SowiDataNet|datorium. doi:10.7802/2343.
  • Schauffel, N., I. Schmidt, H. Peiffer, and T. Ellwart. 2021b. “Self-concept Related to Information and Communication Technology: Scale Development and Validation.” Computers in Human Behavior Reports 4. Article 100149. doi:10.1016/j.chbr.2021.100149.
  • Scheerder, A., A. van Deursen, and J. van Dijk. 2017. “Determinants of Internet Skills, Uses and Outcomes. A Systematic Review of the Second- and Third-Level Digital Divide.” Telematics and Informatics 34 (8): 1607–1624. doi:10.1016/j.tele.2017.07.007.
  • Schmidt, I., M. Brunner, L. Keller, V. Scherrer, R. Wollschläger, T. G. Baudson, and F. Preckel. 2017. “Profile Formation of Academic Self-Concept in Elementary School Students in Grades 1 to 4.” PloS One 12 (5). Article e0177854. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0177854.
  • Schulze Heuling, L., and S. Wild. 2021. “How Student Characteristics Affect Cooperative Students’ Digital Competences: A Latent Profile Study.” Nordic Journal of Vocational Education and Training 11 (1): 44–70. doi:10.3384/njvet.2242-458X.2111144.
  • Schulze Heuling, L., S. Wild, and A. Vest. 2021. “Digital Competences of Prospective Engineers and Science Teachers: A Latent Profile and Correspondence Analysis.” International Journal of Education in Mathematics, Science and Technology 9 (4): 760–782. doi:10.46328/ijemst.1831.
  • Shamshiri, R. R., C. Weltzien, I. A. Hameed, I. J. Yule, T. E. Grift, S. K. Balasundram, L. Pitonakova, D. Ahmad, and G. Chowdhary. 2018. “Research and Development in Agricultural Robotics: A Perspective of Digital Farming.” International Journal of Agricultural and Biological Engineering 11 (4): 1–14. doi:10.25165/j.ijabe.20181104.4278.
  • Shavelson, R. J., J. J. Hubner, and G. C. Stanton. 1976. “Self-concept: Validation of Construct Interpretations.” Review of Educational Research 46 (3): 407–441. doi:10.3102/00346543046003407.
  • Siddiq, F., O. E. Hatlevik, R. V. Olsen, I. Throndsen, and R. Scherer. 2016. “Taking a Future Perspective by Learning from the Past – a Systematic Review of Assessment Instruments That aim to Measure Primary and Secondary School Students’ ICT Literacy.” Educational Research Review 19: 58–84. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2016.05.002.
  • Spurk, D., A. Hirschi, M. Wang, D. Valero, and S. Kauffeld. 2020. “Latent Profile Analysis: A Review and ‘How To’ Guide of Its Application Within Vocational Behavior Research.” Journal of Vocational Behavior 120. Article 103445. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103445.
  • Tein, J.-Y., S. Coxe, and H. Cham. 2013. “Statistical Power to Detect the Correct Number of Classes in Latent Profile Analysis.” Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal 20 (4): 640–657. doi:10.1080/10705511.2013.824781.
  • Venkatesh, V. 2000. “Determinants of Perceived Ease of Use: Integrating Control, Intrinsic Motivation, and Emotion Into the Technology Acceptance Model.” Information Systems Research 11 (4): 342–365. doi:10.1287/isre.11.4.342.11872.
  • Vuorikari, R., Y. Punie, S. Carretero, and L. van den Brande. 2016. DigComp 2.0: The Digital Competence Framework for Citizens. Update Phase 1: The Conceptual Reference Model. Luxembourg: Publications Office of the European Union. doi:10.2791/11517.
  • WEF (World Economic Forum). 2021. The Global Gender Gap Report. Accessed April 11, 2022. https://www3.weforum.org/docs/WEF_GGGR_2021.pdf.
  • Weller, B. E., N. K. Bowen, and S. J. Faubert. 2020. “Latent Class Analysis: A Guide to Best Practice.” Journal of Black Psychology 46 (4): 287–311. doi:10.1177/0095798420930932.
  • Wolff, F., G. Nagy, J. Retelsdorf, F. Helm, O. Köller, and J. Möller. 2019. “The 2I/E Model: Integrating Temporal Comparisons into the Internal/External Frame of Reference Model.” Journal of Educational Psychology 111 (7): 1131–1161. doi:10.1037/edu0000319.
  • Zylka, J., G. Christoph, U. Kroehne, J. Hartig, and F. Goldhammer. 2015. “Moving Beyond Cognitive Elements of ICT Literacy: First Evidence on the Structure of ICT Engagement.” Computers in Human Behavior 53: 149–160. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.008.