24,399
Views
52
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Sports Performance

Technical and tactical skills related to performance levels in tennis: A systematic review

ORCID Icon, , , &
Pages 108-121 | Accepted 24 May 2018, Published online: 11 Jun 2018

ABSTRACT

The aim of this systematic review is to provide an overview of outcome measures and instruments identified in the literature for examining technical and tactical skills in tennis related to performance levels. Such instruments can be used to identify talent or the specific skill development training needs of particular players. Searches for this review were conducted using the PubMed, Web of Science, and PsycInfo databases. Out of 733 publications identified through these searches, 40 articles were considered relevant and included in this study. They were divided into three categories: (1) technical skills, (2) tactical skills and (3) integrated technical and tactical skills. There was strong evidence that technical skills (ball velocity and to a lesser extent ball accuracy) and tactical skills (decision making, anticipation, tactical knowledge and visual search strategies) differed among players according to their performance levels. However, integrated measurement of these skills is required, because winning a point largely hinges on a tactical decision to perform a particular stroke (i.e., technical execution). Therefore, future research should focus on examining the relationship between these skills and tennis performance and on the development of integrated methods for measuring these skills.

Introduction

Performance in sports results from the interaction of multiple factors (Newell, Citation1986). According to Newell’s constraints-led approach, motor performance is influenced by interactions of the task at hand, the environment and the concerned individual. This approach has been elaborated within the model for talent identification and development in sports (Elferink-Gemser, Visscher, Lemmink, & Mulder, Citation2007). In this model, multidimensional performance characteristics are seen to affect sports performance. Specifically in tennis, performance is multidimensional, as revealed by the integration of anthropometrical, physiological, technical, tactical and psychological characteristics that all influence (future) performance (Elferink-Gemser, Visscher, Lemmink, & Mulder, Citation2004; Kovacs, Citation2007). Anthropometrical characteristics include factors such as height and weight (Sanchez-Muñoz, Sanz, & Zabala, Citation2007), whereas physiological characteristics include speed, agility, strength and endurance (Kovacs, Citation2007). These characteristics are considered to be general ones, because they apply to many sports and not exclusively to tennis (Baker, Cote, & Abernethy, Citation2003). This is also true for psychological skills, such as motivation, attention and arousal regulation, all of which are important performative elements in a variety of sports (Birrer & Morgan, Citation2010). By contrast, technical and tactical skills are more specific to particular sports (Fernandez-Fernandez et al., Citation2011). In tennis, they include factors like ball and racket handling, recognition of on-court tactical situations and appropriate decision making (MacCurdy, Citation2006). Technical skills in tennis are mostly demonstrated through serves and groundstrokes. Two important variables of a serve include ball velocity and the percentage of correct first serves (Knudson, Noffal, Bahamonde, Bauer, & Blackwell, Citation2004). Tactical skills are defined as knowledge about in-game adaptations and decision-making activities on court (Elferink-Gemser, Kannekens, Lyons, Tromp, & Visscher, Citation2010). Compared with other factors, the combination of technical and tactical skills is more likely to differentiate players whose performance levels differ (Vaeyens, Lenoir, Williams, & Philippaerts, Citation2008). This hypothesis is supported by the findings of other studies, suggesting that these skills may be important for identifying talent and for sporting prowess (Meylan, Cronin, Oliver, & Hughes, Citation2010; Strecker, Foster, & Pascoe, Citation2011).

A player’s ball velocity and success rate, combined with ball accuracy, are key determinants of his or her stroke quality (Landlinger, Stöggl, Lindinger, Wagner, & Müller, Citation2012; Strecker et al., Citation2011; Vergauwen, Madou, & Behets, Citation2004). The key role of ball velocity in relation to tennis performance is supported by the findings of Ulbricht, Fernandez-Fernandez, Mendez-Villanueva, and Ferrauti (Citation2016), who measured correlations between players’ physical qualities and tennis performance. Their findings revealed that serve velocity was most strongly correlated with players’ rankings across all age categories, indicating the importance of ball velocity in tennis performance. This is demonstrated by the ability of professional tennis players to direct their strokes both forcefully and accurately to any intended location on the court (Elliott, Reid, & Crespo, Citation2009). An accurate stroke that lacks a high ball velocity benefits the opponent, giving this player more time to prepare. Therefore, the combination of ball velocity and accuracy is essential for almost every successful stroke. Accordingly, this review focused on ball velocity and ball accuracy as outcome measures of technical skills. These technical skills are also required for the execution of appropriate tactics. Thus, the quality of tactical skills may also improve with the development of technical skills (Wang, Liu, & Chen, Citation2013).

