79,487
Views
182
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The relationships between school belonging and students’ motivational, social-emotional, behavioural, and academic outcomes in secondary education: a meta-analytic review

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 641-680 | Received 10 Jun 2018, Accepted 30 Apr 2019, Published online: 27 May 2019

ABSTRACT

This meta-analytic review examines the relationships between students’ sense of school belonging and students’ motivational, social-emotional, behavioural, and academic functioning in secondary education. Moreover, it examines to what extent these relationships differ between different student groups (grade level, SES), measurement instruments, and region. The meta-analysis included 82 correlational studies, published in peer-reviewed journals between 2000 and 2018. Results revealed, on average, a small positive correlation with academic achievement, and small to moderate positive correlations with motivational outcomes such as mastery goal orientations; with social-emotional outcomes such as self-concept and self-efficacy; and with behavioural outcomes such as behavioural, cognitive, and agentic engagement. A small negative correlation is observed with absence and dropout rates. Similar results are found across different student groups (grade level, SES). Although the results vary to some extent across measurement instruments and region, generally, the results reveal that school belonging plays an important role in students’ school life.

Introduction

Meeting the psychological needs of adolescents who have become disaffected from school is one of the biggest challenges in education (Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012; Fredricks, Blumenfeld, and Paris Citation2004). In the past decades, increased attention has been given to the importance of fulfilling the need to belong (Maslow Citation1962) in educational contexts. Researchers emphasise the importance of a caring school environment that facilitates a sense of community and a feeling of belongingness among students (Allen et al. Citation2018; Battistich et al. Citation1997; Osterman Citation2000). This feeling of belongingness is often defined in the literature as a sense of school belonging.Footnote1 A widely accepted definition of school belonging is ‘the extent to which students feel personally accepted, respected, included, and supported by others in the school social environment’ (Goodenow Citation1993, 80; also using the label school membership). Multiple studies conducted in the last decade have shown that having a sense of school belonging is positively related to student functioning such as students’ school motivation (e.g. Gonida, Voulala, and Kiosseoglou Citation2009; Walker and Greene Citation2009), their social-emotional functioning such as their self-esteem (e.g. Dotterer and Wehrspann Citation2016), their classroom behaviour (e.g. Kiefer, Alley, and Ellerbrock Citation2015), and their academic achievement (e.g. Anderman Citation2003; Ma Citation2003; Niemiec and Ryan Citation2009; Pittman, Richmond, and Richmond Citation2007; Zimmer-Gembeck et al. Citation2006) but negatively related to school dropout (e.g. Hascher and Hagenauer Citation2010; Ream and Rumberger Citation2008).

The meta-analysis presented here gives estimates of the strength of the suggested relationships between school belonging and various student outcomes across a broad range of studies and educational contexts. In the present meta-analysis, school belonging is considered as independent variable and students’ motivational, social-emotional, behavioural, and academic achievement outcomes are considered as dependent variables. In line with the belongingness hypothesis (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995), having a sense of school belonging is perceived as a prerequisite for overall school functioning.

Meta-analyses provide insight into the strength of presumed relationships among variables by combining the knowledge from previously published papers about a specific topic. This paper builds on previous review studies on school belonging and related student outcomes. For example, Osterman (Citation2000) published a review on students’ need for belonging in the school community by integrating a broad range of previously published papers on belongingness, feelings of acceptance, and the role of school communities. Thereafter, Fredricks et al. (Citation2004) published a comprehensive review on school engagement, including a focus on school belonging. Roorda, Koomen, Spilt, and Oort (Citation2011) published a meta-analysis on the relation between teacher–student relationships and students’ school engagement and achievement. Several years later, the ‘Handbook of research on student engagement’ was published, incorporating multiple contributions on engagement and belonging of scholars from various academic fields (Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012). Following Roorda et al. (Citation2011), the mediating effect of school engagement on the relationship between teacher–student relations and achievement was studied by (Roorda et al. Citation2017). In Allen et al. (Citation2018) published a meta-analysis on the relationships between secondary school students’ sense of school belonging (as dependent variable) and a broad range of student variables such as academic motivation, parental support, and teacher support. Yet, little attention was given to academic achievement in Allen et al., whereas improvement of student achievement is one of the most important goals in education. Additionally, they only included studies originating from English-speaking countries. Another observation is that few review studies have incorporated unfavourable student outcomes in their search terms, such as early school dropout, despite the fact that several studies emphasise the importance of engagement to prevent students from dropping out of school (Hascher and Hagenauer Citation2010; Ream and Rumberger Citation2008).

The present study builds on these previously published reviews by providing a comprehensive meta-analysis on the relationships between school belonging and a wide range of student outcomes, including their academic achievement and unfavourable outcomes such as school dropout. This was done by conducting a meta-analysis of the relevant peer-reviewed studies published between 2000 and 2018. Few correlational studies have been published about school belonging before 2000. Thereafter, the number of studies considering this topic increased quite rapidly, which is why the year 2000 was taken as the starting point for the literature search. To further our understanding of the suggested relationships in secondary education, the presented meta-analysis examines to what extent these relationships differ between different student groups (across grade levels, namely lower/upper grades in secondary education; and across students from different socioeconomic backgrounds), measurement instruments, and region. Students in the lower grades may not (yet) have developed a strong connection to their new school, still being in the early phase of their identity formation process (Flum and Kaplan Citation2012). For them, a sense of school belonging may be more strongly related to their school functioning than for students in the upper grades. Moreover, little is known about differences between regions, particularly because most studies about school belonging have been published in the USA, whereas some relationships among variables may be stronger or weaker in other regions. Few scholars have studied the moderating role of culture (e.g. Western versus Eastern countries) in their meta-analyses. An exception is the study of Lei, Cui, and Chiu (Citation2016). Their results indicated that positive teacher–student relationships might reduce externalising behaviour problems more for Western than for Eastern students, whereas negative teacher–student relationships might increase externalising behaviour problems more for Eastern than for Western students. Following these results, the associations between school belonging and the various student outcomes might be stronger for Western students than for Eastern students.

Some researchers have posed that students with low SES ‘have more to gain or to lose than other students’ (Roorda et al. Citation2011, 497; see also Hamre and Pianta Citation2001), suggesting that interpersonal relationships at school are more important for low SES students than for high SES students. Therefore, we expected stronger associations between school belonging and student outcomes for low SES students. SES is a strong predictor of students’ overall school functioning (e.g. Van Rooijen et al. Citation2017). When the relationships between school belonging and, for example, academic achievement are stronger for low-SES students, this further enhances our understanding of (how to deal with) underperformance of low-SES students. Finally, due to the fact that various measurement instruments have been used to measure students’ sense of school belonging in the past decades, we examined to what extent the associations between school belonging and the student outcomes differed across measurement instruments.

The research questions guiding this meta-analysis were:

  1. To what extent is secondary school students’ school belonging related to students’ motivational, social-emotional, behavioural, and academic outcomes?

  2. To what extent do these relationships differ between different student groups (grade level, SES), different measurement instruments that were used to measure school belonging, and different regions where the included studies were conducted?

Since scholars have used a broad range of related constructs, we will explain the meaning of these constructs in the theoretical framework, clarify similarities and dissimilarities, and explain the theoretical rationale behind the present study. Thereafter, the meta-analysis is presented. Important to note here is that the meta-analysis is correlational in nature. In line with the earlier stated belongingness hypothesis as well as the self-determination theory (Ryan and Deci Citation2009; see theoretical framework for further details), one could argue that having a sense of school belonging is a basic psychological need that, if fulfilled, will result in favourable outcomes (e.g. motivation for school). In that sense, school belonging can be viewed as the predictor of students’ academic, motivational, social-emotional, and behavioural outcomes. As will be explained in the results section, seven broad categories of relevant (i.e. school-related) student outcomes were included in the meta-analysis, namely academic achievement, motivational outcomes, perceived learning environment, behavioural engagement, educational aspirations/attitudes, absence/dropout rates, and self-perceptions.

Theoretical framework

School belonging

The concepts school belonging, school relatedness, school connectedness, school membership, and identification with school are interchangeably used by scholars, with small differences in operationalisation (Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012). As a result, the precise meaning of each construct is not always transparent, although they seem to have the same general meaning. The theoretical basis for all these concepts is the so-called belongingness hypothesis. This hypothesis states that human beings ‘have a pervasive drive to form and maintain at least a minimum quantity of lasting, positive, and significant interpersonal relationships’ (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995, 497). Belonging is defined as ‘an individual’s sense of being accepted, valued, included, and encouraged by others’ (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995), strongly in line with the definition provided by Goodenow (Citation1993; see also Goodenow and Grady, Citation1993), presented in the introduction. In a similar vein, Libbey (Citation2004) defined school connectedness as ‘a student’s relationship to school’ (274). The various labels and different operationalisations are explained and discussed below to clarify the similarities and dissimilarities across the various constructs.

The often-cited work of Finn (Citation1989) has been used to develop measurement frameworks for the school belonging construct, presumably because Finn used a variety of important concepts in his participation-identification model to explain dropout behaviour, including students’ identification with school (e.g. Voelkl Citation1996). Finn and Kasza (Citation2009) have indicated that the identification with school construct is similar to the constructs school bonding, school connectedness, school membership, and school attachment. The participation-identification model posits that only the students who identify with their school develop a perception of school belonging. This perception of belonging then facilitates the students’ engagement and their commitment to schooling. As stated previously, the social context determines whether students identify with the school, since teacher–student relations and support from teachers and peers are the basis of students’ perception of school belonging (see also Allen et al. Citation2018; Roorda et al. Citation2011; Voelkl Citation1996, Citation1997, Citation2012).

The concept of school belonging has been integrated in various broader theoretical models as well. For example, Wehlage et al. (Citation1989) and Smerdon (Citation2002) used a broader definition of school membership, including three interrelated dimensions, namely (a) students’ feeling of belonging (based on Baumeister and Leary Citation1995), (b) students’ commitment to school (regarding the institute as a whole, e.g. valuable for their own future), and (c) students’ commitment to academic work (e.g. whether the investment in it is personally rewarding). These authors state that full membership only occurs when students have all these feelings.

