5,591
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The effect of moral reasoning and values as the mediator towards student’s prosocial behaviour

, , , , , & show all
Pages 32-44 | Received 03 May 2021, Accepted 18 Dec 2021, Published online: 07 Jan 2022

ABSTRACT

This study aims to determine the relationship between religiosity, moral reasoning, and internalization of pure values as the mediator of prosocial behaviour among students in Banda Aceh, Indonesia. The Secondary Islamic Education Internalization Scale (SPPIP-M), Behaviour Reasoning Scheme Indicator (UPSTA), and Prosocial Tendencies Measure (PTM) scale were used to measure the religiosity, moral reasoning, and internalization of pure values prosocial behaviour of students. The result showed a link between religiosity, moral reasoning, and internalization of pure values with prosocial behaviour of secondary school students in Banda Aceh.

Introduction

Prosocial or positive social behaviour is the opposite of anti-social behaviour that occurs when people benefit others rather than themselves (Hao et al., Citation2016; Kavussanu & Boardley, Citation2009; Luo, Citation2018; Vorlaufer, Citation2019). According to Penner (Citation2000), prosocial behaviour benefits others by looking at the norms that prevail in the social living environment. This behaviour is necessary to maintain a person’s active role as a supporter and one being supported (Klein, Citation2016; Kuem et al., Citation2017; Malti & Buchmann, Citation2010). In addition, preliminary studies have been extensively conducted on adolescents’ behaviour type (Eisenberg & Shell, Citation1986; Haan et al., Citation1976), which increases with age (Fabes et al., Citation1999).

Development of moral and moral reasoning

According to Kohlberg (Citation1984), the development of moral reasoning is closely related to the structure of thinking about interpersonal conflict situations. Kohlberg (Citation1984) and Ma (Citation2013), classified moral development into three levels, namely pre-conventional (motivated by self-interest), conventional (motivated by keeping social order, rules, and laws), and post-conventional (motivated by social contracts and universal ethical principles) levels. Kohlberg’s theory states that individuals are likely to reach the highest level of post-conversional moral reasoning, assuming moral issues are judged based on deeper principles and shared ideals rather than self-interest or obedience to laws and regulations (Fang et al., Citation2017).

Presently, most people have strong moral beliefs in determining right and wrong, even though universal moral values vary across cultures and ethnic groups. However, the moral development of each individual also referred to as moral reasoning, is the ability to analyse various complex issues and problems. Based on various literature, the most influential and studied theory of moral reasoning and development was proposed by Kohlberg (Fang et al., Citation2017).

Rest (Citation1986) proposed the theory of moral reasoning, which developed a problem-defining test based on the premise that each individual interprets moral dilemmas differently and reaches varying conclusions (Murk & Addleman, Citation1992; J. Rest et al., Citation1999). The study conducted showed that age and education explain many differences in moral reasoning. It stated that age and education can explain approximately 30 to 52% of the variance used to define test score problems (Murk & Addleman, Citation1992). According to Rest & Thoma (Citation1985), years of education are more strongly correlated with higher moral reasoning than age (King & Mayhew, Citation2002). Rest et al. (Citation1999) proposed a theory on the development of moral reasoning, called the Neo-Kohlbergian approach, by borrowing many of Kohlberg’s basic ideas. They described the development of moral reasoning as a distributional shift in which complex ones gradually replace more primitive ways of thinking.

At the beginning of the study on cognitive development, Kohlberg noted that apart from moral reasoning, other processes are essential for moral behaviour. However, Rest (Citation1984) made this process more explicit in a four-component morality model (Bebeau & Thoma, Citation1999; Wang et al., Citation2018). The first deals with moral sensitivity, which aims to interpret the situation by focusing on the various actions available and how they affect oneself and others. The second component concerns moral reasoning, which assesses the most justifiable actions. The third component prioritizes morals over other concerns by recognizing that individuals have numerous appropriate and inappropriate concerns on moral choices. The fourth component is related to constructing and implementing actions that serve moral choices. This component focuses on establishing appropriate actions, avoiding distractions, and maintaining the courage to continue (Bebeau & Thoma, Citation1999; Maeda et al., Citation2009).

Social problems among students

The need for developing prosocial behaviour is exacerbated by social symptoms, which are in line with the economic growth of the rapidly developing world. The National Center for Statistics BPS (Citation2015) showed that cases such as drug addiction, rape, and incest are among the most prevalent social problems among students. These cases were similar to those of BPS data, including rape, sexual harassment, and incest as common crimes against humanity that affect their victims’ psychology with prolonged trauma (Stroebel et al., Citation2012). Rape is a form of violence against women, which they tend to experience at work, home, and street, irrespective of the time (Suyanto et al., Citation2019). According to (Wismayanti et al., Citation2019), several low- and middle-income countries, such as Indonesia, are increasingly realizing the social problem associated with child sexual abuse, which requires effective intervention and prevention strategies. Statistically an average of five to six women are raped daily in Indonesia, which means every 4 hours, someone is raped.