Expert players exhibit advanced decision-making skills. This is because the characteristics of knowledge structures that support motor performance gradually change over time, with a progressive increase in the degree of implicit (unconscious) control and a corresponding reduction in the degree of explicit (conscious) control (Masters, Poolton, Maxwell, & Raab, Citation2008). Declarative knowledge or “knowing what to do”, which is consciously accessible, can be distinguished from procedural knowledge that relates to “doing it”, which is implicit. The relationship between the two types of knowledge is such that knowing facilitates doing and vice versa (Williams & Davids, Citation1995). The ability of experts to apply complex visual information is essential for anticipating future events and is widely considered to be one of the core skills associated with motor performance (Abernethy, Gill, Parks, & Packer, Citation2001; Williams, Ward, Knowles, & Smeeton, Citation2002).

Previous studies have demonstrated that both technical and tactical skills are important for reaching the top ranks in tennis (MacCurdy, Citation2006; Strecker et al., Citation2011). The continued interplay of technical and tactical skills assumes critical importance in the winning of every point in a match. Technique plays a functional role in achieving a tactical goal. For example, if the tactical goal is to make the opponent move outside of the court, a short ball cross-court strategy entailing a certain ball velocity is required. Moreover, players’ own positions prompt another technical execution. When a player is playing defensively, high and deep ball hits are useful for gaining time and covering the court more effectively. These examples illustrate how the interplay of technical and tactical skills occurs in practice. However, few studies have examined how these skills relate to performance levels. Moreover, little is known about the effects of specific technical and tactical skills on performance. Additionally, there is a need to explore practical solutions in relation to performance analyses, because few coaches and instructors use tools for assessing technical and tactical skills with the aim of improving performance levels in tennis. Therefore, the purpose of this review is to provide an overview of outcome measures and instruments identified in the literature for examining technical and tactical skills in tennis related to performance levels. Furthermore, recommendations are offered on the analysis of these skills in tennis.

Methods

The PubMed, Web of Science, and PsycInfo databases were used to search for articles that contained the following terms:

  1. Tennis AND (techni* OR accuracy OR velocity OR speed OR precision) AND (serv* OR groundstroke OR forehand OR backhand) AND (performance OR level OR expertise OR elite) NOT table.

  2. Tennis AND (tactic* OR knowledge OR decision OR anticipation OR declarative OR procedural) AND (performance OR level OR expertise OR elite) NOT table.

The following inclusion criteria were used to select articles for this review: English language content, studies focusing on sports-specific skills applied in tennis (i.e., technical and tactical skills), comparative studies of tennis players with different performance levels and original articles. The exclusion criteria applied in the review were studies on participants with health problems, studies focusing solely on kinematics and intervention studies.

Studies on participants with health problems were excluded, because an objective of this review was to acquire knowledge about the technical and tactical skills of players whose health status was not compromised. Studies that focused purely on kinematics were excluded, because this review targeted outcome measures of technical and tactical skills rather than the mechanisms underlying these skills. Last, intervention studies were excluded, because it is difficult to interpret the effect of an intervention.

Articles were initially analysed based on the inclusion criteria. Subsequently, the articles were evaluated based on the exclusion criteria. The steps used in the systematic search resulted in the identification of 40 relevant articles for further analysis ().

Figure 1. Stages adopted in the systematic selection of articles measuring technical and/or tactical skills in tennis players with different levels of performance.

Figure 1. Stages adopted in the systematic selection of articles measuring technical and/or tactical skills in tennis players with different levels of performance.

The quality of the applied methodology in the included articles was assessed using the Critical Review Form – Qualitative Studies (Law et al., Citation1998). This tool can be used to evaluate many types of qualitative studies. This method was applied to assess each article according to the following categories: study purpose, literature background, study design, sample, outcomes, data analysis methods, results, conclusions and implications for future research (see the note below ). These questions were assigned a score of either 1 (meet the criteria) or 0 (do not meet the criteria). The seventh and eighth questions were exceptional, as a NR (not registered) score could also be assigned to articles. A NR score indicated that no information was available on the reliability or validity of the instruments used in this systematic review. For the fifth question, articles reporting on studies with a sample size of at least 21 were assigned a score of 1, because this was the number required to obtain a statistical power of .80 or greater for detecting a large (one-tailed) difference at a 5% level of significance (Onwuegbuzie & Leech, Citation2005). The scores obtained for the 14 questions were summed for each article, with the NR score counted as 0. shows the methodological quality of the reviewed studies. A total score below seven indicated low quality, a total score between seven and ten points indicated that the quality was good and a total score of 11 points or higher indicated high quality (van der Fels et al., Citation2015). Two researchers assessed the methodological quality of the included articles independently of one another. In less than 5% of all cases the researchers disagreed regarding scores. They discussed the disagreements and reached a consensus in all cases.

Table 1. Methodological quality of the reviewed articles.a

Studies that satisfied certain conditions, discussed below, were considered to demonstrate one of four categories of evidence of the relationship between knowledge of technical and tactical skills and performance levels. Consistent results, reported by at least three studies of high methodological quality, were ranked as “strong evidence”. Alternatively, consistent results reported by 66% of more than four studies, with no more than 25% of these studies reporting conflicting results, indicated strong evidence. Consistent findings presented by two out of three studies, or reported by at least two high or good quality studies were interpreted as “weak evidence”. Inconsistent results reported by low or moderate quality studies, or by fewer studies of any quality were indicative of “insufficient evidence”. Last, “no evidence” was considered to be demonstrated when only one study was available.