Another theoretical model that has incorporated school belonging to some extent is the model proposed in the self-determination theory (SDT). SDT is a macrotheory of human motivation that explains people’s inherent growth tendencies and innate psychological needs. The theory suggests that when basic psychological needs such as the need for relatedness, the need for autonomy, and the need for competence are met, positive outcomes occur. The need for autonomy refers to the experience of behaviour as volitional and reflectively self-endorsed. The need for competence refers to the experience of behaviour as effectively enacted (Niemiec and Ryan Citation2009, 133). The need to belong is included in this theory under the label ‘need for relatedness’ (Ryan and Deci Citation2009; see also Deci and Ryan, Citation1985; Deci and Ryan Citation2002; Deci et al. Citation1991; Niemiec and Ryan Citation2009), facilitating the process of internalisation. This means that people tend to internalise values and practices from contexts (and people within that context) in which they experience a sense of belonging (Niemiec and Ryan Citation2009). Within the school context, as explained in Korpershoek (Citation2016), following both the belongingness hypothesis as well as SDT, students generally have a pervasive drive (or in SDT an innate need) to form and maintain significant interpersonal relationships (e.g. with their teachers and peers) and a psychological need to create ties to the school as institution. In SDT, individual needs are perceived as mediators between contextual factors and engagement (what is meant here is behavioural engagement, see the next paragraph), suggesting that students will be more engaged when the school or classroom context meets their basic psychological needs. Moreover, the theory posits that intrinsic motivation is sustained when basic psychological needs are met. The need for relatedness is not necessarily a prerequisite for intrinsic motivation, but a ‘needed backdrop’ that makes expression of the innate growth tendency of intrinsic motivation more likely (Deci and Ryan Citation2000, 235).

Notably, in the above-stated literature, it is generally assumed that those who do not have a sense of connection to a group or community will likely experience a variety of ill effects on health, adjustment, and well-being (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995). Several studies have indicated that school bonding (e.g. Maddox and Prinz Citation2003), school relatedness (e.g. Deci and Ryan Citation2000) and school connectedness (e.g. Resnick et al. Citation1997; Shochet et al. Citation2006; see also Lohmeier and Lee Citation2011) are negatively associated with unfavourable characteristics such as anxiety, low self-esteem, depression, substance use, delinquency, and antisocial behaviour. A positive relation with teachers and peers is vital for students’ engagement and achievement in school (Lam et al. Citation2012; Roorda et al. Citation2011) and students are less likely to drop out of high schools where these relationships are positive (Lee and Burkam Citation2003).

School belonging and school engagement

Research on school engagement can be used to place the school belonging construct in a broader theoretical framework. School engagement is a multidimensional construct (Appleton, Christenson, and Furlong Citation2008; Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012; Finn and Zimmer Citation2012; Skinner, Kindermann, and Furrer Citation2009; Wang, Willett, and Eccles Citation2011), that can be defined as the quality of a student’s connection or involvement with the endeavour of schooling and hence with the people, activities, goals, values, and place that compose it (Skinner, Kindermann, and Furrer Citation2009, 494). Engagement is a ‘meta’ construct that represents the interaction between the individual and the environment, that is, not as a trait-like characteristic of individuals. However, numerous different conceptualisations of school (or student) engagement have been used in prior studies. Corno and Mandinach (Citation2004) state that school engagement ‘emerges from the productive exercise of academic work’ (311), but what is meant by the term is not explained. In the PISA 2000 studies, student engagement (in the school context) refers to students’ attitudes towards schooling and their participation in school activities (Willms Citation2003, 8). Somewhat in contrast, some scholars suggest that engagement is a manifestation of motivation (Wigfield et al. Citation2006). The often referred to review study of Fredricks et al. (Citation2004) distinguishes three domains, namely between behavioural, emotional, and cognitive engagement (see also Finn and Zimmer Citation2012; Furlong et al. Citation2003; Lawson and Lawson Citation2013; Wang, Willett, and Eccles Citation2011).

Behavioural engagement (or classroom engagement; Archambault, Pagani, and Fitzpatrick Citation2013; Blatchford, Bassett, and Brown Citation2011) represents students’ active involvement and participation in academic or extracurricular activities. It can be split into three levels of observable engagement (Finn Citation1989; Finn and Rock Citation1997). Level one includes students’ conformity to classroom and school rules, being prepared, and paying attention to the teacher. Level two refers to student initiative, enthusiasm, and spending more time on schoolwork. Level three includes involvement in school-related extracurricular activities. Appleton et al. (Citation2006) distinguish between behavioural engagement (attendance, suspensions, participation in extracurricular activities) and academic engagement (e.g. time on task, homework completion). Cognitive engagement incorporates ‘thoughtfulness and willingness to exert the effort necessary to comprehend complex ideas and master difficult skills’ (60; see also Greene and Miller Citation1996; Walker, Greene, and Mansell Citation2006). The construct is generally measured by indicators such as students’ perceptions and value of learning and the utilisation of self-regulation strategies.

Emotional engagement encompasses positive and negative reactions to teachers, classmates, academics, and school and is presumed to influence willingness to do the required school work. It encompasses students’ relationship with their teachers and peers (Appleton, Christenson, and Furlong Citation2008) and has to do with ‘students’ feelings about school and the degree to which they care about their school’ (Sciarra and Seirup Citation2008, 218), thus with students’ feelings of belongingness (Osterman Citation2000). It is presumed to create ties to an institution, in other words, create a sense of school belonging. Appleton et al. (Citation2006) use the label ‘psychological engagement’ for this component, while Jimerson, Campos, and Greif (Citation2003) utilise the label ‘affective engagement’. Following these definitions, school belonging is, in our view, conceptually similar to emotional engagement. For more clarity, gives an overview of the various construct labels and synonyms for school belonging and presents that, in our view, school belonging is conceptually similar to emotional engagement.

Figure 1. Relationships between the various constructs

Figure 1. Relationships between the various constructs

In the literature, it is widely accepted that emotional, behavioural, and cognitive engagement are interrelated, that there are multiple indicators for each type, and that these indicators partly overlap with constructs such as motivation to learn, self-efficacy, and attitudes towards school (e.g. Appleton et al. Citation2006; Fredricks et al., Citation2004). Fredricks et al. (Citation2004) further stress that engagement can vary in intensity and duration; it can be short term and situation specific or long term and stable. Moreover, they emphasise that engagement in the classroom and engagement in the larger school community are distinct types of engagement. They, however, report that many studies they included in their review have failed to make these distinctions. Conceptualising school belonging as the emotional dimension in the broader school engagement construct further underlines the theoretical distinction between students’ emotional and behavioural functioning in school. The distinction between behavioural and emotional engagement has been used in other publications besides Fredricks et al. (Citation2004) and Appleton et al. (Citation2006). For example, Skinner and Belmont (Citation1993; see also Skinner, Wellborn, & Connell, Citation1990; Skinner et al., Citation2009; Skinner, Marchand, Furrer, & Kindermann, Citation2008) considered students’ behavioural engagement in terms of students’ effort, attention, and persistence during the initiation and execution of learning activities, and students’ emotional engagement in terms of their emotional reactions in the classroom. This includes emotions such as interest versus boredom, happiness versus sadness, anxiety, and anger (see also Roeser, Strobel, and Quihuis Citation2002). Skinner et al. (Citation2009) explain that the quality of students’ participation includes engaged behaviour (e.g. on-task behaviour) as well as engaged emotion (e.g. enthusiasm) and that these components interact.

In addition to the three types of engagement used by Fredricks et al. (Citation2004), other scholars have also studied disengagement, mainly to characterise students who ‘do not feel they belong at school and have withdrawn from school activities in a significant way’ (Willms Citation2003, 8). Finn and Kasza (Citation2009) define disengagement as ‘the failure to develop a sense of school membership, failure to participate actively in class and school activities or failure to become cognitively involved in learning’ (8). Other terms for this are school alienation, disaffection, and disconnectedness. Like the engagement construct, these terms incorporate behavioural indicators (e.g. irregular school and class attendance, no homework completion) and emotional indicators (e.g. a feeling of being disaffected), though they are not always labelled in that fashion in the literature. Some scholars suggest that disaffection and engagement are separate constructs, and not the ends of one continuum (e.g. Skinner et al. Citation2009; Skinner et al. Citation2008). Salmela-Aro et al. (Citation2009) use the term school burnout when they refer to feelings of disengagement, although their definition refers to a broader set of feelings such as exhaustion because of school demands, cynical and detached attitude towards one’s school, and feelings of inadequacy as a student (49). In line with their expectations, Salmela-Aro and Upadyaya (Citation2014) found that school burnout predicted schoolwork engagement negatively.

Following these theoretical notions, it was decided to include a broad range of search terms and synonyms of school belonging and engagement in our database searches, to provide a comprehensive overview of relevant studies. Moreover, it stresses the need to analyse potential differences between the various measurement instruments that were used to measure school belonging (or a similar construct) in the primary studies. In the following section, we will elaborate on the methods we have used.

Method

Literature search and inclusion criteria

This meta-analysis focuses on studies published in English in peer-reviewed journals between January 2000Footnote2 and December 2018. Online database searches included ERIC and PsycINFO, focusing on peer-reviewed journal articles. The following combination of terms was used: (school OR education) AND (engagement OR belonging OR relatedness OR connectedness OR membership OR identification with school OR identity OR commitment OR adjustment OR attachment) AND (educational outcomes OR cognitive OR achievement OR performance OR grades OR motivation OR dropout* OR self-concept OR self-efficacy OR competence OR mastery OR expectanc* for success OR intrinsic value OR valu* of schooling OR aspirations OR expectations OR importance of schooling OR absenteeism OR attendance), focusing on students in secondary education. Additionally, the Handbook of research on student engagement (Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012), the review study of Roorda et al. (Citation2011) and the meta-analysis of Allen et al. (Citation2018) were consulted for relevant additional papers by screening the reference lists.

The selection criteria were:

  1. The study related students’ sense of school belonging to relevant academic, motivational, social-emotional, and/or behavioural student outcomes;

  2. The study was conducted in regular secondary education classrooms (excluding special needs education, studies with more than 25% single-sex schools, physical education, and out-of-school activities) and focuses on regular students (students with emotional and/or behavioural disorders, learning disabilities, and gifted students were excluded). Secondary education includes grades 6/7 to 12, including middle school and high school. When grade 6 students were still in primary school, the sample was not included;

  3. The study used quantitative research methods (excluding qualitative studies, ethnographic studies, case studies, and action research) and the student sample used in the study consisted of at least 30 students in order to calculate the correlation coefficients.

Empirical evidence for a causal relationship between school belonging and the various student outcomes is scarce. Despite the fact that some studies label school belonging as predictor of other variables, they often only studied correlational effects. Hence, it was decided to focus on all types of correlational studies, irrespective of the direction of the effects.

The ERIC and PsycINFO database searches resulted in 7,090 unique records (see ). After initial screening of the titles and abstracts by the first author, 679 papers were included for further inspection (eliminating the off-topic papers and/or papers that already showed in the abstract that they did not meet the inclusion criteria). The additional searches in Christenson et al. (Citation2012) and Roorda et al. (Citation2011) did not result in new potentially relevant papers (those that appeared relevant were already included in the 679 selected papers). The additional searches in Allen et al. (Citation2018) resulted in 19 potentially relevant papers that were not yet included, resulting in an overall number of 698 potentially relevant papers. Two researchers (including the first author) read and judged a first subset of about 17% of the 698 full papers (n = 121), using the three inclusion criteria. The inter-rater agreement was 0.94 (114 out of 121 cases), Cohen’s Kappa being 0.84. When there was a disagreement (n = 7), the authors read the full papers again in more detail and discussed until full consensus was reached. While assessing the papers, it was discovered that in some studies, the dependent and independent variables measured were part of the same measurement instrument. After deliberation, it was decided to not include those results, by adding a fourth inclusion criterion:

  • (4) The dependent and independent variables measured in the study were not part of the same measurement instrument.