Furthermore, there is an increase in violence and drug addiction among adolescents, which is increasingly recognized as an important public health problem (Schulenberg et al., Citation2020). Several studies showed that higher levels of violence are strongly associated with increased rates of drug use, such as cigarettes, alcohol, and marijuana (Scal et al., Citation2003; Weiner, Citation2002). Therefore, it is important to educate adolescents on the adverse effects of drug and alcohol abuse in school. The study by Riva et al. (Citation2018), Shahraki et al. (Citation2019), and Wang et al. (Citation2018) revealed that age is an important predictor in educating students on the use of drugs and the associated dangers. This is in addition to an increase in the bullying rate among children globally. According to a 2019 UNESCO report, there is a global increase in bullying among students, categorized into four types, namely physical, psychological/verbal, sexual, and cyberbullying. This condition is still a major problem in primary, secondary, and higher education levels, which adversely affect victims’ emotional and behavioural development. Apart from the impacts described, social violence also impacts the mental and physical health and academic performance of the victims (Jackson et al., Citation2019; Koyanagi et al., Citation2019) UNESCO; 2019). Therefore, these issues need to be addressed and controlled early by mitigating a high degree of pure values internalization integrated with knowledge on students, specifically through education.

Myers (Citation2012) stated that the religious factor is important in developing adolescent prosocial behaviour. Mercer et al. (Citation2017) and Read (Citation2015) stated that religiosity is a key component in adolescent identity formation. In this situation, adolescent identity formation is closely related to their involvement in religious services, practices, and activities (McCullough & Willoughby, Citation2009; Shariff & Norenzayan, Citation2007). This is in accordance with the study by Krause (Citation2003), which stated that religion helps individuals form the identity and meaning of life.

Moral emotion and moral reasoning

Moral reasoning is a cognitive process that underlies why an action is considered morally right or wrong (Malti et al., Citation2009). Meanwhile, moral emotion and its attributes are actual responses to moral and immoral situations by oneself or from others (Keller et al., Citation2003). Krettenauer et al. (Citation2013), stated that moral emotion is also an important measure of moral behaviour which is consistently associated with prosocial behaviour. According to Tangney et al. (Citation2007), moral emotions motivate moral behaviour by providing internalized rules with meaning for oneself. Hoffman (Citation2001) and Malti & Ongley ((Citation2014)) identified emotion as a key effective process in the moral development of children’s prosocial behaviour. Additionally, moral reasoning and emotion have been theorized to play an important role in the early development of moral action tendencies and considerations of the welfare of others (Arsenio, Citation2014; Hoffman; Citation2001).

Moral development is not limited to moral cognition, such as judgement and reasoning because it includes cognitive and emotional aspects (Gibbs, Citation2013; Malti et al., Citation2009). Several aspects of moral cognition and emotions have a close relationship with ongoing interactions because their emergence is dependent on one’s moral cognition (Dentici & Pagnin, Citation1992; Malti & Latzko, Citation2010). Both have a moral aspect that is equally important for prosocial behaviour and related actions. According to Dentici and Pagnin (Citation1992), moral cognition generates knowledge structures, which evoke moral emotions. Also, the study by Malti and Krettenauer (Citation2013) found that moral emotions are significantly related to prosocial and anti-social behaviour. This is because its attributes can be positively related to the type of social activities, such as guilt over the moral violation. Moral emotions are focused primarily on known negative emotions such as guilt and shame. Its attributes also have a strong cognitive commitment because individuals need to consider the consequences for themselves and others in the context of moral actions. Therefore, attributes of moral emotions are considered important for the development of prosocial behaviour tendencies.

Teaching and applying values, morals, ethics, and behaviours can help individuals build their self-esteem and personal development in a way that is more relevant to their religion. In addition, personal development based on the appreciation of pure values creates a strong sense of prosocial behaviour in daily life. According to Kumru et al. (Citation2012), prosocial behaviour exemplifies positive and inclusive social feelings, including cooperation, sharing, helping, leadership, expressing empathy, providing verbal support or promotion, and general friendliness. Therefore, this will indirectly assist in developing an integrated education between knowledge, social science, life skills, and Islamic Shariah values among students. In providing answers to study problems in accordance with the focus and objectives of the study, several hypotheses were developed as follows:

Hypothesis 1 (Ha1): There are differences in students’ prosocial behavior based on gender and place of residence

Hypothesis 2 (Ha2): There is an effect of religiosity on students’ prosocial behavior with moral reasoning as a mediator

Hypothesis 3 (Ha3): There is an effect of religiosity on students’ prosocial behavior with the pure value internalization variable as a mediator

Research methodology

This is a quantitative research with a cross-sectional approach used to collect data from selected individuals over a certain period (Glasow, Citation2005). The cross-sectional approach was chosen to provide in-depth analysis to an entity in the group, which can be used to express various issues and problems such as explaining attitudes, views, beliefs, feelings, behaviours, etc (Cohen et al., Citation2007). Furthermore, the cross-sectional approach was selected based on Creswell’s (Citation2014) view, which stated that a cross-sectional study can provide evidence related to the development of prosocial behaviours.

The sample consisted of 649 students selected based on the disciplinary cases of eight high schools in Banda Aceh using a multi-stage cluster sampling technique. The instruments used in this study, namely religiosity, moral reasoning, pure values and prosocial behaviour were measured using the SPPIM-R Secondary Islamic Education Internalization Scale (SPPIP-M) from Mahmood (Citation2006), Behaviour Reasoning Scheme Indicator (UPSTA; Hamzah, Citation2007), items for the appreciation of pure values (Othman et al., Citation2015) and the Prosocial Tendencies Measure (PTM) scale developed by (Carlo et al., Citation2010). The questionnaire instrument also used to measure religiosity, moral reasoning, pure values, and students’ prosocial behaviour contained 30, 14, 29, and 23 items with a reliability value of α = .90, .89, .89, and .89, respectively.