Articles were divided into three categories: (1) technical skills (n = 9), (2) tactical skills (n = 27) and (3) technical and tactical skills (n = 4). Technical skills comprised the ball velocity, ball accuracy, efficiency, success rates and percentage errors of players. Tactical skills comprised anticipatory and decision-making skills, tactical knowledge and visual search strategies. Studies that assessed technical as well as tactical skills examined at least one aspect of each of the two skill types.

The subjects in the studies included in the review were classified as professionals, advanced players, intermediate players or novices according to their performance levels described in the studies. Players were defined as professionals if they had a position in the ranking lists of the Women’s Tennis Association or of the Association of Tennis Professionals or an International Tennis Number (ITN) of 1. Players were defined as advanced if they had a national ranking or an ITN ranging between 2 and 4. Players were defined as intermediate if they had competitive tennis experience, at least 5 years of tennis experience or an ITN of 5 or 6. Players who were beginners, had no competitive tennis experience or had ITNs ranging between 7 and 10.1 were defined as novices.

Results

shows the study authors; number, sex, age, performance level and tennis experience of subjects; measures of technical and tactical skills; and results reported in the 40 articles included in the review. Nine studies were included in the technical skills category (Girard, Micallef, & Millet, Citation2005, Citation2007; Kolman, Huijgen, Kramer, Elferink-Gemser, & Visscher, Citation2017; Landlinger et al., Citation2012; Lyons, Al Nakeeb, Hankey, & Nevill, Citation2013; Martin, Bideau, Ropars, Delamarche, & Kulpa, Citation2014; Söğüt, Citation2017; Vergauwen et al., Citation2004; Vergauwen, Spaepen, Lefevre, & Hespel, Citation1998). Five of these studies were of high methodological quality and four were of good methodological quality. There was strong evidence that ball velocity produced in serves and/or groundstrokes differentiates professionals from advanced players and advanced players from intermediate players and novices (Girard et al., Citation2005, Citation2007; Kolman et al., Citation2017; Landlinger et al., Citation2012; Martin et al., Citation2014; Söğüt, Citation2017; Vergauwen et al., Citation1998, Citation2004). The findings of Landlinger et al. (Citation2012) and Vergauwen et al. (Citation1998) showed higher ball velocities produced by forehand strokes compared with backhand strokes. However, no statistical tests were performed to confirm these visible differences.

Table 2. Characteristics of the 40 studies reviewed measuring technical and/or tactical skills in players with different levels of performance.

There was weak evidence for greater accuracy of ball placement among advanced players compared with players demonstrating lower performance levels (Girard et al., Citation2005; Kolman et al., Citation2017; Lyons et al., Citation2013; Vergauwen et al., Citation2004). No evidence was found for differences in serve success rates or serve efficiency in relation to performance levels (Girard et al., Citation2005; Martin et al., Citation2014).

Twenty-seven studies belonging to the tactical skills category were included in the review. Seven of these studies were of high methodological quality, eighteen were of good methodological quality and two studies were of low methodological quality. There was strong evidence that advanced players have greater and more elaborate tactical knowledge than players with lower performance levels (García-González, Iglesias, Moreno, Moreno, & Del Villar, Citation2012; McPherson, Citation1999a, Citation2000; McPherson & Kernodle, Citation2007). There was also evidence that superior visual search strategies are deployed by players with higher performance levels compared with those in the intermediate or novice categories. Specifically, high-performing players required less time to predict the directions of serves or groundstrokes (Balser et al., Citation2014; Cañal-Bruland, van Ginneken, van der Meer, & Williams, Citation2011; Goulet, Bard, & Fleury, Citation1989; Jackson & Mogan, Citation2007; Loffing & Hagemann, Citation2014; Loffing, Wilkes, & Hagemann, Citation2011; Mahadas et al., Citation2015; Singer, Cauraugh, Chen, Steinberg, & Frehlich, Citation1996; Tenenbaum, Levy-Kolker, Sade, Liebermann, & Lidor, Citation1996; Williams et al., Citation2002).