Figure 2. Flow chart

Figure 2. Flow chart

When other relevant outcome variables were included in those studies (i.e. the studies that did not meet the fourth criterion), only the outcome variables that were part of the same measurement instrument were excluded. Because full consensus was reached about all papers by adding the fourth criterion, it was decided that the remaining papers (n = 577) would be assessed by the first author only. In 10 cases, the co-authors were consulted to decide about the eligibility.

In total, 82 studies met all inclusion criteria (see flow chart in ). The other 616 papers were excluded due to the following reasons: off-topic papers were eliminated (criterion 1; n = 535). These were studies that did not measure sense of school belonging (or a related construct), studies that measured sense of school belonging as a broader, more general construct (e.g. school engagement without subdimensions), and/or studies that did not relate sense of school belonging to relevant outcome measures (e.g. achievement measures). From the remaining set of papers, 44 were not conducted in regular secondary education classrooms (criterion 2), 17 studies did not use quantitative research methods and/or reported on samples of less than 30 students (criterion 3), in seven studies the dependent and independent variables measured were part of the same measurement instrument (e.g. measuring behavioural, cognitive, and emotional engagement; criterion 4), and from 13 papers only the abstracts were available.

Coding

The selected studies were coded for further study, including the following information: (a) general information (title, country in which the study was conducted), (b) background characteristics of the students (ethnicity, socioeconomic status, age, grade level), (c) which constructs were measured (how they were measured, who provided the ratings, and type of measurement instrument), and (d) statistical information (sample size and correlation coefficients). A summary of the included studies is presented in . The table includes information on the sample size, the labelling of the school belonging construct, the measurement instrument used, and a list of student outcomes (categorised into seven broad categories for ease of interpretation, see footnote of for detailed information).

Table 1. Overview of the studies included

All in all, the meta-analysis included the following numbers of studies per region: USA/Canada (n = 57), Europe (n = 9), Asia (n = 8), Australia (n = 4), South America (n = 2), and multiple countries (n = 2 ; of which one study reported the data for a subgroup of Asian students and a subgroup of USA students separately). From the 82 included studies, 24 were conducted in middle school, 38 in high school, and 20 studies included students from both grade levels. From the studies focused on both grade levels, 8 reported data for middle school and high school separately. From the 51 studies (62%) that reported students’ SES, 16 were considered low SES samples (>40% free or reduced lunch), 33 were considered middle/high SES (<40% free or reduced lunch), and two studies included a subgroup of students with low SES and a subgroup of middle/high SES and reported associations for both groups separately.

Analyses

From each study, the correlation coefficients between school belonging and all relevant student outcomes are included. These correlation coefficients can serve as the effect size index. For the meta-analysis, the correlation coefficients are transformed into Fisher’s Z (ZFisher). The studies are weighted by the accuracy of the effect size they provide, by using the inverse of the variance as a weight. The variance depends on the sample size. Larger sample sizes yield lower variances and subsequently higher weights. For each characteristic, a summary ZFisher value (an average effect size) is calculated. For the purpose of clear presentation, these summary values are converted back to correlations. When a study reported correlations with more than one similar student outcome, we averaged these measures and used this as the study’s effect.

The variance of this averaged study effect was calculated by means of the formula provided by Borenstein et al. (Citation2009; 230, formula 24.6), which is Vȳ = 1mV1+m1r.V is the variance, m is the number of outcomes within a study, r is the correlation between outcome measures of the same type. We estimated this correlation at 0.7. By doing this, the variance of the studies was adjusted in such a way that the variance decreased slightly when multiple outcome measures were reported.

Correlations with school belonging are calculated for seven domains (see ): academic achievement, motivational outcomes, perceived learning environment, behavioural engagement, educational aspirations/attitudes, absence/dropout rates, and self-perceptions. Since different measurement instruments were used to measure school belonging in the primary studies, separate analyses for each measurement instrument are conducted as well. Correlations are reported when a measurement instrument was used in at least three studies. This is also done for regions (Asia, Australia, Europe, South America, USA/Canada, multiple countries). Additionally, group differences are reported to evaluate differences between students in different grade levels and between students from different socioeconomic backgrounds.

For the analyses of the summary effects, the study is used as unit of analysis. For the analyses of group differences related to country, socioeconomic background and grade level, the study was also used as unit of analysis. However, when studies reported data for more than one category, the study was split up. This happened for studies that reported data for two subsamples, but also in the case of grade year when there were multiple measurement moments of the same sample (in the latter case the variances of the split up studies were adjusted so that its total weight in the analysis remained the same). For the analyses of group differences related to the measurement instrument and the specified outcome type, the various measures within a study are used as the unit of analysis, instead of the study itself as the unit. This is because there were several studies in which results were reported for more than one measurement instrument or specific outcome type. In short: the within-study differences compelled us to change the unit of analysis. We adjusted the variances, and thus the weights, by multiplying the variances of each outcome measure with the number of measures of the same outcome type within a study. By doing this, the overall weight of a study with multiple outcome measures remained largely the same, but not fully, as we were unable to correct for the changing within-group variances (T-squared) per analysis. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software of Biostat was used to carry out all statistical analyses (Borenstein et al. Citation2009).

Results

Results for research question 1

A total of 82 studies (including 208,796 students) were included in the meta-analysis. shows the overall results of the average correlations between school belonging and the seven domains.

Table 2. Summary effects for the student characteristics (n = 82 studies)

reveals, on average, small to moderately large associations between school belonging and a broad range of student outcomes. The average correlation coefficients are positive for all student outcomes except for the absence/dropout rates, which was negative. The correlations between school belonging and academic achievement and school belonging and absence/dropout rates were smallFootnote3 (r = .18 and r = −.16, respectively), whereas the correlations with the other motivational, social-emotional, and behavioural outcomes were medium to moderately large (varying between r = .30 and .39). The Q-statistics in show if there is significant heterogeneity among the effect sizes. For all student outcomes (except absence/dropout rates) the Q-statistic is significant, indicating that the variations in effect size reflect real differences and that it is not a random error.

shows the results for specific student outcomes within the seven domains, that is, those student outcomes that were reported at least three times. Across the studies included in the analyses, sense of school belonging was, as expected, positively associated with most of the specific student outcomes, that is, with academic achievement such as school grades; with motivational outcomes such as mastery goal orientations; with social-emotional outcomes such as self-concept and self-efficacy; and with behavioural outcomes such as behavioural, cognitive, and agentic engagement. Some notable results are discussed below.

Table 3. Summary effects for specific student outcomes (n ≥ 3)

Many studies included a measure of school grades (n = 67). School belonging was positively associated with this variable (r = .18). The association with standardised test scores (n = 19) was also in the positive direction (r = .12), though less strong. Additionally, students’ sense of school belonging was positively related to their perceived learning environment, such as the perceived classroom climate (r = .40) and a more mastery-oriented classroom goal structure (r = .44), indicating that students with high levels of school belonging perceived their learning environment as more mastery oriented and generally as a more favourable classroom climate. The correlations between school belonging and some motivational outcomes (performance approach goals, extrinsic motivation, and social motivation) were small and non-significant, in contrast with the positive association between school belonging and mastery approach goals. The correlation with performance approach classroom goal structure was also very small and non-significant, in contrast with the positive association between school belonging and mastery approach classroom goal structure. further reveals negative associations for the dropout and absence data, indicating that students with a stronger sense of school belonging showed lower absence and dropout rates. The differences in effect sizes between the various student outcomes were significant (Q-between = 1353.88; df = 20; p < .001).

Subsequently, we examined whether the findings were influenced by publication bias. This might happen because studies with large sample sizes and studies with high effect sizes are more likely to be published than studies based on small sample sizes or reporting small or non-significant effects. For each outcome type, we created a funnel plot of the relationship between standard error and effect size of the studies and applied Duval and Tweedie’s Trim and Fill method (Borenstein et al. Citation2009) to estimate the degree of bias. According to this method, publication bias is indicated when the effect sizes of the primary studies in the meta-analysis are not distributed evenly around the mean effect in a funnel plot. The Duval and Tweedie’s method explores if the symmetry of the distribution can be optimised by imputing (filling in) trimmed values of the most extreme effect sizes, but with opposite effect direction, and if so, it calculates an adjusted estimate of the effect size based on the observed and imputed studies. We searched for potentially missing studies on the left as well as the right side of the mean and used a random effects model for this.

We found no publication bias for the outcome types focused on motivation, perceived learning environment, educational aspirations and self-perceptions. However, we did find some publication bias for the other outcome types. The estimated adjusted effect sizes for these outcomes are: academic outcomes r = 0.215 (LL = 0.182; UL = 0.247), engagement r = 0.421 (LL = 0.353; UL = 0.485), and absence/dropout r = −0.158 (LL = −0.174; UL = −0.141). The estimated adjusted summary effects for the academic outcomes and engagement are somewhat higher than the observed summary effects. The adjusted summary effect for absence/dropout is practically the same as the observed summary effect. shows the funnel plots with the observed and imputed studies (when applicable) for each outcome type. The vertical line in the middle represents the average effect. The white circles represent the observed studies that were included in the meta-analysis, the black circles represent the imputed (missing) studies. The white diamond at the bottom of each funnel plot shows the summary effect of the observed studies only, the black diamond shows the summary effect after adjustment for publication bias, with the imputed studies included.

Figure 3. Funnel plots

Figure 3. Funnel plots

Results for research question 2

The significant heterogeneity we found for the summary effects of most outcome types indicated that there are real differences between the effects within each category. We examined whether the measurement instrument used to measure the effects, the region in which the study was executed, and the student characteristics (grade level and SES) moderated the summary effects, by executing multiple meta-ANOVA tests. shows the test results for the group differences. A significant effect indicates that the groups differ, for example, that the correlations are higher in one region compared to the others.

Table 4. Test results for the group differences (between measurement instruments, region, grade level, and SES with ntests ≥3 per group)

In summary, shows that the reported correlations differed per measurement instrument for several student outcomes, namely for academic achievement, motivational outcomes, behavioural engagement, and self-perceptions. For the other student outcomes, group differences could not be calculated, because there were no groups (or only one group) with at least three tests that used the same measurement instrument. The more precise results are presented in . The reported correlations differed to some extent per region, but only for perceived learning environment and behavioural engagement. For grade level and for socioeconomic background (SES), no significant between-group differences were found. The precise results are presented in .

Table 5. Correlations per student outcome (ntests ≥3), per measurement instrument

Table 6. Correlations per student outcome (ntests ≥3), per region, grade level, and SES

In and , the correlations per group are presented when it was included in at least three tests. Note that it may be the case that these tests were either conducted within one primary study or in across primary studies. For absence/dropout rates, the number of conducted tests per measurement instrument was all below three and are therefore not reported in these tables.