Furthermore, this study examined the role of moral reasoning and internalization of pure values as a mediator of the relationship between religiosity and prosocial behaviour. Two series of simple multiple regression tests were conducted to determine the effect of the mediator variable on the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour. According to Baron and Kenny (Citation1986), the three assumptions that must exist to develop the mediation variable are, first, there is a significant relationship between the independent variable and the mediator. Second, there is a significant relationship between the mediator and dependent variables. Finally, there is a significant relationship between the independent variable (IV) and the dependent variable (DV). The following diagram illustrates the mediation model:

shows a mediation model of the relationship between the independent and interrelated variables in this study:

  1. Route a denotes the regression coefficient of the relationship between the independent variable and the mediators.

  2. Route b denotes the regression coefficient of the relationship between mediators and the dependent variable.

  3. Route c denotes the regression coefficient of the independent variable and dependent variable before being controlled by the mediator variables.

  4. Route c’ denotes the regression coefficient of the independent variable and dependent variable after being controlled by the mediator variables.

Figure 1. Mediation Model Framework.

Figure 1. Mediation Model Framework.

Also, four regression coefficients were calculated to determine the mediation relationship in this study. The first conducted the average regression coefficient of the dependent variable (prosocial behaviour) on the independent (religiosity). The second conducted the average regression coefficient of a mediator (moral reasoning) on the independent variable (religiosity). The third conducted the average regression coefficient of the dependent variable (prosocial behaviour) on the mediator (moral reasoning). The fourth conducted the average regression coefficient of the dependent variable (prosocial behaviour) on the independent (religiosity) and intermediate variable (moral reasoning; Baron & Kenny, Citation1986).

Research findings

Differences in Prosocial Behaviour based on Gender

shows no difference in students’ prosocial behaviours based on gender (t = .30; p = .76 > .05). However, two aspects of prosocial behaviour also indicated differences in demographic factors, namely community (t = −2.11; p = .03 < .05) and altruism (t = 2.66; p = .00 < .00). This shows the prosocial behaviour of male students was higher than females. Therefore, the null hypothesis indicates no significant difference in students’ prosocial behaviour by gender was accepted.

Table 1. Differences in Prosocial Behaviour based on Gender

Differences in Prosocial Behaviour based on Residence

shows the differences in students’ prosocial behaviour based on residence factor (t = 2.22; p = 0.02 < 0.05). Based on the aspects studied, anonymous (t = 2.78; p = 0.00 < 0.05), request (t = 2.30; p = 0.02 < 0.05) and altruism (t = −3.44; p = 0.00 < 0.05) showed significant differences, as opposed to the community, anxiety and emotional aspects. Overall, students living in rural areas were found to have higher prosocial behaviour levels than those in urban areas. Therefore, the results indicated that there was no significant difference between students’ prosocial behaviour based on residence, hence, the null hypothesis was rejected.

Table 2. Differences in Prosocial Behaviour based on Residence

The Effect of Moral Reasoning as the Mediator Variable towards the Relationship between Religiosity and Prosocial Behaviour

Regression analysis was performed to examine the mediator variable’s influence (moral reasoning) on the relationship between religiosity and prosocial behaviour.

For a better understanding, see, as follows:

Figure 2. Mediation Model Framework A.

Figure 2. Mediation Model Framework A.

In , the result showed that religiosity had a significant effect on prosocial behaviour without and with the mediator variable which obtained (β = 0.606, p = 0.00) and (β = 0.628, p = 0.00), respectively. It also showed that the beta value of the coefficient (β) has increased.

These results suggested that moral reasoning did not exist as a mediator of the relationship between religiosity and prosocial behaviour of students in a secondary school in Banda Aceh. Therefore, the null hypothesis was accepted ().

Table 3. The relationship between Religiosity and Student Prosocial Behaviours with Moral Reasoning as the Mediator Variable

The Effect of Internalization of Pure Values as the Mediator Variable on the Relationship between Religiosity and Prosocial Behaviour

Regression analysis was performed to examine the influence of the mediator (internalization of pure values) on the relationship between religiosity and prosocial behaviour.

For a better understanding, see, below:

Figure 3. Mediation Model Framework B.

Figure 3. Mediation Model Framework B.

In , the results showed that religiosity had a significant influence on prosocial behaviour without and with the mediator variable which obtained (β = 0.606, p = 0.00) and (β = 0.199, p = 0.00), respectively. It also showed a decrease in the coefficient beta value (β).

This means that the internalization of pure values is needed as a mediator variable for the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour due to a direct effect. Therefore, the null hypothesis was rejected due to the significant effect of the internalization of moral values as a mediator in the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour in a secondary school in Banda Aceh ().

Table 4. Religiosity and Prosocial Behaviour with Internalization of Pure Value as the Mediator Variable

Discussion

Differences in Student’s Prosocial Behaviour based on Gender and Residence

The results showed no significant difference in prosocial behaviour between male and female students. This finding suggested that male students’ level of prosocial behaviour was similar to females. However, this is contrary to the study by Madsen et al. (Citation2017), stating that men are more likely to offer help in emergencies for victims in need of help than women. Similar empirical research by Yang et al. (Citation2018) indicated that boys and girls have the same prosocial behaviour towards family, while girls are more prosocial towards friends and strangers than boys. Meanwhile, Veenstra et al. (Citation2008) found that female students are more likely to exhibit empathic and responsible behaviours than their male counterparts that are more likely to help in emergencies and dangerous situations. According to Hoxmeier et al. (Citation2020), in some cases, women show higher prosocial behaviours in assisting victims of trauma, mental health care, counselling, and emotional support than men.