The studies provided strong evidence that professionals and advanced players are able to predict final ball locations or the performed stroke types more accurately compared with novices (Balser et al., Citation2014; Farrow, Abernethy, & Jackson, Citation2005; Goulet et al., Citation1989; Huys, Smeeton, Hodges, Beek, & Wiliams, Citation2008; Jackson & Mogan, Citation2007; Loffing & Hagemann, Citation2014; Loffing et al., Citation2011; Rowe, Horswill, Kronvall-Parkinson, Poulter, & McKenna, Citation2009; Shim, Carlton, Chow, & Chae, Citation2005; Shim, Miller, & Lutz, Citation2005; Singer et al., Citation1996; Tenenbaum et al., Citation1996; Tenenbaum, Sar-El, & Bar-Eli, Citation2000). However, there was weak evidence for the differentiation of professionals and advanced players, advanced players and intermediate players and intermediate players and novices based on players’ predictions of final ball locations (Cañal-Bruland et al., Citation2011; Cocks et al., Citation2016; Farrow & Abernethy, Citation2003; Farrow et al., Citation2005; Jackson & Mogan, Citation2007; Murphy et al., Citation2016; Smeeton & Huys, Citation2011; Williams, Huys, Cañal-Bruland, & Hagemann, Citation2009; Williams et al., Citation2002). Six out of the nine studies revealed differences between professionals and advanced players, advanced players and intermediate players or intermediate players and novices (Buckolz, Prapavesis, & Fairs, Citation1988; Cañal-Bruland et al., Citation2011; Cocks et al., Citation2016; Murphy et al., Citation2016; Smeeton & Huys, Citation2011; Williams et al., Citation2009), whereas three studies did not find any differences between players whose performance levels differed (Farrow et al., Citation2005; Jackson & Mogan, Citation2007; Williams et al., Citation2002). One study revealed differences between advanced and intermediate players in their movement-based responses but not in their verbal responses (Farrow & Abernethy, Citation2003).

Four of the studies included in the review examined both technical and tactical skills (Del Villar, González, Iglesias, Moreno, & Cervelló, Citation2007; McPherson, Citation1999b; McPherson & Thomas, Citation1989; Nielsen & McPherson, Citation2001). Two studies were deemed to be of high methodological quality, whereas the other two studies were of good methodological quality. There was strong evidence that advanced players outscored novices in their serve and groundstroke performances (i.e., technical skills) and in the quality of their decision making (Del Villar et al., Citation2007; McPherson, Citation1999a; McPherson & Thomas, Citation1989; Nielsen & McPherson, Citation2001). However, there was weak evidence that advanced players outscored novices in terms of their demonstrated tactical knowledge (McPherson, Citation1999a; McPherson & Thomas, Citation1989).

Discussion

The aim of this review was to provide an overview of outcome measures and instruments identified in the literature for examining technical and tactical skills in tennis in relation to performance levels and to provide recommendations for the analysis of these skills in tennis performance. Studies in which only technical skills were measured revealed that ball velocity distinguished high-performing players from lower-performing players. However, there was weak evidence that advanced players demonstrated greater accuracy in their ball placement compared with their counterparts with less advanced skills. The finding that advanced players outscored players whose performance levels were lower in terms of ball velocity is supported by the results of studies done on other sports, such as handball and volleyball. These studies showed that highly skilled players produced higher ball velocities than their less skilled counterparts (Laffaye, Debanne, & Choukou, Citation2012). Although few studies have assessed the relationship between ball accuracy and performance level, it seems that differences only exist between advanced and intermediate players and between advanced players and novices (Lyons et al., Citation2013; Vergauwen et al., Citation2004). No differences were found between professionals and advanced players (Landlinger et al., Citation2012). However, it is noteworthy that the studies applied different methodologies (relating to size and target areas). Consequently, it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions about the relationship between ball accuracy and performance level. Studies done on other sports have revealed differences in ball accuracy that exist not only between experts and novices (Beilock, Bertenthal, McCoy, & Carr, Citation2004) but also between players whose performance levels are more homogeneous (Huijgen, Elferink-Gemser, Ali, & Visscher, Citation2013). Two studies that assessed technical skills revealed that young players with more experience scored higher than less experienced players for ball velocity and ball accuracy, and their success rates were higher than those of players with less experience (Kolman et al., Citation2017; Vergauwen et al., Citation2004). However, more research is needed to examine the relationship between ball velocity and accuracy, as these factors appear to be important for future performance levels. According to the speed-accuracy trade-off hypothesis, an increase in the execution time of a movement is required to achieve greater accuracy (Fitts, Citation1954). A previous study conducted on soccer revealed that top players demonstrated greater accuracy in their ball control, especially under time pressure, compared with lower ranked players (Huijgen et al., Citation2013). Future studies should focus especially on investigating whether more experienced players are able to maintain accurate strokes under conditions of increasing demands (e.g., speed) in tennis. However, other characteristics such as anthropometry and physiology could affect serve velocity (Kraemer et al., Citation2003; Perry, Wang, Feldman, Ruth, & Signorile, Citation2004).

The studies that assessed only tactical skills showed strong evidence that performance levels were differentiated according to decision-making and anticipatory skills, tactical knowledge and visual search strategies. Advanced players make better decisions than novices, possibly because of their acquisition of a greater degree of implicit (unconscious) control (Masters et al., Citation2008). Implicit processes are organized and occur faster than conscious control processes, because unlike the latter, the application of advanced procedural knowledge does not entail conscious thought (e.g., Masters et al., Citation2008; Raab, Citation2003). Implicit processes are therefore independent of working memory (Baddeley, Citation2003), which explains why experts have sufficient remaining resources to make reasonable decisions. By contrast, explicit processes depend on working memory for the retrieval of consciously accessible (declarative) knowledge (Maxwell, Masters, & Eves, Citation2003). Because novices’ performances are more dependent on explicit processes and working memory, the demands of a complex task are likely to overload them and reduce their performance.