The results are highly consistent regarding the direction of effects, that is, all measurement instruments revealed positive correlations between school belonging and the student outcomes. The only exceptions were the school belonging measures in PISA and TIMSS for academic achievement and the school bonding measure in Add Health for self-perceptions. The Psychological Sense of School Membership (PSSM) questionnaire was most frequently used in the primary studies to assess students’ sense of school belonging and has been related to a wide range of student outcomes. Also here, we see a highly consistent positive trend across all student outcomes. Other measurement instruments were used far less frequently, but also showed positive associations.

shows the results for region, grade level, and SES.

The correlation between school belonging and academic achievement appears somewhat higher in Asian countries than in the USA/Canada, and non-significant in Europe (note that the differences between regions were not significant for academic achievement, see ). Moreover, the correlation between school belonging and perceived learning environment is somewhat higher in Australia than in the USA/Canada, and the correlation between school belonging and behavioural engagement is higher in Asian countries than in the USA/Canada. However, the samples of Asian, European, and Australian studies are small, so these results need to be interpreted with caution. In line with the test results presented earlier, the associations among the variables were generally similar across grade levels (e.g. middle school versus high school samples). The differences in correlations between school belonging and the student outcomes across SES groups were also small. Comparing the low SES group to the middle/high SES group reveals that the correlation between school belonging and educational aspirations/attitudes is somewhat lower among the low SES group, but both indicated a positive association between these variables.

Discussion

Discussion of the findings

The need to belong (Maslow Citation1962) has shown its relevance in the secondary school context. The meta-analytic results show that students who ‘feel personally accepted, respected, included, and supported by others in the school social environment’ (Goodenow Citation1993, 80) are likely to perform better in school (e.g. academic achievement) and show more favourable motivational (e.g. mastery goal orientations), social-emotional (e.g. self-concept and self-efficacy), and behavioural outcomes (e.g. behavioural, cognitive, and agentic engagement). The importance of maintaining ‘at least a minimum quantity of lasting, positive, and significant interpersonal relationships’ (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995, 497) in school is visibly expressed in our results by the pattern of positive associations across a broad range of student outcomes. Although the associations are small to moderate in size, the central role sense of school belonging appears to play in school settings is striking.

One of the notable findings was that the results distinctly showed that students who perceived their classroom as mastery goal oriented and were positive about the overall classroom climate, also felt more strongly related to school. The effectiveness of interventions with a dual focus on, for example, strengthening students’ sense of school belonging and triggering them to strive for mastery goals, needs to be further explored. The importance of mastery goal oriented classrooms (instead of performance goal oriented classrooms) was also presented by Rolland (Citation2012) in her meta-analysis on classroom goal structures (see also Fokkens-Bruinsma et al. Citation2018).

Furthermore, the positive and moderately strong relationships between school belonging and self-concept and self-efficacy highlight the importance of school belonging for students’ social-emotional functioning in school. The feeling of being supported and encouraged by significant others (Baumeister and Leary Citation1995; Goodenow Citation1993), such as teachers and peers, seems to stimulate students’ self-esteem (or vice versa, see discussion later on). In a similar fashion, when students’ need to belong is met, they are more behaviourally engaged, in line with the belongingness hypothesis as well as SDT.

Our results further revealed a small positive association between school belonging and achievement (for school grades; and to a lesser extent for standardised test scores). That is, students’ with a higher sense of school belonging also showed more favourable school grades, which was confirmed across no less than 67 tests reported in primary studies. This is an important outcome for teachers. A plausible explanation for the slightly stronger relationship for school grades compared to standardised achievement tests could be that school grades represent more than achievement alone, as they are often more subjective assessments of student functioning. School grades may, to some extent, follow from teachers’ perception of students’ motivation and/or behaviour, in addition to an assessment of students’ actual performance. It is important to note, however, that a reciprocal relationship between school belonging and academic achievement is probable, due to the deteriorating effect of underperformance on students (e.g. Willms Citation2003).

Similarly, school belonging was negatively associated with absence and dropout rates, confirming that those who do not feel attached to their school are more likely to skip classes or even become early school-leavers (see Hascher and Hagenauer Citation2010; Lee and Burkam Citation2003; Ream and Rumberger Citation2008). Developing a stronger sense of school belonging may prevent students from dropping out of school, as was already suggested by Finn (Citation1989) in his participation-identification model. The perception of belonging facilitates the students’ engagement and their commitment to schooling. Woolley and Bowen (Citation2007) stress the importance of significant others (parents, people from school) to stimulate feelings of school belonging among adolescents.

The presented results further revealed that the associations between the variables differed to some extent per region, but that the differences across the region were small. Therefore, we did not find support for the idea that the relationships between school belonging and the various student outcomes were systematically stronger in Western studies compared to studies conducted in, for example, Asian studies (see Liu Citation2016). As stated earlier, the samples of studies conducted outside the USA/Canada are relatively small. More research in these areas is needed to further understand the potential moderating role of country and culture on the relationship between school belonging and student outcomes.

Grade level (middle versus high school) did not moderate the reported relationships. For both grade levels, having a sense of school belonging was positively (and equally strong) related to the student outcomes. Not only in the early phase of the identity formation process (Flum and Kaplan Citation2012) do students need to feel connected to their school, our findings show this is important later in students’ educational career as well. For socioeconomic background (SES), no significant between-group differences were found, although stronger associations were expected among low SES students (Hamre and Pianta Citation2001; Roorda et al. Citation2011). The only exception was the somewhat lower correlation between school belonging and educational aspirations/attitudes among low SES students.

These are relevant findings for educational practice, highlighting the importance of stimulating a sense of school belonging among all grade levels and among both advantaged and disadvantaged (e.g. low SES) student groups. An overview of ‘curricular and pedagogical ideas educators might successfully use to better engage students in learning’ can be found in Taylor and Parsons (Citation2011, 1). Few classroom intervention studies, however, explicitly include (learning) activities that improve teacher–student relationships (Korpershoek et al. Citation2016) and/or students’ sense of school belonging. This is an aspect that researchers could consider including in further research into enhancing school belonging.

Finally, the direction of correlations was the same across measurement instruments, yet the strength of the correlations differed per type of instrument used to measure school belonging. This not only stresses the importance of testing the construct validity of measurement instruments in scientific research, but also underscores the relevance of conducting separate analyses for each measurement instrument used in the primary studies. Different measurement instruments yield slightly different results, despite overlap in theoretical framing of the construct. This aspect is, to our knowledge, often overlooked in social sciences. Notably, despite considerable overlap in construct definitions of the various synonyms for school belonging (and, in our view, also for emotional engagement), the differences in construct operationalisation in the questionnaires and the presented findings in this paper, stress the importance of analysing potential differences between measurement instruments in meta-analytic studies.

Limitations and directions for future research

Our study underlines the relevance of adolescents’ sense of school belonging throughout secondary education. Readers should, nevertheless, bear in mind that our findings are based on a relatively small number of primary studies for some of the specific student outcomes, and relatively few primary studies conducted in, for example, Europe or Asia. The more stringent the inclusion criteria, the smaller the number of studies meeting these criteria. However, stringent criteria also result in a more homogeneous sample of studies and inclusion of studies of higher quality. The included studies used similar definitions of school belonging, which made the findings of these studies better comparable and thus suitable for calculating average effect sizes in a meta-analysis.

The primary studies used various instruments to measure school belonging. Although results from highly unreliable instruments were excluded, the construct validity of the various instruments was often unclear. A recent study of Abubakar et al. (Citation2016) examined the validity of the frequently used PSSM (Goodenow Citation1993) instrument. Their analyses revealed that a valid one-factorial model for school belonging could be found when items were combined per target (i.e. school, student, teacher, other people, and self). This result still needs to be confirmed in other student samples. Unfortunately, most primary studies did not present sufficient information on the construct validity (e.g. on the factor structure and dimensionality) of the instrument they had used. For enabling methodologically sound meta-analyses, we would like to stress the importance of providing this information in each primary study.

Another issue is that we found indicators of publication bias for two of our outcome categories. The estimated adjusted summary effects for the academic outcomes (r = .22 instead of .18) and engagement (r = .42 instead of .36) were somewhat higher than the observed summary effects.

Finally, the selected studies used cross-sectional designs; therefore, conclusions about causality cannot be drawn. The relationships between school belonging and the various student outcomes are likely to be reciprocal and cumulative. The ‘reverse’ direction of effects, for example, academic achievement predicting school belonging, or reciprocal effects (e.g. Skinner and Belmont Citation1993), are rarely considered (Juvonen Citation2006). This is remarkable because low academic performance is one of the risk factors for school disaffection (Willms Citation2003). Those who already dropped out, are likely to feel disconnected to their school and may not have been included in the cross-sectional studies. Hence, it is important to further investigate cause and effect in more detail, using longitudinal designs and well-targeted intervention studies. By doing so, empirical evidence may reveal which variables have the highest potential for improved student functioning in secondary education.

School engagement (and the narrower construct of school belonging) is a potentially malleable target for intervention (Lazowski and Hulleman Citation2016; see also Christenson et al. Citation2001; Lawson and Lawson Citation2013; Maddox and Prinz Citation2003). The insights from the meta-analyses presented here can be used to design relevant intervention studies incorporating the most relevant school belonging measures presented in this paper. Particularly the PSSM questionnaire (Goodenow Citation1993) yields promising results across all student characteristics (e.g. Van Houtte and Van Maele Citation2012; Walker and Greene Citation2009). Using a longitudinal design, evaluating the effect of interventions aiming to enhance students’ sense of school belonging and overall school functioning would further increase our understanding of the variability and stability of students’ sense of school belonging (e.g. Hughes, Im, and Allee Citation2015). Furthermore, longitudinal studies (e.g. using cross-lagged models) are necessary to investigate the reciprocity of the relationships between school belonging and, for example, their academic achievement.

Evidence-based classroom interventions (e.g. based on randomised control trials) that clearly enhance students’ sense of school belonging are yet to be developed. Future studies that pinpoint how sense of school belonging can be enhanced will further develop our thoughts on how to progress students’ learning processes and their academic achievement. Our meta-analysis provides a strong basis for this by exemplifying that a sense of school belonging plays an important role in students’ school life.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

H. Korpershoek

H. Korpershoek is an associate professor at Groningen Education/Research of the University of Groningen, Grote Rozenstraat 3, 9712 TG Groningen, the Netherlands; e-mail: [email protected]. Her research focuses on motivation and school belonging of students in primary and secondary education, choice behavior of adolescents, and classroom management of teachers in primary education.

E. T. Canrinus

E. T. Canrinus is an associate professor at the Department of Education, University of Agder, Postbox 422, 4604 Kristiansand S., Norway; email: [email protected]. Her research focuses on the improvement of teacher education, teachers’ professional development, and their professional identity. She is, furthermore, interested in teachers’ social networks, classroom behaviour, and teachers’ and students’ motivation.