Furthermore, this study showed the differences in the degree of prosocial behaviour based on residence. The analysis indicated a significant difference between students living in urban and rural areas. Overall, students living in rural areas showed higher mean prosocial behaviour scores than those in urban. According to Sng et al. (Citation2017) population density and living costs are negatively correlated with helping behaviours, therefore, individuals residing in densely packed environments are less likely to help others. The chaotic and unstable environment can easily motivate students to act aggressively (Jaffee et al., Citation2012). Therefore, a rural community that practices fairness and respect becomes the stimulus for individuals’ prosocial behaviour (Strelan, Citation2007).

The Effect of Moral Reasoning and the Internalization of Pure Values as Mediators towards the Relationship between Religiosity and Prosocial Behaviour

It is important to identify the significant influence of two aspects of moral reasoning and the value of pure values as mediator variables on the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour. Therefore, this study conducted two series of simple multiple regression analyses against the hypothesis test for the existence of a moral reasoning variable as the mediator on the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour. This is in addition to the existence of the internalization of pure values as a mediator variable on the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour.

The Effect of Religiosity on Student Prosocial Behaviour based on Moral Reasoning as the mediator

Religiosity has a significant relationship with student prosocial behaviour without the contribution of moral reasoning variables. This is based on the regression analysis findings for the absence of significant influence of moral reasoning as a mediator variable on the relationship between religiosity and student prosocial behaviour. It is also in accordance with the study by Hardy and Carlo (Citation2005), which revealed that there is a significant positive relationship between religiosity and obedient prosocial behaviour. The study by Roth (Citation2017) also indicated that prosocial behaviour is highly dependent on a person’s level of empathy, religiosity, and spirituality. Students who adhere to religious teachings involve in prosocial behaviour without deep moral reasoning. Milot and Ludden (Citation2009) stated that the importance of religiosity was a more prominent protective factor against drug use even after considering parental support. A person’s level of religiosity makes a strong contribution to shaping prosocial traits, even under false detectors, religious people still report high moral self-image (Hur et al., Citation2019; Moulin-stożek et al., Citation2018; Ward & King, Citation2018).

Eisenberg et al. (Citation2005) found no significant relationship between moral reasoning and prosocial behaviour. Individuals engage in prosocial behaviour because they transform moral reasoning over time. Children tend to use hedonistic thinking, and prosocial orientation needs to care and agree with their peers. According to preliminary studies, there is not always a positive relationship between student’s moral reasoning and prosocial behaviour, such as understanding that stealing, cheating, bullying, and involving in sexual misconduct is wrong and immoral (Anyanwu et al., Citation2020; Ugoji, Citation2014; Van der Graaff et al., Citation2018).

Laible et al. (Citation2014) stated that moral reasoning allows adolescents to understand the consequences of their actions towards others and analyse the victim’s perspective due to their aggressive behaviour. Therefore, moral reasoning can also promote feelings of responsibility towards others and make adolescents have good prosocial behaviour, preventing them from behaving aggressively.

The Effect of Religiosity on Student Prosocial Behaviour based on Internalization of Pure Values as the mediator

The existence of pure values internalization as the mediator variable for the relationship between religiosity and students’ prosocial behaviour can be further strengthened by the results of this study. The result indicated a significant influence of religiosity on students’ prosocial behaviour based on the internalization of pure values. This showed that the internalization of pure values contributed to students’ prosocial behaviour. Its importance in reinforcing the effectiveness of students’ prosocial behaviour is also highlighted in studies conducted by (Hui, Citation2007; Othman et al., Citation2015; Zaidin, Citation2015).

These findings support recent study, which explains a strong relationship between religiosity and prosocial behaviour without a significant difference between school types in terms of students’ prosocial characteristics (Arslantürk & Harput, Citation2020). Other preliminary studies also stated that the variables of religiosity, empathy, and anti-social behaviour cause students’ prosocial behaviour. This study shows a strong bond between prosocial behaviour, empathy, anti-social behaviour, and spirituality (Roth, Citation2017). The implementation of the process that is implanted in learning either through textbooks at school (Widodo, Citation2018) contributed to the formation of pure values among students, such as the willingness to behave, be polite, disciplined, and friendly (Muhammad & Bakar, Citation2018).

This is in line with Olivier et al. (Citation2020) study, which stated that internalization of pure values can directly enhance students’ prosocial behaviour. It further stated that the six aspects of internalization of pure values, namely appreciation for others, self-esteem, discipline, courtesy, bravery, and patriotism, are important aspects related to students’ prosocial behaviour.

This study is also in line with Tarip (Citation2020) opinion that the internalization of pure values and morals can be interpreted through thinking, behaviours, personal attributes, and conversations expressed from an individual’s conscience. It was also supported by Vansteenkiste et al. (Citation2018), which claimed that internalization of pure values is a barrier to becoming involved in participating in bad habits (Webster et al., Citation2021), and it involves other values such as humanitarian, spiritual, and citizenship (Idris, Citation2015).