Advanced players were found to be faster and more accurate in their anticipation of the directions of their opponents’ strokes than players whose performance levels were lower. This finding is supported by those of a soccer study, which showed that advanced players’ predictions of the directions of penalty kicks were more accurate than those of novices (Savelsbergh, Williams, Kamp, & Ward, Citation2002). Advanced players use more selective visual search patterns than do novices, as reflected by the higher response accuracy in anticipatory tasks reported in several studies (Balser et al., Citation2014; Buckolz et al., Citation1988; Cocks et al., Citation2016; Farrow & Abernethy, Citation2003; Goulet et al., Citation1989; Jackson & Mogan, Citation2007; Loffing & Hagemann, Citation2014; Loffing et al., Citation2011; Murphy et al., Citation2016; Rowe et al., Citation2009; Shim, Carlton, et al., Citation2005, Shim, Miller, et al., Citation2005; Singer et al., Citation1996; Smeeton & Huys, Citation2011; Tenenbaum et al., Citation1996, Citation2000; Williams et al., Citation2009). Moreover, Farrow and Reid (Citation2012) showed that the anticipatory capability of players is also dependent on age, with older players demonstrating more advanced anticipatory skills than younger players. Anticipatory tasks entail mostly temporal occlusion paradigms, but point-light displays and stick-figure conditions are used as well to examine these abilities. Singer et al. (Citation1996) found that whereas advanced players focused their attention on the wrist and shoulder of the opponent when anticipating the ball’s direction, novices focused more on the opponent’s head and non-dominant side. It seems that advanced players focus on relevant proximal cues (e.g., those associated with the opponent’s trunk, arm and hips), whereas novices focus more on distal cues like the opponent’s head (Goulet et al., Citation1989; Singer et al., Citation1996). However, tennis performance entails multidimensional performance characteristics that include technical and tactical skills among others. This review did not examine other performance characteristics, such as perception, that may also be relevant, particularly in relation to technical and tactical skills. Thus, future studies could explore the importance of perception related to tennis performance.

The instruments identified in this systematic review are important for analysing performance in tennis. Newell’s constraints-led approach, which has been applied in performance analysis (Glazier, Citation2010; Newell, Citation1986), suggests that sports performance hinges on three sources: the task, the environment and the individual. According to Davids, Button, and Bennett (Citation2008), the key role of coaches and instructors is to manipulate these constraints so that they facilitate players’ discovery of functional movement patterns. Constraints can be manipulated in practice through the introduction of instruments. For example, task constraints can be changed by introducing target areas used to measure or improve ball accuracy. However, the instruments identified in this review varied in their practicality.

The interview procedures applied during actual match situations for examining decision-making skills require less experience and materials and could be easily incorporated in practice by coaches and instructors. In addition a radar system and target areas for assessing ball velocity and ball accuracy, respectively, could be easily integrated in practice. However, the use of video-based experiments for measuring anticipatory skills and visual behaviours would be more difficult to incorporate into training, as these require more expertise and resources. Moreover, a point to consider when introducing such instruments to practically monitor and improve anticipatory skills is that implicit learning techniques may be more effective than explicit learning instructions, especially under stressful conditions (Liao & Masters, Citation2001; Williams et al., Citation2002). Players could benefit from instructions that direct their attention towards information-rich areas as opposed to specific information cues. They should be instructed to focus solely on the contact zone so that they can discern regularities between the racket and ball orientation for each type of serve (Williams et al., Citation2002).

For this review, two expert researchers working independently from one another assessed the methodological quality of the studies as accurately as possible using the quality assessment form developed by Law et al. (Citation1998). It is conceivable that scores might have been slightly different if another form had been used. However, a limitation of this review was that sex and age were not considered in comparisons of performance levels. Therefore, it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions regarding which factors are important for players of different ages and sexes. Very little research has been conducted on technical and tactical skills. In addition, few studies were found that assessed these skills longitudinally or focused on young tennis players. Technical and tactical skills should be measured over time in studies of young players to deepen understanding of the development of these skills.

Thirty-six articles in which technical or tactical skills were separately measured were included in the review. These studies provided insights relating to particular skills that differentiate players with different performance levels. One advantage of conducting separate assessments of technical and tactical skills is that this leads to more knowledge about a specific skill related to the level of performance within a more controllable environment. By contrast, a significant disadvantage of measuring technical or tactical skills in isolation is that this measure is not reflective of actual match play, because a tennis stroke is always executed in a particular context and not in isolation. The tactical possibilities depend on players’ technical abilities, given that technical skills both determine and limit players’ tactical solutions and decisions. The reverse is also true, as players’ technical skills determine tactical possibilities. The performance of a particular stroke (i.e., technical execution) that is most likely to result in winning the point is based on a tactical decision, meaning that these skills should be studied in an integrated way. Future studies should explore the relationship between technical and tactical skills and tennis performance. In addition, further studies should focus on developing a test for the integrated measurement of these skills.