M. Fokkens-Bruinsma

M. Fokkens-Bruinsma is an assistant professor at the department of Teacher Education, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS, Groningen; email: [email protected]. Her work focuses on educating preservice teachers on doing research. She was the project coordinator for a university-wide evaluation of PhD trajectories (2009-2011), and is co-project leader in a project for assessing PhD trajectories (Succesvol Promoveren, 2016-until now). Her research interest lies in engagement, wellbeing, and, motivation and learning strategies of students in higher education. is an assistant professor at the department of Teacher Education, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS, Groningen; email: [email protected]. Her work focuses on educating preservice teachers on doing research. She was the project coordinator for a university-wide evaluation of PhD trajectories (2009-2011), and is co-project leader in a project for assessing PhD trajectories (Succesvol Promoveren, 2016-until now). Her research interest lies in engagement, wellbeing, and, motivation and learning strategies of students in higher education.

H. de Boer

H. de Boer is a post-doc researcher at GION Education/Research of the University of Groningen, Grote Rozenstraat 3, 9712 TG Groningen, the Netherlands; e-mail: [email protected]. Her research interests include expectations and aspirations of teachers and parents and their influence on student academic performance, the effects of the attributes related to the implementation and evaluation of educational intervention studies, and metacognitive strategy instruction. Her methodological expertise is in meta-analysis and multilevel-analysis.

Notes

1. The concepts of school belonging, school relatedness, school connectedness, school membership, and identification with school can be considered synonyms for the same underlying construct (Christenson, Reschly, and Wylie Citation2012).

2. Because we did not find any relevant studies that met the inclusion criteria in the years 2000–2002, we decided not to look further back for relevant studies. The first study included in our meta-analyses was published in 2003.

3. Correlations below .10 are considered as small, between .10 and .25 as small to medium, around .25 as medium, between .25 and .40 as medium to large, and above .40 as large (Lipsey and Wilson Citation2001, 147).