CONCLUSION

This study analysed students’ prosocial behaviour through a process of Pure Values Education based on their religious beliefs and potentials. According to preliminary studies, Muslim students are unique, and their potential can be sharpened and strengthened for a brighter future. Therefore, the value of Islamic teaching and prosocial moral reasoning needs to be embedded in Pure Values Education in school due to its ability to influence students indirectly. This is also because Pure Values Education is a life-long education at the individual, social, environmental, civilization, and religious levels. The process of building dynamic and engaging student behaviour through Islamic education can lead individuals to unprecedented success as they grow.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Mohd Zailani Mohd Yusoff

Mohd Zailani Mohd Yusoff is a Associate Professor in School of Education, Universiti Utara Malaysia (UUM). His research expertise is Ethics and Moral Development, Moral Psychology, Religious Education, Islamic Education, Moral Education. He has published books and articles in Scopus journal and refereed journal. He has 20 years of teaching and research experience in Universiti Utara Malaysia.

Safrilsyah Safrilsyah

Safrilsyah is a Associate Professor in psychology Department, Ar-Raniry Islamic State University, Indonesia (UIN Ar-Raniry). His research expertise is Prosocial Behavior, Moral Psychology, Religious Education, Islamic Psychology. He has published books and articles in Scopus journal and refereed journal. He has 22 years of teaching and research experience in Ar-raniry Islamic State University.

Mohamad Khairi Haji Othman

Mohamad Khairi Haji Othman Is a Associate Professor in the Department of Science of Learning and Human Development, School of Education. He is holds the doctoral degree from Universiti Putra Malaysia in the field of Values and Islamic Education; Master of Education (Islamic Education) from Universiti Malaya; and Bachelor of Islamic Education also from Universiti Malaya. He is interested and involved in research, publication and consultancy works in the field of Islamic Education, Values/Moral Education, Pedagogy and Education Management. He has presented several papers in conferences at the national and international levels

Iwan Fajri

Iwan Fajri is a Research Assistant at the Department of Civic Education, Faculty of Teacher Training and Education, Syiah Kuala University, Indonesia. He is also a postgraduate student in Civic Education at the University of Education Indonesia. His current research focuses on civic education, moral education, youth engagement, digital citizenship, and 21st century skills

Sufriadi Muhammad Yusuf

Sufriadi Muhammad Yusuf is a research assistant at the Tarbiyah and Teacher Training Faculty, Ar-Raniry State Islamic University, Banda Aceh, Indonesia. Completed Ph.D in leadership in education, Universiti Utara Malaysia (UUM). Currently the focus of research is in the field of educational leadership in Islam, Islamic education management and Islamic religious education

Ibrahim Ibrahim

Ibrahim is an Associate Professor at the Department of Biology Education, Serambi Mekkah University Banda Aceh, Indonesia. His research expertise is curriculum and learning, biology education, teacher development and learning models. He is currently a lecturer at the Department of Biology Education, Faculty of Education, Serambi Mekkah University, Banda Aceh, Indonesia.

Wan Husna Wan Mohd Zain

Wan Husna Wan Mohd Zain is currently a teacher at SMK Puteri Titiwangsa, Kuala Lumpur. She completed her study in Bachelor of Education with Honors (Moral Education) and Master of Education Specialization in Values and Moral Education at Universiti Utara Malaysia (UUM).