Conclusions

The aim of this review was to provide an overview of outcome measures and instruments identified in the literature for examining technical and tactical skills in tennis in relation to performance levels and to provide recommendations for the analysis of these skills in tennis performance. The results of the studies that measured only technical skills revealed that performance levels were differentiated based on ball velocity. Weak evidence was found for more accurate ball placement by advanced players compared with their less skilled counterparts. The studies that assessed only tactical skills showed strong evidence that players with higher performance levels display superior decision-making and anticipatory skills, more elaborate tactical knowledge and better visual search strategies than players whose performance levels are lower. However, a significant disadvantage of the studies was that they mainly measured technical and tactical skills in isolation. This is a drawback because players’ technical skills determine and limit their tactical solutions and decisions (and vice versa); therefore these skills should be studied in an integrated manner. Future studies should explore the relationship between technical and tactical skills and tennis performance. In addition, they should focus on developing a test that enables these skills to be measured in an integrated manner and is also easy to incorporate in practice.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  • Abernethy, B., Gill, D. P., Parks, S. L., & Packer, S. T. (2001). Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception, 30(2), 233–252.
  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839.
  • Baker, J., Cote, J., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Sport-specific practice and the development of expert decision-making in team ball sports. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15(1), 12–25.
  • Balser, N., Lorey, B., Pilgramm, S., Stark, R., Bischoff, M., Zentgraf, K., … Munzert, J. (2014). Prediction of human actions: Expertise and task‐related effects on neural activation of the action observation network. Human Brain Mapping, 35(8), 4016–4034.
  • Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., McCoy, A. M., & Carr, T. H. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(2), 373–379.
  • Birrer, D., & Morgan, G. (2010). Psychological skills training as a way to enhance an athlete’s performance in high‐intensity sports. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 20(s2), 78–87.
  • Buckolz, E., Prapavesis, H., & Fairs, J. (1988). Advance cues and their use in predicting tennis passing shots. Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences, 13(1), 20–30.
  • Cañal-Bruland, R., van Ginneken, W. F., van der Meer, B. R., & Williams, A. M. (2011). The effect of local kinematic changes on anticipation judgments. Human Movement Science, 30(3), 495–503.
  • Cocks, A. J., Jackson, R. C., Bishop, D. T., & Williams, A. M. (2016). Anxiety, anticipation and contextual information: a test of attentional control theory. Cognition and Emotion, 30(6), 1037–1048. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1044424
  • Davids, K., Button, C., & Bennett, S. J. (2008). Coordination and control of movement in sport: an ecological approach. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  • Del Villar, F., González, L. G., Iglesias, D., Moreno, M. P., & Cervelló, E. M. (2007). Expert-novice differences in cognitive and execution skills during tennis competition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 104(2), 355–365.
  • Elferink-Gemser, M., Visscher, C., Lemmink, K., & Mulder, T. (2004). Relation between multidimensional performance characteristics and level of performance in talented youth field hockey players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22(11–12), 1053–1063.
  • Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Kannekens, R., Lyons, J., Tromp, Y., & Visscher, C. (2010). Knowing what to do and doing it: Differences in self-assessed tactical skills of regional, sub-elite, and elite youth field hockey players. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(5), 521–528.
  • Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Visscher, C., Lemmink, K. A., & Mulder, T. (2007). Multidimensional performance characteristics and standard of performance in talented youth field hockey players: A longitudinal study. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(4), 481–489.
  • Elliott, B., Reid, M., & Crespo, M. (2009). Technique development in tennis stroke production. Valencia: ITF Publications.
  • Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception-action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32(9), 1127–1139.
  • Farrow, D., Abernethy, B., & Jackson, R. C. (2005). Probing expert anticipation with the temporal occlusion paradigm: Experimental investigations of some methodological issues. Motor Control, 9(3), 330–349.
  • Farrow, D., & Reid, M. (2012). The contribution of situational probability information to anticipatory skill. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 15(4), 368–373.
  • Fernandez-Fernandez, J., Sanz-Rivas, D., Sanchez-Muñoz, C., de la Aleja Tellez, J. G., Buchheit, M., & Mendez-Villanueva, A. (2011). Physiological responses to on-court vs running interval training in competitive tennis players. Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 10(3), 540–545.
  • Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47(6), 381–391.
  • García-González, L., Iglesias, D., Moreno, A., Moreno, M. P., & Del Villar, F. (2012). Tactical knowledge in tennis: A comparison of two groups with different levels of expertise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 115(2), 567–580.