References

  • Abubakar, A., F. J. R. Van de Vijver, I. Alonso-Arbiol, A. O. Suryani, W. S. Pandia, P. Handani, … M. Murugumi. 2016. “Assessing Sense of School Belonging across Cultural Contexts Using the PSSM: Measurement and Functional Invariance.” Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 34: 380–388. doi:10.1177/0734282915607161.
  • Adelabu, D. H. 2007. “Time Perspective and School Membership as Correlates to Academic Achievement among African American Adolescents.” Adolescence42: 525–538.
  • Aerts, S., M. Van Houtte, A. Dewaele, N. Cox, and J. Vincke. 2012. “School Motivation in Secondary Schools: A Survey of LGB and Heterosexual Students in Flanders.” Journal of Homosexuality 59: 90–113. doi:10.1080/00918369.2012.638548.
  • Ahmavaara, A., and D. M. Houston. 2007. “The Effects of Selective Schooling and Self-Concept on Adolescents’ Academic Aspiration: An Examination of Dweck’s Self-Theory.” British Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 613–632. doi:10.1348/000709906x120132.
  • Akiba, M. 2010. “What Predicts Fear of School Violence among U.S. Adolescents?” Teachers College Record112: 68–102.
  • Allen, K., M. L. Kern, D. Vella-Brodrick, J. Hattie, and L. Waters. 2018. “What Schools Need to Know about Fostering School Belonging: A Meta-Analysis.” Educational Psychology Review 30: 1–34. doi:10.1007/s10648-016-9389-8.
  • Anderman, L. H. 2003. “Academic and Social Perceptions as Predictors of Change in Middle School Students’ Sense of School Belonging.” The Journal of Experimental Education 72: 5–22. doi:10.1080/00220970309600877.
  • Appleton, J., S. L. Christenson, D. Kim, and A. Reschly. 2006. “Measuring Cognitive and Psychological Engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument.” Journal of School Psychology 4: 427–445. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2006.04.002.
  • Appleton, J. J., S. L. Christenson, and M. J. Furlong. 2008. “Student Engagement with School: Critical Conceptual and Methodological Issues of the Construct.” Psychology in the Schools 45: 369–386. doi:10.1002/pits.20303.
  • Archambault, I., L. S. Pagani, and C. Fitzpatrick. 2013. “Transactional Associations between Classroom Engagement and Relations with Teachers from First through Fourth Grade.” Learning and Instruction 23: 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.09.003.
  • Battistich, V., D. Solomon, M. Watson, and E. Schaps. 1997. “Caring School Communities.” Educational Psychologist 32: 137–151. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3203_1.
  • Baumeister, R. F., and M. R. Leary. 1995. “The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation.” Psychological Bulletin 117: 497–529. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.117.3.497.
  • Bear, G. G., B. Holst, C. Lisboa, D. Chen, C. Yang, and F. F. Chen. 2015. “A Brazilian Portuguese Survey of School Climate: Evidence of Validity and Reliability.” International Journal of School & Educational Psychology 4: 165–178. doi:10.1080/21683603.2015.1094430.
  • Bear, G. G., C. Yang, L. Mantz, E. Pasipanodya, S. Hearn, and D. Boyer 2014. “Technical Manual for Delaware School Survey: Scales of School Climate, Bullying Victimization, Student Engagement, and Positive, Punitive, and Social Emotional Learning Techniques.” Delaware Positive Behavior Supports Project, Delaware Department of Education & Center for Disabilities Studies, University of Delaware. http://wordpress.oet.udel.edu/pbs/wp-content/uploads/2011/12/Technical-Manual-DE-School-Survey-Final-12.8.14.pdf
  • Benner, A. D., S. Graham, and R. S. Mistry. 2008. “Discerning Direct and Mediated Effects of Ecological Structures and Processes on Adolescents’ Educational Outcomes.” Developmental Psychology 44: 840–854. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.840.
  • Bernardo, A. B. I., F. A. Ganotice Jr., and R. B. King. 2015. “Motivation Gap and Achievement Gap between Public and Private High Schools in the Philippines.” The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher 24: 657–667. doi:10.1007/s40299-014-0213-2.
  • Blatchford, P., P. Bassett, and P. Brown. 2011. “Examining the Effect of Class Size on Classroom Engagement and Teacher-Pupil Interaction: Differences in Relation to Pupil Prior Attainment and Primary Vs. Secondary Schools.” Learning and Instruction 21: 715–730. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.04.001.
  • Bonell, C., N. Shackleton, F. J. Fletcher, E. Allen, A. Mathiot, W. Markham, and R. Viner. 2017. “Student- and School-Level Belonging and Commitment and Student Smoking, Drinking and Misbehaviour.” Health Education Journal 76: 206–220. doi:10.1177/0017896916657843.
  • Booth, M. Z., and J. M. Gerard. 2012. “Adolescents’ Stage-Environment Fit in Middle and High School: The Relationship between Students’ Perceptions of Their Schools and Themselves.” Youth & Society 46: 735–755. doi:10.1177/0044118x12451276.
  • Borenstein, M., L. V. Hedges, J. P. T. Higgings, and H. R. Rothstein. 2009. Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
  • Boston, C., and S. R. Warren. 2017. “The Effects of Belonging and Racial Identity on Urban African American High School Students’ Achievement.” Journal of Urban Learning, Teaching, and Research 13: 26–33.
  • Bryan, J., C. Moore-Thomas, S. Gaenzle, J. Kim, C.-H. Lin, and G. Na. 2012. “The Effects of School Bonding on High School Seniors’ Academic Achievement.” Journal of Counseling & Development 90: 467–480. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.2012.00058.x.
  • Cavendish, W. 2013. “Student Perceptions of School Efforts to Facilitate Student Involvement, School Commitment, Self-Determination, and High School Graduation.” Social Psychology of Education 16: 257–275. doi:10.1007/s11218-013-9212-z.
  • Christenson, S. L., A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie, Eds.. 2012. Handbook of Research on Student Engagement. New York, NY/ Dordrecht,the Netherlands/ Heidelberg,Germany/ London, UK: Springer.
  • Christenson, S. L., M. F. Sinclair, C. A. Lehr, and Y. Godber. 2001. “Promoting Successful School Completion: Critical Conceptual and Methodological Guidelines.” School Psychology Quarterly 16: 468–484. doi:10.1521/scpq.16.4.468.19898.
  • Chun, H., and G. Dickson. 2011. “A Psychoecological Model of Academic Performance among Hispanic Adolescents.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 40: 1581–1594. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9640-z.
  • Cleary, T. J., and A. Kitsantas. 2017. “Motivation and Self-Regulated Learning Influences on Middle School Mathematics Achievement.” School Psychology Review 46: 88–107. doi:10.17105/spr46-1.88-107.
  • Corno, L., and E. B. Mandinach. 2004. “What We Have Learned about Student Engagement in the past Twenty Years.” In Big Theories Revisited, edited by D. M. McInerney and S. Van Etten, 299–328. Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
  • Cueto, S., G. Guerrero, C. Sugimaru, and A. M. Zevallos. 2010. “Sense of Belonging and Transition to High Schools in Peru.” International Journal of Educational Development 30: 277–287. doi:10.1016/j.ijedudev.2009.02.002.
  • Datu, J. A. D., M. Yuen, and G. Chen. 2018. “The Triarchic Model of Grit Is Linked to Academic Success and Well-Being among Filipino High School Students.” School Psychology Quarterly 33: 428–438. doi:10.1037/spq0000234.
  • Deci, E. L., R. J. Vallerland, L. G. Pelletier, and R. M. Ryan. 1991. “Motivation and Education: The Self-Determination Perspective.” Educational Psychologist 26: 325–346. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2603&4_6.
  • Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan. 1985. Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
  • Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan. 2000. “The “What” and “Why” of Goal Pursuits: Human Needs and the Self-Determination of Behavior.” Psychological Inquiry 11: 227–269. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1104_01.
  • Deci, E. L., and R. M. Ryan, Eds. 2002. Handbook of Self-Determination Theory Research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
  • Demir, K., and Y. Akman Karabeyoglu. 2015. “Factors Associated with Absenteeism in High Schools.” Eurasian Journal of Educational Research 62: 37–56. doi:10.14689/ejer.2016.62.4.
  • Dotterer, A. M., and E. Wehrspann. 2016. “Parent Involvement and Academic Outcomes among Urban Adolescents: Examining the Role of School Engagement.” Educational Psychology 36: 812–830. doi:10.1080/01443410.2015.1099617.
  • Eccles, J. S., C. Midgley, A. Wigfield, C. M. Buchanan, D. Reuman, and C. Flanagan. 1993. “Development during Adolescence: The Impact of Stage Environment Fit on Young Adolescents’ Experiences in Schools and in Families.” American Psychologist 48: 90–101. doi:10.1037/10254-034.
  • Fall, A.-M., and G. Roberts. 2012. “High School Dropouts: Interactions between Social Context, Self-Perceptions, School Engagement, and Student Dropout.” Journal of Adolescence 35: 787–798. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.11.004.
  • Fatou, N., and V. Kubiszewski. 2018. “Are Perceived School Climate Dimensions Predictive of Students’ Engagement?” Social Psychology of Education 21: 427–446. doi:10.1007/s11218-017-9422-x.
  • Finn, J. D. 1989. “Withdrawing from School.” Review of Educational Research 59: 117–142. doi:10.2307/1170412.
  • Finn, J. D., and K. A. Kasza. 2009. “Disengagement from School” In Engaging Young People in Learning: Why Does It Matter and What Can We Do? edited by J. Morton, 4–35. Wellington, New Zealand: New Zealand Council for Educational Research.
  • Finn, J. D., and K. S. Zimmer. 2012. “Student Engagement: What Is It? Why Does It Matter?.” In Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, edited by S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie, 97–132. New York, NY: Springer.
  • Finn, J. D., and D. A. Rock. 1997. “Academic Success among Students at Risk for School Failure.” Journal of Applied Psychology 82: 221–235. doi:10.1037//0021-9010.82.2.221.
  • Flum, H., and A. Kaplan. 2012. “Identity Formation in Educational Settings: A Contextualized View of Theory and Research in Practice.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 37: 240–245. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.01.003.
  • Fokkens-Bruinsma, M., E. T. C. Canrinus, M. Ten Hove, and L. Rietveld. 2018. “The Relationship between Teacher’s Work Motivation and Classroom Goal Orientation.” Pedagogische Studiën 95: 86–100.
  • Fredricks, J. A., P. C. Blumenfeld, and A. H. Paris. 2004. “School Engagement: Potential of the Concept, State of Evidence.” Review of Educational Research 74: 59–109. doi:10.3102/00346543074001059.
  • Froiland, J. M., M. L. Davison, and F. C. Worrell. 2016. “Aloha Teachers: Teacher Autonomy Support Promotes Native Hawaiian and Pacific Islander Students’ Motivation, School Belonging, Course-Taking and Math Achievement.” Social Psychology of Education 19: 879–894. doi:10.1007/s11218-016-9355-9.
  • Furlong, M. J., A. D. Whipple, G. S. Jean, J. Simental, A. Soliz, and S. Punthuna. 2003. “Multiple Contexts of School Engagement: Moving toward a Unifying Framework for Educational Research and Practice.” The California School Psychologist 8: 99–113. doi:10.1007/bf03340899.
  • Galliher, R. V., S. S. Rostosky, and H. K. Hughes. 2004. “School Belonging, Self-Esteem, and Depressive Symptoms in Adolescents: An Examination of Sex, Sexual Attraction Status, and Urbanicity.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 33: 235–245. doi:10.1023/b:joyo.0000025322.11510.9d.
  • Giano, Z., B. M. Tuttle, M. J. Merten, K. L. Gallus, R. B. Cox, and K. M. Shreffler. 2018. “Parental Documentation Status and Educational Aspirations among Latino Adolescents: Mediating Effects of School Connectedness and Parental Attitudes Towards Education.” Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 40: 279–293. doi:10.1177/0739986318770377.
  • Gillen-O’Neel, C., and A. Fuligni. 2012. “A Longitudinal Study of School Belonging and Academic Motivation across High School.” Child Development 84: 678–692. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01862.x.
  • Gonida, E. N., K. Voulala, and G. Kiosseoglou. 2009. “Students’ Achievement Goal Orientations and Their Behavioral and Emotional Engagement: Co-Examining the Role of Perceived School Goal Structures and Parents Goals during Adolescence.” Learning and Individual Differences 19: 53–60. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2008.04.002.
  • Goodenow, C. 1993. “The Psychological Sense of School Membership among Adolescents: Scale Development and Educational Correlates.” Psychology in the Schools 30: 79–90. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1<79::aid-pits2310300113>3.0.co;2-x.
  • Goodenow, C., and K. E. Grady. 1993. “The Relationship of School Belonging and Friends’ Values to Academic Motivation among Urban Adolescent Students.” The Journal of Experimental Education 62: 60–71. doi:10.1080/00220973.1993.9943831.
  • Gottfredson, G. 1984. Effective School Battery. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Gray, J., and M. Hackling. 2009. “Wellbeing and Retention: A Senior Secondary Student Perspective.” The Australian Educational Researcher 36: 119–145. doi:10.1007/bf03216902.
  • Greene, B. A., and R. B. Miller. 1996. “Influences on Achievement: Goals, Perceived Ability, and Cognitive Engagement.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 21: 181–192. doi:10.1006/ceps.1996.0015.
  • Griffin, C. B., S. M. Cooper, I. W. Metzger, A. R. Golden, and C. N. White. 2017. “School Racial Climate and the Academic Achievement of African American High School Students: The Mediating Role of School Engagement.” Psychology in the Schools 54: 673–688. doi:10.1002/pits.22026.