References

  • Anyanwu, F. C., Akinsola, H. A., Tugli, A. K., & Obisie-Nmehielle, N. (2020). A qualitative assessment of the influence of family dynamics on adolescents’ sexual risk behaviour in a migration-affected community. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 15(1), 1717322. https://doi.org/10.1080/17482631.2020.1717322
  • Arsenio, W. (2014). Moral emotion attributions and aggression. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (pp. 235–255). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203581957.ch11.
  • Arslantürk, G., & Harput, D. (2020). Prosociality, religiosity and values in adolescence : Comparing the impact of religious and general schooling in Turkey. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/13617672.2020.1848150
  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research. Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
  • Bebeau, M. J., & Thoma, S. J. (1999). “ Intermediate “ Concepts and the Connection to Moral Education. Educational Psychology Review, 11(4), 343–360. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022057316180
  • BPS. (2015). Statistik Kriminal 2015 [Criminal Statistics 2015]. Badan Pusat Statistik. https://www.bps.go.id/indikator/indikator/view/0000/publication/43655fff07d2351f22255371/pub_06
  • Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., & Armenta, B. E. (2010). Feelings or cognitions? Moral cognitions and emotions as longitudinal predictors of prosocial and aggressive behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 872–877. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.010
  • Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2007). Research Methods in Education (7th ed.). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203029053-23
  • Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design : Qualitative, quantitative and mixed methods approaches. SAGE.
  • Dentici, O. A., & Pagnin, A. (1992). Moral Reasoning in Gifted Adolescents: Cognitive Level and Social Values. European Journal of High Ability, 3(1), 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/0937445920030111
  • Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., & Shepard, S. A. (2005). Age Changes in Prosocial Responding and Moral Reasoning in Adolescence and Early Adulthood. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15(3), 235–260. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00095.x
  • Eisenberg, N., & Shell, R. (1986). Prosocial Moral Judgment and Behavior in Children: The Mediating Role of Cost. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(4), 426–433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167286124005
  • Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Kupanoff, K., & Laible, D. (1999). Early Adolescence and Prosocial/Moral Behavior I : The Role of Individual Processes. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(1), 5–16. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431699019001001
  • Fang, Z., Jung, W. H., Korczykowski, M., Luo, L., Prehn, K., Xu, S., Detre, J. A., Kable, J. W., Robertson, D. C., & Rao, H. (2017). Post-conventional moral reasoning is associated with increased ventral striatal activity at rest and during task. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-07115-w
  • Gibbs, G. (2013). Reflections on the changing nature of educational development. International Journal for Academic, 18(1), 37–41. https://doi.org/10.1080/1360144X.2013.751691
  • Glasow, P. A. (2005). Fundamentals of Survey Research Methodology. Retrieved from http://www.uky.edu/~kdbrad2/EPE619/Handouts/SurveyResearchReading.pdf
  • Haan, N., Langer, J., & Kohlberg, L. (1976). Family Patterns of Moral Reasoning. Child Development, 47(4), 1204–1206. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128466
  • Hamzah, A. (2007). Satu kajian Skema Taakulan Akhlak dalam kalangan pelajar Melayu. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Penang, Malaysia: Universiti Sains Malaysia.
  • Hao, J., Liu, Y., & Cowell, J. M. (2016). A Mind-Reader Does Not Always Have Deontological Moral Judgments and Prosocial Behavior : A Developmental Perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(August), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01261
  • Hardy, S. A., & Carlo, G. (2005). in adolescence : The mediating role of prosocial values Religiosity and prosocial behaviours in adolescence : The mediating role of prosocial values. Journal of Moral Education, 34(2), 231–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240500127210
  • Hoffman, M. L. (2001). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press.
  • Hoxmeier, J. C., Mennicke, A., & McMahon, S. (2020). Bystander Intervention Opportunities and Prosocial Behaviors Among Gender and Sexual Minority College Students. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260520967131
  • Hui, T. S. (2007). Kepentingan Nilai-nilai Murni: Satu Tinjauan Di Kalangan pelajar-pelajar Tingkatan Satu, Dua Dan Empat di Sekolah Menengah Kebangsaan (laki-laki) Methodist sentul, Kuala Lumpur (Unpublished master thesis), Johor, Malaysia: Universiti Teknologi Malaysia.
  • Hur, Y., Jeong, H., Ajose, F., & Knafo-noam, A. (2019). Religious Attendance Moderates the Environmental E ff ect on Prosocial Behavior in Nigerian Adolescents. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 22(1), 42–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2018.71
  • Idris, S. (2015). The Internalization of democratic values into education and their relevance to islamic education development (synthetic, analytic, and eclectic implementation of John Dewey’s thoughts). Advanced Science Letters, 21(7), 2301–2304. https://doi.org/10.1166/asl.2015.6257
  • Jackson, D. B., Vaughn, M. G., & Kremer, K. P. (2019). Annals of Epidemiology Original article Bully victimization and child and adolescent health : New evidence from the 2016 NSCH. Annals of Epidemiology, 29, 60–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2018.09.004
  • Jaffee, S. R., Hanscombe, K. B., Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., & Plomin, R. (2012). Chaotic Homes and Children’s Disruptive Behavior: A Longitudinal Cross-Lagged Twin Study. Psychological Science, 23(6), 643–650. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611431693
  • Kavussanu, M., & Boardley, I. D. (2009). The prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31(1), 97–117. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.31.1.97
  • Keller, M., Lourenc, O., Malti, T., & Saalbach, H. (2003). The multifaceted phenomenon of ‘ happy victimizers ’: A cross-cultural comparison of moral emotions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151009X403838
  • King, P. M., & Mayhew, M. J. (2002). Moral Judgement Development in Defining Issues Test Moral Judgement Development in Higher Education : Insights from the De ning Issues Test. Journal of Moral Education, 31(3), 247–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/0305724022000008106
  • Klein, N. (2016). Prosocial behavior increases perceptions of meaning in life. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 12(4), 354–361. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1209541
  • Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development/2 The psychology of moral development. Harper & Row.