  • Girard, O., Micallef, J., & Millet, G. P. (2005). Lower-limb activity during the power serve in tennis: Effects of performance level. Medicine in Science in Sports and Exercise, 37(6), 1021–1029.
  • Girard, O., Micallef, J. P., & Millet, G. P. (2007). Influence of restricted knee motion during the flat first serve in tennis. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 21(3), 950–957.
  • Glazier, P. S. (2010). Game, set and match? Substantive issues and future directions in performance analysis. Sports Medicine, 40(8), 625–634.
  • Goulet, C., Bard, C., & Fleury, M. (1989). Expertise differences in preparing to return a tennis serve: A visual information processing approach. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(4), 382–398.
  • Huijgen, B. C., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Ali, A., & Visscher, C. (2013). Soccer skill development in talented players. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 34(8), 720–726.
  • Huys, R., Smeeton, N. J., Hodges, N. J., Beek, P. J., & Wiliams, A. M. (2008). On the dynamic information underlying visual anticipation skill. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(7), 1217–1234.
  • Jackson, R. C., & Mogan, P. (2007). Advance visual information, awareness, and anticipation skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 39(5), 341–351.
  • Knudson, D. V., Noffal, G. J., Bahamonde, R. E., Bauer, J. A., & Blackwell, J. R. (2004). Stretching has no effect on tennis serve performance. The Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 18(3), 654–656.
  • Kolman, N., Huijgen, B., Kramer, T., Elferink-Gemser, M., & Visscher, C. (2017). The Dutch Technical-Tactical Tennis Test (D4T) for talent identification and development: Psychometric characteristics. Journal of Human Kinetics, 55(1), 127–138.
  • Kovacs, M. S. (2007). Tennis physiology. Sports Medicine, 37(3), 189–198.
  • Kraemer, W. J., Häkkinen, K., Triplett-McBride, N. T., Fry, A. C., Koziris, L. P., Ratamess, N. A., … Gordon, S. E. (2003). Physiological changes with periodized resistance training in women tennis players. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 35(1), 157–168.
  • Laffaye, G., Debanne, T., & Choukou, A. M. (2012). Is the ball velocity dependent on expertise? A multidimensional study in handball. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport, 12(3), 629–642.
  • Landlinger, J., Stöggl, T., Lindinger, S., Wagner, H., & Müller, E. (2012). Differences in ball speed and accuracy of tennis groundstrokes between elite and high-performance players. European Journal of Sport Science, 12(4), 301–308.
  • Law, M., Stewart, D., Pollock, N., Letts, L., Bosch, J., & Westmorland, M. (1998). Critical review form – Quantitative studies. Hamilton: McMaster University.
  • Liao, C.-M., & Masters, R. S. (2001). Analogy learning: A means to implicit motor learning. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19(5), 307–319.
  • Loffing, F., & Hagemann, N. (2014). On-court position influences skilled tennis players’ anticipation of shot outcome. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 36(1), 14–26.
  • Loffing, F., Wilkes, T., & Hagemann, N. (2011). Skill level and graphical detail shape perceptual judgments in tennis. Perception, 40(12), 1447–1456.
  • Lyons, M., Al Nakeeb, Y., Hankey, J., & Nevill, A. (2013). The effect of moderate and high-intensity fatigue on groundstroke accuracy in expert and non-expert tennis players. Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, 12(2), 298–308.
  • MacCurdy, D. (2006). Talent identification around the world and recommendations for the chinese tennis association. Retrieved from http://www.tennis.co.kr/UpLoad/Front/Files/TID_China.pdf
  • Mahadas, K., Mohammand, F., Samim, H., Jannapureddy, S., Hung, G. K., & Ciuffreda, K. J. (2015). Timing differences in eye-hand coordination between experienced and inexperienced tennis players. Optometry & Visual Performance, 3(2), 149–158.
  • Martin, C., Bideau, B., Ropars, M., Delamarche, P., & Kulpa, R. (2014). Upper limb joint kinetic analysis during tennis serve: Assessment of competitive level on efficiency and injury risks. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 24(4), 700–707.
  • Masters, R. S. W., Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., & Raab, M. (2008). Implicit motor learning and complex decision making in time-constrained environments. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40(1), 71–79.
  • Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., & Eves, F. F. (2003). The role of working memory in motor learning and performance. Consciousness and Cognition, 12(3), 376–402.
  • McPherson, S. L. (1999a). Expert-novice differences in performance skills and problem representations of youth and adults during tennis competition. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70(3), 233–251.
  • McPherson, S. L. (1999b). Tactical differences in problem representations and solutions in collegiate varsity and beginner female tennis players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70(4), 369–384.
  • McPherson, S. L. (2000). Expert-novice differences in planning strategies during collegiate singles tennis competition. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 22(1), 39–62.
  • McPherson, S. L., & Kernodle, M. (2007). Mapping two new points on the tennis expertise continuum: Tactical skills of adult advanced beginners and entry-level professionals during competition. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25(8), 945–959.
  • McPherson, S. L., & Thomas, J. R. (1989). Relation of knowledge and performance in boys’ tennis: Age and expertise. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48(2), 190–211.