  • Hagborg, W. J. 1994. “An Exploration of School Membership among Middle- and High-School Students.” Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment 12: 312–323. doi:10.1177/073428299401200401.
  • Hagborg, W. J. 1998. “An Investigation of a Brief Measure of School Membership.” Adolescence 33: 461–468.
  • Hamre, B. K., and R. C. Pianta. 2001. “Early Teacher-Child Relationships and the Trajectory of Children‘S School Outcomes through Eighth Grade.” Child Development 72: 625–638. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00301.
  • Harter, S. 1988. The Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents. Denver: University of Colorado.
  • Hascher, T., and G. Hagenauer. 2010. “Alienation from School.” International Journal of Educational Research 49: 220–232. doi:10.1016/j.ijer.2011.03.002.
  • Hernández, M. M., R. W. Robins, K. F. Widaman, and R. D. Conger. 2017. “Ethnic Pride, Self-Esteem, and School Belonging: A Reciprocal Analysis over Time.” Developmental Psychology 53: 2384–2396. doi:10.1037/dev0000434.
  • Hill, N. E., B. Liang, M. Price, W. Polk, J. Perella, and M. Savitz-Romer. 2018. “Envisioning a Meaningful Future and Academic Engagement: The Role of Parenting Practices and School-Based Relationships.” Psychology in the Schools 55: 595–608. doi:10.1002/pits.22146.
  • Hill, N. E., and M.-T. Wang. 2015. “From Middle School to College: Developing Aspirations, Promoting Engagement, and Indirect Pathways from Parenting to Post High School Enrollment.” Developmental Psychology 51: 224–235. doi:10.1037/a0038367.
  • Ho, E. S. C. 2005. “Effect of School Decentralization and School Climate on Student Mathematics Performance: The Case of Hong Kong.” Educational Research for Policy and Practice 4: 47–64. doi:10.1007/s10671-005-1546-7.
  • Houston, D. M. 2000. Attribution, Social Comparison and Academic Performance ESRC Small Grants Scheme End of Award Report to ESRC. Kent, UK: Economic and Social Research Council.
  • Hughes, J. N., M. H. Im, and P. J. Allee. 2015. “Effect of School Belonging Trajectories in Grades 6–8 on Achievement: Gender and Ethnic Differences.” Journal of School Psychology 53: 493–507. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2015.08.001.
  • Hurd, N. M., S. Hussain, and C. P. Bradshaw. 2018. “School Disorder, School Connectedness, and Psychosocial Outcomes: Moderation by a Supportive Figure in the School.” Youth & Society 50: 328–350. doi:10.1177/0044118X15598029.
  • Ibañez, G. E., G. P. Kuperminc, G. Jurkovic, and J. Perilla. 2004. “Cultural Attributes and Adaptations Linked to Achievement Motivation among Latino Adolescents.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 33: 559–568. doi:10.1023/b:joyo.0000048069.22681.2c.
  • Irvin, M. J., J. L. Meece, S. Byun, T. W. Farmer, and B. C. Hutchins. 2011. “Relationships of School Context to Rural Youth’s Educational Achievement and Aspirations.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 40: 1225–1242. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9628-8.
  • Jenkins, P. 1995. “School Delinquency and School Commitment.” Sociology of Education 68: 221–236. doi:10.2307/2112686.
  • Jimerson, S. R., E. Campos, and J. L. Greif. 2003. “Toward an Understanding of Definitions and Measures of School Engagement and Related Terms.” The California School Psychologist 8: 7–27. doi:10.1007/bf03340893.
  • Johnson, M. K., R. Crosnoe, and G. H. Elder. 2001. “Student Attachment and Academic Engagement: The Role of Race and Ethnicity.” Sociology of Education 74: 318–340. doi:10.2307/2673138.
  • Johnson, M. K., R. Crosnoe, and L. L. Thaden. 2006. “Gendered Patterns in Adolescents’ School Attachment.” Social Psychology Quarterly 69: 284–295. doi:10.1177/019027250606900305.
  • Juvonen, J. 2006. “Sense of Belonging, Social Relationships, and School Functioning.” In Handbook of Educational Psychology, edited by P. A. Alexander and P. H. Winne, 255–674. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Kaminski, J. W., R. W. Puddy, D. M. Hall, S. Y. Cashman, A. E. Crosby, and L. A. Ortega. 2010. “The Relative Influence of Different Domains of Social Connectedness on Self-Directed Violence in Adolescence.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 39: 460–473. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9472-2.
  • Kiefer, S. M., K. M. Alley, and C. R. Ellerbrock. 2015. “Teacher and Peer Support for Young Adolescents’ Motivation, Engagement, and School Belonging.” RMLE Online 38 8: 1–18. doi:10.1080/19404476.2015.11641184.
  • King, R. B., D. M. McInerney, and D. A. Watkins. 2012. “Studying for the Sake of Others: The Role of Social Goals on Academic Engagement.” Educational Psychology 32: 749–776. doi:10.1080/01443410.2012.730479.
  • Knifsend, C. A., and S. Graham. 2011. “Too Much of a Good Thing? How Breath of Extracurricular Participation Relates to School-Related Affect and Academic Outcomes during Adolescence.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 41: 379–389. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9737-4.
  • Korpershoek, H. 2016. “Relationships among Motivation, Commitment, Cognitive Capacities, and Academic Achievement in Secondary Education.” Frontline Learning Research 4: 28–43. doi:10.14786/flr.v4i3.182.
  • Korpershoek, H., G. J. Harms, H. De Boer, M. F. Van Kuijk, and S. Doolaard. 2016. “A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Classroom Management Strategies and Classroom Management Programs on Students’ Academic, Behavioural, Emotional, and Motivational Outcomes.” Review of Educational Research 86: 643–680. doi:10.3102/0034654315626799.
  • Kuperminc, G. P., A. J. Darnell, and A. Alvarez-Jimenez. 2008. “Parent Involvement in the Academic Adjustment of Latino Middle and High School Youth: Teacher Expectations and School Belonging as Mediators.” Journal of Adolescence 31: 469–483. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.09.003.
  • Lam, S., S. Jimerson, B. P. H. Wong, E. Kikas, H. Shin, F. H. Veiga, … J. Zollneritsch. 2014. “Understanding and Measuring Student Engagement in School: The Results of an International Study from 12 Countries.” School Psychology Quarterly 29: 213–232. doi:10.1037/spq0000057.
  • Lam, S.-F., B. P. H. Wong, H. Yang, and Y. Liu. 2012. “Understanding Student Engagement with a Contextual Model.” In Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, edited by S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie, 403–420. New York, NY: Springer.
  • Lam, U. F., -W.-W. Chen, J. Zhang, and T. Liang. 2015. “It Feels Good to Learn Where I Belong: School Belonging, Academic Emotions, and Academic Achievement in Adolescents.” School Psychology International 36: 393–409. doi:10.1177/0143034315589649.
  • Lawson, M. A., and H. A. Lawson. 2013. “New Conceptual Frameworks for Student Engagement Research, Policy, and Practice.” Review of Educational Research 83: 432–479. doi:10.3102/0034654313480891.
  • Lazowski, R. A., and C. S. Hulleman. 2016. “Motivation Interventions in Education: A Meta-Analytic Review.” Review of Educational Research 86: 602–640. doi:10.3102/0034654315617832.
  • LeCroy, C. W., and J. Krysik. 2008. “Predictors of Academic Achievement and School Attachment among Hispanic Adolescents.” Children & Schools 30: 197–209. doi:10.1093/cs/30.4.197.
  • Lee, G., and Y. Kim. 2016. “Interrelationship among School Characteristics, Parental Involvement, and Children’s Characteristics in Predicting Children’s Victimization by Peers: Comparison between the United States and Three Eastern Asia Countries.” Journal of International Education Research 12: 119–128. doi:10.19030/jier.v12i4.9798.
  • Lee, J.-S. 2013. “The Relationship between Student Engagement and Academic Performance: Is It a Myth or Reality?.” The Journal of Educational Research 107: 177–185. doi:10.1080/00220671.2013.807491.
  • Lee, V. E., and D. T. Burkam. 2003. “Dropping Out of High School: The Role of School Organization and Structure.” American Educational Research Journal 40: 353–393. doi:10.3102/00028312040002353.
  • Leffert, N., P. L. Beson, P. C. Scales, A. R. Sharma, D. R. Drake, and D. A. Blyth. 1998. “Developmental Assets: Measurement and Prediction of Risk Behaviors among Adolescents.” Applied Developmental Science 2: 209–230. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0204_4.
  • Lei, H., Y. Cui, and M. M. Chiu. 2016. “Affective Teacher-Student Relationships and Students‘ Externalizing Behavior Problems: A Meta-Analysis.” Frontiers in Psychology 7: 1–12. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01311.
  • Lewis, K. M., C. M. Sullivan, and D. Bybee. 2006. “An Experimental Evaluation of a School-Based Emancipatory Intervention to Promote African American Well-Being and Youth Leadership.” Journal of Black Psychology 32: 3–28. doi:10.1177/0095798405283229.
  • Li, Y., and R. M. Lerner. 2011. “Trajectories of School Engagement during Adolescence: Implications for Grades, Depression, Delinquency, and Substance Use.” Developmental Psychology 47: 233–247. doi:10.1037/a0021307.
  • Li, Y., and R. M. Lerner. 2012. “Interrelations of Behavioral, Emotional, and Cognitive School Engagement in High School Students.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 42: 2–32. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9857-5.
  • Li, Y. B., and R. M. Lerner. 2013. “Interrelations of Behavioral, Emotional, and Cognitive School Engagement in High School Students.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 42: 20–32. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9857-5.
  • Liang, B., A. J. Tracy, M. E. Kenny, D. Brogan, and R. Gatha. 2010. “The Relational Health Indices for Youth: An Examination of Reliability and Validity Aspects.” Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development 42: 255–274. doi:10.1177/0748175609354596.
  • Libbey, H. P. 2004. “Measuring Student Relationships to School: Attachment, Bonding, Connectedness, and Engagement.” Journal of School Health 74: 274–283. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08284.x.
  • Lipsey, M. W., and D. B. Wilson. 2001. Practical Meta-Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Liu, R. X. 2016. “School Bonding, Peer Associations, and Self-Views: The Influences of Gender and Grandparent Attachment on Adolescents in Mainland China.” Youth & Society 48: 451–469. doi:10.1177/0044118x13496825.
  • Liu, Y., and Z. Lu. 2011. “Trajectories of Chinese Students’ Sense of School Belonging and Academic Achievement over the High School Transition Period.” Learning and Individual Differences 21: 187–190. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.12.007.
  • Lohmeier, J. H., and S. W. Lee. 2011. “A School Connectedness Scale for Use with Adolescents.” Educational Research and Evaluation 17: 85–95. doi:10.1080/13803611.2011.597108.
  • Ma, X. 2003. “Sense of Belonging to School: Can Schools Make a Difference?” The Journal of Educational Research 96: 340–349. doi:10.1080/00220670309596617.
  • Maddox, S. J., and R. J. Prinz. 2003. “School Bonding in Children and Adolescents: Conceptualization, Assessment, and Associated Variables.” Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review 6: 31–49. doi:10.1023/a:1022214022478.
  • Maslow, A. H. 1962. Toward a Psychology of Being. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
  • McGill, R. K., D. Hughes, S. Alicea, and N. Way. 2012. “Academic Adjustment across Middle School: The Role of Public Regard and Parenting.” Developmental Psychology 48: 1003–1018. doi:10.1037/a0026006.
  • McInerney, D. M., M. Dowson, and A. S. Yeung. 2005. “Facilitating Conditions for School Motivation: Construct Validity and Applicability.” Educational and Psychological Measurement 65: 1–21. doi:10.1177/0013164405278561.
  • Mo, Y., and K. Singh. 2008. “Parents’ Relationships and Involvement: Effects on Students’ School Engagement and Performance.” RMLE Online 31 10: 1–11. doi:10.1080/19404476.2008.11462053.
  • Molinari, L., and C. Mameli. 2018. “Basic Psychological Needs and School Engagement: A Focus on Justice and Agency.” Social Psychology of Education 21: 157–172. doi:10.1007/s11218-017-9410-1.
  • Niemiec, C. P., and R. M. Ryan. 2009. “Autonomy, Competence and Relatedness in the Classroom: Applying Self-Determination Theory to Classroom Practice.” Theory and Research in Education 7: 133–144. doi:10.1177/1477878509104318.
  • Okilwa, N. S. A. 2016. “Exploring School- and Home-Related Protective Factors for Economically Disadvantaged Middle School Students.” Journal of At-Risk Issues19: 34–46.
  • Osterman, K. F. 2000. “Students’ Need for Belonging in the School Community.” Review of Educational Research 70: 323–367. doi:10.2307/1170786.
  • Perry, J. C., X. Liu, and Y. Pabian. 2009. “School Engagement as a Mediator of Academic Performance among Urban Youth: The Role of Career Preparation, Parental Career Support, and Teacher Support.” The Counseling Psychologist 38: 269–295. doi:10.1177/0011000009349272.
  • Pittman, L. D., A. Richmond, and A. Richmond. 2007. “Academic and Psychological Functioning in Late Adolescence: The Importance of School Belonging.” The Journal of Experimental Education 75: 270–290. doi:10.3200/jexe.75.4.270-292.
  • Popp, A. M., and A. A. Peguero. 2012. “Social Bonds and the Role of School-Based Victimization.” Journal of Interpersonal Violence 27: 3366–3388. doi:10.1177/0886260512445386.
  • Prelow, H. M., M. A. Bowman, and S. R. Weaver. 2006. “Predictors of Psychosocial Well-Being in Urban African American and European American Youth: The Role of Ecological Factors.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 36: 543–553. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9038-5.
  • Ream, R. K., and R. W. Rumberger. 2008. “Student Engagement, Peer Social Capital, and School Dropout among Mexican American and Non-Latino White Students.” Sociology of Education 81: 109–139. doi:10.1177/003804070808100201.