  • Koyanagi, A., Oh, H., Carvalho, A. F., Smith, L., Haro, J. M., Vancampfort, D., Stubbs, B., Devylder, J. E., & Asia, E. (2019). Bullying Victimization and Suicide Attempt Among Adolescents Aged 12-15 Years From 48 Countries. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(9), 907–918.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2018.10.018
  • Krause, N. (2003). Religious Meaning and Subjective Well-Being in Late Life. Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(3), 160–170. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.3.S160
  • Krettenauer, T., Asendorpf, J. B., & Nunner-winkler, G. (2013). Moral emotion attributions and personality traits as long-term predictors of antisocial conduct in early adulthood : Findings from a 20-year longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(3), 192–201. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025412472409
  • Kuem, J., Ray, S., Siponen, M., & Kim, S. S. (2017). What Leads to Prosocial Behaviors on Social Networking Services : A Tripartite Model What Leads to Prosocial Behaviors on Social Networking Services : A Tripartite Model. Journal of Management Information Systems, 34(1), 40–70. https://doi.org/10.1080/07421222.2017.1296744
  • Kumru, A., Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., & Samper, P. (2012). Prosocial moral reasoning and prosocial behavior among turkish and Spanish adolescents. Social Behavior and Personality, 40(2), 205–214. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2012.40.2.205
  • Laible, D. J., Murphy, T. P., & Augustine, M. (2014). Adolescents’ aggressive and prosocial behaviors: Links with social information processing, negative emotionality, moral affect, and moral cognition. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 175(3), 270–286. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2014.885878
  • Luo, J. (2018). The Neural Basis of and a Common Neural Circuitry in Different Types of Pro-social Behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(June), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00859
  • Ma, H. K. (2013). The moral development of the child : An integrated model. Frontiers in Public Health, 1(November), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpubh.2013.00057
  • Madsen, T. E., Linden, J. A., Rounds, K., Hsieh, Y.-H., Lopez, B. L., Boatright, D., Garg, N., Heron, S. L., Jameson, A., Kass, D., Lall, M. D., Melendez, A. M., Scheulen, J. J., Sethuraman, K. N., Westafer, L. M., Safdar, B., & Choo, E. K. (2017). Current Status of Gender and Racial/Ethnic Disparities Among Academic Emergency Medicine Physicians. Academic Emergency Medicine, 24(10), 1182–1192. https://doi.org/10.1111/acem.13269
  • Maeda, Y., Thoma, S. J., & Bebeau, M. J. (2009). Understanding the Relationship Between Moral Judgment Development and Individual Characteristics : The Role of Educational Contexts Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 233–247. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013381
  • Mahmood, A. (2006). Pengukuran tahap penghayatan pendidikan Islam pelajar-pelajar sekolah menengah di Malaysia. Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia Bangi.
  • Malti, T., & Buchmann, M. (2010). Socialization and individual antecedents of adolescents’ and young adults’ moral motivation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(2), 138–149. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9400-5
  • Malti, T., Gasser, L., & Buchmann, M. (2009). Aggressive and Prosocial Children ’ s Emotion Attributions and Moral Reasoning. Aggressive Behavior, 35(September 2008), 90–102. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20289
  • Malti, T., & Krettenauer, T. (2013). The Relation of Moral Emotion Attributions to Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Child Development, 84(2), 397–412. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01851.x
  • Malti, T., & Latzko, B. (2010). Children’s Moral Emotions and Moral Cognition: Towards an Integrative Perspective. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2010(129), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.272
  • Malti, T., & Ongley, S. (2014). The development of moral emotions and moral reasoning. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 163–183). New York: Psychology Press.
  • McCullough, M. E., & Willoughby, B. L. B. (2009). Religion, Self-Regulation, and Self-Control: Associations, Explanations, and Implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 69–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014213
  • Mercer, N., Crocetti, E., Branje, S., van Lier, P., & Meeus, W. (2017). Linking Delinquency and Personal Identity Formation Across Adolescence : Examining Between- and Within-Person Associations. Developmental Psychology, 53(11), 2182–2194. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000351
  • Milot, A. S., & Ludden, A. B. (2009). The effects of religion and gender on well-being, substance use, and academic engagement among rural adolescents. Youth & Society, 40(3), 403–425. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X08316668
  • Moulin-stożek, D., de Irala, J., Beltramo, C., & Osorio, A. (2018). Relationships between religion, risk behaviors and prosociality among secondary school students in Peru and El Salvador. Journal of Moral Education, 47(4), 466–480. https://doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2018.1438250
  • Muhammad, N. B. P. H., & Bakar, D. O. (2018). Implementation of the “Integrated Education System” in Brunei Darussalam: Issues and Challenges. Journal of Middle Eastern and Islamic Studies (In Asia), 7(4), 97–120. https://doi.org/10.1080/19370679.2013.12023234
  • Murk, D. A., & Addleman, J. A. (1992). Relations among moral reasoning, locus o f control, and demographic variables among college students. Psychological Reports, 70(2), 467–476. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1992.70.2.467
  • Myers, D. G. (2012). Reflections on religious belief and prosociality: Comment on Galen. Psychological Bulletin, 138(5), 913–917. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029009
  • Olivier, E., Morin, A. J. S., Langlois, J., Tardif-Grenier, K., & Archambault, I. (2020). Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems and Student Engagement in Elementary and Secondary School Students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(11), 2327–2346. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-020-01295-x
  • Othman, M., H., K., Suhid, A., & Roslan, S. (2015). Penghayatan Nilai Murni dalam kalangan Pelajar Sekolah Menengah Masa Kini. Jurnal Pembangunan Sosial, 18 (June), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.32890/jps.18.2015.11529
  • Penner, L. A. (2000). Promoting prosocial actions: The importance of culture and values. Journal of Social Philosophy, 31(4), 477–487. https://doi.org/10.1111/0047-2786.00068
  • Read, G. (2015). Gender, Religious Identity, and Civic Engagement among Arab Muslims in the United States. Sociology of Religion, 76(1), 30–48. https://doi.org/10.1093/socrel/sru042
  • Rest, J. R., & Thoma, S. J. (1985). Relation of moral judgment development to formal education. Developmental Psychology, 21(4), 709–714. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.4.709
  • Rest, J. R. (1984). Research on moral development: Implications for training counseling psychologists. The Counseling Psychologist, 12(3), 19–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000084123003