  • Meylan, C., Cronin, J., Oliver, J., & Hughes, M. (2010). Talent identification in soccer: The role of maturity status on physical, physiological and technical characteristics. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 5(4), 571–592.
  • Murphy, C. P., Jackson, R. C., Cooke, K., Roca, A., Benguigui, N., & Williams, A. M. (2016). Contextual information and perceptual-cognitive expertise in a dynamic, temporally-constrained task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22(4), 455–470.
  • Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In M. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341–360). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.
  • Nielsen, T. M., & McPherson, S. L. (2001). Response selection and execution skills of professionals and novices during singles tennis competition. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93(2), 541–555.
  • Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Leech, N. L. (2005). Taking the “Q” out of research: Teaching research methodology courses without the divide between quantitative and qualitative paradigms. Quality and Quantity, 39(3), 267–295.
  • Perry, A. C., Wang, X., Feldman, B. B., Ruth, T., & Signorile, J. (2004). Can laboratory-based tennis profiles predict field tests of tennis performance? The Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research, 18(1), 136–143.
  • Raab, M. (2003). Decision making in sports: Influence of complexity on implicit and explicit learning. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1(4), 406–433.
  • Rowe, R., Horswill, M. S., Kronvall-Parkinson, M., Poulter, D. R., & McKenna, F. P. (2009). The effect of disguise on novice and expert tennis players’ anticipation ability. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21(2), 178–185.
  • Rowe, R. M., & McKenna, F. P. (2001). Skilled anticipation in real-world tasks: Measurement of attentional demands in the domain of tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7(1), 60–67.
  • Sanchez-Muñoz, C., Sanz, D., & Zabala, M. (2007). Anthropometric characteristics, body composition and somatotype of elite junior tennis players. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 41(11), 793–799.
  • Savelsbergh, G. J., Williams, A. M., Kamp, J. V. D., & Ward, P. (2002). Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(3), 279–287.
  • Shim, J., Carlton, L. G., Chow, J. W., & Chae, W. (2005). The use of anticipatory visual cues by highly skilled tennis players. Journal of Motor Behavior, 37(2), 164–175.
  • Shim, J., Miller, G., & Lutz, R. (2005). Visual cues and information used to anticipate tennis ball shot and placement. Journal of Sport Behavior, 28(2), 186–200.
  • Singer, R. N., Cauraugh, J. H., Chen, D., Steinberg, G. M., & Frehlich, S. G. (1996). Visual search, anticipation, and reactive comparisons between highly-skilled and beginning tennis players. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 8(1), 9–26.
  • Smeeton, N., & Huys, R. (2011). Anticipation of tennis-shot direction from whole-body movement: The role of movement amplitude and dynamics. Human Movement Science, 30(5), 957–965.
  • Söğüt, M. (2017). A comparison of serve speed and motor coordination between elite and club level tennis players. Journal of Human Kinetics, 55(1), 171–176.
  • Strecker, E., Foster, E. B., & Pascoe, D. D. (2011). Test-retest reliability for hitting accuracy tennis test. The Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 25(12), 3501–3505.
  • Tenenbaum, G., Levy-Kolker, N., Sade, S., Liebermann, D. G., & Lidor, R. (1996). Anticipation and confidence of decisions related to skilled performance. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 27(3), 293–307.
  • Tenenbaum, G., Sar-El, T., & Bar-Eli, M. (2000). Anticipation of ball location in low and high-skill performers: A developmental perspective. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 1(2), 117–128.
  • Ulbricht, A., Fernandez-Fernandez, J., Mendez-Villanueva, A., & Ferrauti, A. (2016). Impact of fitness characteristics on tennis performance in elite junior tennis players. The Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 30(4), 989–998.
  • Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A. M., & Philippaerts, R. M. (2008). Talent identification and development programmes in sport. Sports Medicine, 38(9), 703–714.
  • Van der Fels, I. M. J., Te Wierike, S. C., Hartman, E., Elferink-Gemser, M. T., Smith, J., & Visscher, C. (2015). The relationship between motor skills and cognitive skills in 4–16 year old typically developing children: A systematic review. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 18(6), 697–703.
  • Vergauwen, L., Madou, B., & Behets, D. (2004). Authentic evaluation of forehand groundstrokes in young low- to intermediate-level tennis players. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 36(12), 2099–2106.
  • Vergauwen, L., Spaepen, A. J., Lefevre, J., & Hespel, P. (1998). Evaluation of stroke performance in tennis. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 30(8), 1281–1288.
  • Wang, M., Liu, Y., & Chen, C. (2013). Techniques and tactics analysis related to personality in table tennis doubles. Retrieved from http://ittf.com/ittf_science/SSCenter/docs/Wang%20M%20Y_Chen%20C%20J-1-revised-OK.pdf
  • Williams, A. M., Huys, R., Cañal-Bruland, R., & Hagemann, N. (2009). The dynamical information underpinning anticipation skill. Human Movement Science, 28(3), 362–370.
  • Williams, A. M., Ward, P., Knowles, J. M., & Smeeton, N. J. (2002). Anticipation skill in a real-world task: Measurement, training, and transfer in tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(4), 259–270.
  • Williams, M., & Davids, K. (1995). Declarative knowledge in sport: A by-product of experience or a characteristic of expertise? Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17(3), 259–275.