  • Reeve, J., and C.-M. Tseng. 2011. “Agency as a Fourth Aspect of Students’ Engagement during Learning Activities.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 36: 257–267. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.05.002.
  • Resnick, M. D., P. S. Bearman, R. W. Blum, K. E. Bauman, K. M. Harris, J. Jones, and J. R. Udry. 1998. “Protecting Adolescents from Harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health.” In Adolescent Behavior and Society: A Book of Readings, edited by R. E. Muuss and J. D. Porton, 376–395. 5th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  • Resnick, M. D., P. S. Bearman, R. W. Blum, K. E. Bauman, K. M. Harris, J. Jones, … J. R. Udry. 1997. “Protecting Adolescents from Harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health.” The Journal of the American Medical Association 278: 823–832. doi:10.1001/jama.278.10.823.
  • Reynolds, K. J., E. Lee, I. Turner, D. Bromhead, and E. Subasic. 2017. “How Does School Climate Impact Academic Achievement? an Examination of Social Identity Processes.” School Psychology International 38: 78–97. doi:10.1177/0143034316682295.
  • Roeser, R. W., K. R. Strobel, and G. Quihuis. 2002. “Studying Early Adolescents‘ Academic Motivation, Social-Emotional Functioning, and Engagement in Learning: Variable- and Person-Centered Approaches.” Anxiety, Stress & Coping 15: 345–368. doi:10.1080/1061580021000056519.
  • Rolland, R. G. 2012. “Synthesizing the Evidence on Classroom Goal Structures in Middle and Secondary Schools: A Meta-Analysis and Narrative Review.” Review of Educational Research 82: 396–435. doi:10.3102/0034654312464909.
  • Roorda, D. L., H. M. Y. Koomen, J. L. Spilt, and F. J. Oort. 2011. “The Influence of Affective Teacher-Student Relationships on Students’ School Engagement and Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Approach.” Review of Educational Research 81: 493–529. doi:10.3102/0034654311421793.
  • Roorda, D. L., S. Jak, M. Zee, F. J. Oort, and H. M. Y. Koomen. 2017. “Affective Teacher-Student Relationships and Students‘ Engagement and Achievement: A Meta-Analytic Update and Test of the Mediating Role of Engagement.” School Psychology Review 46: 239–261. doi:10.17105/SPR-2017-0035.V46-3.
  • Rostosky, S. S., G. P. Owens, R. S. Zimmerman, and E. D. B. Riggle. 2003. “Associations among Sexual Attraction Status, School Belonging, and Alcohol and Marijuana Use in Rural High School Students.” Journal of Adolescence 26: 741–751. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.09.002.
  • Ryan, R. M., and E. L. Deci. 2009. “Promoting Self-Determined School Engagement: Motivation, Learning, and Well-Being.” In Handbook on Motivation at School, edited by K. R. Wentzel and A. Wigfield, 171–196. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Salmela-Aro, K., and K. Upadyaya. 2014. “School Burnout and Engagement in the Context of Demands-Resources Model.” British Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 137–151. doi:10.1111/bjep.12018.
  • Salmela-Aro, K., N. Kiuru, E. Leskinen, and J.-E. Nurmi. 2009. “School Burnout Inventory: Reliability and Validity.” European Journal of Psychological Assessment 25: 48–57. doi:10.1027/1015-5759.25.1.48.
  • Sánchez, B., Y. Colón, and P. Esparza. 2005. “The Role of Sense of School Belonging and Gender in the Academic Adjustment of Latino Adolescents.” Journal of Youth and Adolescence 34: 619–628. doi:10.1007/s10964-005-8950-4.
  • Sciarra, D., and H. Seirup. 2008. “The Multidimensionality of School Engagement and Math Achievement among Racial Groups.” Professional School Counseling 11: 218–228. doi:10.5330/psc.n.2010-11.218.
  • Shochet, I. M., and C. L. Smith. 2014. “A Prospective Study Investigating the Links among Classroom Environment, School Connectedness, and Depressive Symptoms in Adolescents.” Psychology in the Schools 51: 480–492. doi:10.1002/pits.21759.
  • Shochet, I. M., M. R. Dadds, D. Ham, and R. Montague. 2006. “School Connectedness Is an Underemphasized Parameter in Adolescent Mental Health: Results of a Community Prediction Study.” Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 35: 170–179. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3502_1.
  • Shochet, I. M., T. L. Smyth, and R. Homel. 2007. “The Impact of Parental Attachment on Adolescent Perception of the School Environment and School Connectedness.” Australian and New Zealand Journal of Family Therapy 28: 109–118. doi:10.1375/anft.28.2.109.
  • Singh, K., M. Chang, and S. Dika. 2010. “Ethnicity, Self-Concept, and School Belonging: Effects on School Engagement.” Educational Research for Policy and Practice 9: 159–175. doi:10.1007/s10671-010-9087-0.
  • Sirin, S. R., and L. Rogers-Sirin. 2005. “Components of School Engagement among African American Adolescents.” Applied Developmental Science 9: 5–13. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0901_2.
  • Skinner, E. A., G. Marchand, C. J. Furrer, and T. Kindermann. 2008. “Engagement and Disaffection in the Classroom: Part of a Larger Motivational Dynamic?” Journal of Educational Psychology 100: 765–781. doi:10.1037/a0012840.
  • Skinner, E. A., J. G. Wellborn, and J. P. Connell. 1990. “What It Takes to Do Well in School and whether I‘Ve Got It: A Process Model of Perceived Control and Children‘S Engagement and Achievement in School.” Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 22–32. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.82.1.22.
  • Skinner, E. A., and M. J. Belmont. 1993. “Motivation in the Classroom: Reciprocal Effects of Teacher Behaviour and Student Engagement across the School Year.” Journal of Educational Psychology 85: 571–581. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.85.4.571.
  • Skinner, E. A., T. A. Kindermann, and C. J. Furrer. 2009. “A Motivational Perspective on Engagement and Disaffection. Conceptualization and Assessment of Children‘S Behavioral and Emotional Participation in Academic Activities in the Classroom.” Educational and Psychological Measurement 69: 493–525. doi:10.1177/0013164408323233.
  • Smerdon, B. A. 2002. “Students’ Perceptions of Membership in Their High Schools.” Sociology of Education 75: 287–305. doi:10.2307/3090280.
  • Stefansson, K. K., S. Gestsdottir, G. J. Geldhof, S. Skulason, and R. M. Lerner. 2016. “A Bifactor Model of School Engagement: Assessing General and Specific Aspects of Behavioral, Emotional and Cognitive Engagement among Adolescents.” International Journal of Behavioral Development 40: 471–480. doi:10.1177/0165025415604056.
  • Taylor, L., and J. Parsons. 2011. “Improving Student Engagement.” Current Issues in Education 14 (1): 1–33.
  • Thompson, D. R., R. Iachan, M. Overpeck, J. G. Ross, and L. A. Gross. 2006. “School Connectedness in the Health Behavior in School-Aged Children Study: The Role of Student, School, and School Neighborhood Characteristics.” Journal of School Health 76: 379–386. doi:10.1111/j.1746-1561.2006.00129.x.
  • Tomek, S., A. C. Bolland, L. M. Hooper, S. Hitchcock, and J. M. Bolland. 2017. “The Impact of Middle School Connectedness on Future High School Outcomes in a Black American Sample.” Middle Grades Research Journal 11: 1–12.
  • Topçu, M. S., E. Erbilgin, and S. Arikan. 2016. “Factors Predicting Turkish and Korean Students’ Science and Mathematics Achievement in TIMSS 2011.” EURASIA Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education 12: 1711–1737. doi:10.12973/eurasia.2016.1530a.
  • Tyler, K. M., and C. M. Boelter. 2008. “Linking Black Middle School Students’ Perceptions of Teachers’ Expectations to Academic Engagement and Efficacy.” Negro Educational Review59: 27–44.
  • Tyler, T. R., and P. Degoey. 1995. “Collective Restraint in Social Dilemmas: Procedural Justice and Social Identification Effects on Support for Authorities.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69: 482–497. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.3.482.
  • Udry, J. R. 1998. The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health), Waves I & II, 1994-1996. Chapel Hill, NC: Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill (Producer). Los Altos, CA: Sociometrics Corporation, American Family Data Archive (Producer & Distributor)
  • Uwah, C., H. McMahon, and C. Furlow. 2008. “School Belonging, Educational Aspirations, and Academic Self-Efficacy among African American Male High School Students: Implications for School Counselors.” Professional School Counseling 11: 296–305. doi:10.5330/psc.n.2010-11.296.
  • Van Houtte, M., and D. Van Maele. 2012. “Students’ Sense of Belonging in Technical/Vocational Schools versus Academic Schools: The Mediating Role of Faculty Trust in Students.” Teachers College Record 114 (7): 1–36.
  • Van Rooijen, M., H. Korpershoek, J. Vugteveen, and M.-C. Opdenakker. 2017. “De Overgang Van Het Basis- Naar Het Voortgezet Onderwijs En De Verdere Schoolloopbaan [Transition from Primary to Secondary Education and the Continuing School Career].” Pedagogische Studiën 94: 110–134.
  • Voelkl, K. E. 1996. “Measuring Students’ Identification with School.” Educational and Psychological Measurement 56: 760–770. doi:10.1177/0013164496056005003.
  • Voelkl, K. E. 1997. “Identification with School.” American Journal of Education 105: 204–319. doi:10.1086/444158.
  • Voelkl, K. E. 2012. “School Identification.” In Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, edited by S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, and C. Wylie, 193–218. New York, NY: Springer.
  • Walker, C. O. 2011. “Student Perceptions of Classroom Achievement Goals as Predictors of Belonging and Content Instrumentality.” Social Psychology of Education 15: 97–107. doi:10.1007/s11218-011-9165-z.
  • Walker, C. O., and B. A. Greene. 2009. “The Relations between Student Motivational Beliefs and Cognitive Engagement in High School.” The Journal of Educational Research 102: 463–472. doi:10.3200/joer.102.6.463-472.
  • Walker, C. O., B. A. Greene, and R. A. Mansell. 2006. “Identification with Academics, Intrinsic/Extrinsic Motivation, and Self-Efficacy as Predictors of Cognitive Engagement.” Learning and Individual Differences 16: 1–12. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.004.
  • Walls, T. A., and T. D. Little. 2005. “Relations among Personal Agency, Motivation, and School Adjustment in Early Adolescence.” Journal of Educational Psychology 97: 23–31. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.1.23.
  • Wang, M.-T., and J. A. Fredricks. 2013. “The Reciprocal Links between School Engagement, Youth Problem Behaviors, and School Dropout during Adolescence.” Child Development 85: 722–737. doi:10.1111/cdev.12138.
  • Wang, M.-T., J. B. Willett, and J. S. Eccles. 2011. “The Assessment of School Engagement: Examining Dimensionality and Measurement Invariance by Gender and Race/Ethnicity.” Journal of School Psychology 49: 465–480. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2011.04.001.
  • Wang, M.-T., and J. E. Eccles. 2011. “Adolescent Behavioral, Emotional, and Cognitive Engagement Trajectories in School and Their Differential Relations to Educational Success.” Journal of Research on Adolescence 22: 31–39. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2011.00753.x.
  • Wang, M.-T., and R. Holcombe. 2010. “Adolescents’ Perceptions of School Environment, Engagement, and Academic Achievement in Middle School.” American Educational Research Journal 47: 633–662. doi:10.3102/0002831209361209.
  • Wang, M.-T., and S. Sheikh-Khalil. 2013. “Does Parental Involvement Matter for Student Achievement and Mental Health in High School?” Child Development 85: 610–625. doi:10.1111/cdev.12153.
  • Wang, M.-T., and S. C. Peck. 2013. “Adolescent Educational Success and Mental Health Vary across School Engagement Profiles.” Developmental Psychology 49: 1266–1276. doi:10.1037/a0030028.
  • Wehlage, G. G., R. A. Rutter, G. A. Smith, N. Lesko, and R. R. Fernandez. 1989. Reducing the Risk: Schools as Communities of Support. New York, NY: Falmer Press.
  • Wellborn, J. G. 1991. Engaged and disaffected action: The conceptualization and measurement of motivation in the academic domain. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, NY.
  • WestEd. 2008. California Healthy Kids Survey: California School District Secondary School Survey Results Fall 07/Spring 08. Los Alamitos, CA: Author.
  • Wettersten, K. B., A. Guilmino, C. G. Herrick, P. J. Hunter, G. Y. Kim, D. Jagow, … J. McCormick. 2005. “Predicting Educational and Vocational Attitudes among Rural High School Students.” Journal of Counseling Psychology 52: 658–663. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.658.
  • Wigfield, A., J. S. Eccles, U. Schiefele, R. W. Roeser, and P. Davis-Kean. 2006. “The Development of Achievement Motivation.” In Handbook of Child Psychology, edited by N. Eisenberg, 933–1002. 6th ed., Vol. 3. New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Willms, J. D. 2003. Student Engagement at School. A Sense of Belonging and Participation. Results from PISA 2000. Paris, France: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.
  • Woolley, M. E., and G. L. Bowen. 2007. “In the Context of Risk: Supportive Adults and the School Engagement of Middle School Students.” Family Relations 56: 92–104. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2007.00442.x.
  • Wormington, S. V., K. G. Anderson, A. Schneider, K. L. Tomlinson, and S. A. Brown. 2016. “Peer Victimization and Adolescent Adjustment: Does School Belonging Matter?” Journal of School Violence 15 1: 1–21. doi:10.1080/15388220.2014.922472.
  • Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., H. M. Chipuer, M. Hanisch, P. A. Creed, and L. McGregor. 2006. “Relationships at School and Stage-Environment Fit as Resources for Adolescent Engagement and Achievement.” Journal of Adolescence 29: 911–933. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.04.008.