  • Rest, J. R. (1986). Moral development: Advances in research and theory. New York: Praeger.
  • Rest, J., Narvaez, D., Bebeau, M., & Thoma, S. (1999). A Neo-Kohlbergian Approach : The DIT and Schema Theory. Educational Psychology Review, 11(4), 291–324. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022053215271
  • Riva, K., Allen-taylor, L., Schupmann, W. D., Mphele, S., Moshashane, N., & Lowenthal, E. D. (2018). Prevalence and predictors of alcohol and drug use among secondary school students in Botswana : A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health, 18(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-018-6263-2
  • Roth, E. (2017). Pro-Social Behavior : Contributions of Religiosity, Empathic Concern, and Spirituality. International Journal of Latin American Religions, 1(2), 401–417. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41603-017-0024-3
  • Scal, P., Ireland, M., & Borowsky, I. W. (2003). Smoking among American adolescents: A risk and protective factor analysis. Journal of Community Health, 28(2), 79–97. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022691212793
  • Schulenberg, J., Johnston, L., O’Malley, P., Bachman, J., Miech, R., & Patrick, M. (2020). Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use, 1975-2019. College Students and Adults Ages, II, 19–60. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan. http://monitoringthefuture.org/pubs. html#monographs
  • Shahraki, G., Sedaghat, Z., & Fararouei, M. (2019). Family and social predictors of substance use disorder in Iran : A case-control study. Substance Abuse Treatment, Prevention, and Policy, 14(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13011-019-0201-x
  • Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God Is Watching You : Priming God Concepts Increases Prosocial Behavior in an Anonymous Economic Game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–809. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.x
  • Sng, O., Neuberg, S. L., Varnum, M. E. W., & Kenrick, D. T. (2017). The crowded life is a slow life: Population density and life history strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(5), 736–754. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000086
  • Strelan, P. (2007). Who forgives others, themselves, and situations? The roles of narcissism, guilt, self-esteem, and agreeableness. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(2), 259–269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2006.06.017
  • Stroebel, S. S., Keefe, S. L. O., Beard, K. W., Kuo, Y., Swindell, S. V. S., & Kommor, M. J. (2012). Father – Daughter Incest : Data from an Anonymous Computerized Survey Father – Daughter Incest : Data from an Anonymous Computerized Survey. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 21(2), 176–199. https://doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2012.654007
  • Suyanto, B., Aginta, M., Sugihartati, R., Ariadi, S., & Wadipalapa, R. P. (2019). Children and Youth Services Review Incestuous abuse of Indonesian girls : An exploratory study of media coverage. Children and Youth Services Review, 96(August 2018), 364–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.11.034
  • Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral Emotions and Moral Behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345–372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145
  • Tarip, I. (2020). Organizational moral learning by spiritual hearts: A synthesis of organizational learning, Islamic and critical realist perspectives. Asian Journal of Business Ethics, 9(2), 323–347. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13520-020-00112-9
  • Ugoji, F. N. (2014). Determinants of risky sexual behaviours among secondary school students in Delta State Nigeria. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 19(3), 408–418. https://doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2012.751040
  • Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., & Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial Behavior in Adolescence: Gender Differences in Development and Links with Empathy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(5), 1086–1099. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0786-1
  • Vansteenkiste, M., Aelterman, N., De Muynck, G. J., Haerens, L., Patall, E., & Reeve, J. (2018). Fostering Personal Meaning and Self-relevance: A Self-Determination Theory Perspective on Internalization. Journal of Experimental Education, 86(1), 30–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2017.1381067
  • Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2008). Prosocial and antisocial behavior in preadolescence: Teachers’ and parents’ perceptions of the behavior of girls and boys. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(3), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408089274
  • Vorlaufer, T. (2019). Effects of double-anonymity on pro- and anti-social behavior : Experimental evidence from a lab in the field. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 81(June), 216–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2019.07.001
  • Wang, C., Hipp, J. R., Butts, C. T., & Lakon, C. M. (2018). The interdependence of cigarette, alcohol, and marijuana use in the context of school- based social networks. PLoS ONE, 13(7), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0200904
  • Ward, S. J., & King, L. A. (2018). Personality and Individual Di ff erences Religion and moral self-image : The contributions of prosocial behavior, socially desirable responding, and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 131(April), 222–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.028
  • Webster, R. J., Morrone, N., Motyl, M., & Iyer, R. (2021). Using trait and moral theories to understand belief in pure evil and belief in pure good Using trait and moral theories to understand belief in pure evil and belief in pure good. Personality and Individual Differences, 173(April), 110584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110584
  • Weiner, M. D. (2002). Aggression and victimization as predictors of drug use in early adolescence. University of Southern California.
  • Widodo, H. P. (2018). A critical micro-semiotic analysis of values depicted in the Indonesian Ministry of National Education-endorsed secondary school English textbook. In H. P. Widodo, L. V. Canh, M. R. G. Perfecto, & A. Buripakdi. (Eds.).Situating moral and cultural values in ELT materials (pp. 131–152). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-63677-1
  • Wismayanti, Y. F., O’Leary, P., Tilbury, C., & Tjoe, Y. (2019). Child sexual abuse in Indonesia : A systematic review of literature, law and policy. Child Abuse & Neglect, 95(May), 104034. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.104034
  • Yang, Z., Fu, X., Yu, X., & Lv, Y. (2018). Longitudinal relations between adolescents ’ materialism and prosocial behavior toward family, friends, and strangers. Journal of Adolescence, 62(July 2017), 162–170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.11.013
  • Zaidin, N. B. M. (2015). Penerapan Nilai-Nilai Murni dalam Proses Pengajaran dan Pembelajaran ke arah Pembentukan Sahsiah Pelajar di Kolej Kemahiran Tinggi Mara. Universiti Tun Hussein Onn Malaysia.