907
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

A psychometric evaluation of a new social subscale for the Common Misconceptions about Traumatic Brain Injury (CM-TBI) questionnaire: toward the CM-TBI-II

&
Pages 1253-1261 | Received 26 Aug 2022, Accepted 27 May 2023, Published online: 31 Jul 2023

ABSTRACT

Objective

Existing TBI misconception measures are critiqued for failing to measure postinjury social experiences. This study developed a social subscale for the Common Misconceptions about TBI (CM-TBI) questionnaire for use in the general public.

Methods

Seven experts independently review items drawn from the literature. Shortlisted items were administered online to 158 adults (aged ≥18 years; 51% postschool educated; 60% no TBI experience), the CM-TBI, and a measure of construct validity (a published TBI-adaptation of the Community Attitudes Towards the Mentally Ill; CAMI-TBI). One week later, the new items were redeployed (n = 46).

Results

Expert review and iterative correlations identified a 10-item social subscale (internal consistency, test-retest reliability, α’s>.80). When added to the CM-TBI (ie. CM-TBI-II), the internal consistency was .71. The social subscale was significantly correlated with CAMI-TBI measures (p’s <.05, r’s > .3). There was no significant difference on the social subscale for education subgroups (school vs post-school, p = 0.056) or previous TBI experience; but there was a difference for the CM-TBI-II (post-school>school; Cohen’s d = 7.83, large effect).

Conclusion

This study found strong preliminary psychometric support for a new social subscale, administered as the CM-TBI-II. This subscale shows promise as a measure of misconceptions about social functioning post-TBI. The CM-TBI-II could support evaluations of programs aiming to improve social engagement and community participation for people with TBI.

Introduction

Traumatic brain injuries (TBI) have a high rate of incidence globally (Citation1) and occur after an external blow or sudden jolt to the head, commonly from motor vehicle accidents or falls (Citation2). TBI is classified as mild through severe depending on clinical indicators such as the duration of loss of consciousness or posttraumatic amnesia (Citation1). Although TBI outcomes are heterogeneous (Citation1), people’s social experiences can be significantly negatively affected following mild through severe injury (Citation3).

There are many ways in which the social experiences of an injured person can be negatively affected (Citation2,Citation4). For example, the ability to use or read social cues can be reduced after severe injury, and new social challenges can be encountered because of public misconceptions of about TBI. Persons with mild through severe TBI describe ongoing social isolation, disrupted, or altered relationships and sense of self, and negative evaluations by others (Citation5–8). There is growing recognition of the importance of improving social outcomes after TBI, and this could include a role for determining if the general public does in fact hold misconceptions about the social effects of TBI.

According to general (Citation9) and TBI-specific models of functioning (Citation4,Citation10,Citation11), when an injured person returns to the community after receiving medical care, their experience is shaped by their environment. This includes their access to social supports, and the availability of social interaction partners (such as employers, spouses, neighbors). The injured person may be able to access or receive social support from proximal (e.g., family) and distal sources (e.g., community, employer, service providers), and the nature of the social interactions with these sources will depend on the recovery stage (Citation12). Further, while different terminology is used to describe social experiences after TBI (Citation12), it is clear that community views about TBI and the capacities of the injured person (e.g., degree of social cognition impairments) can interact with each other. For example, residual TBI effects after more severe TBI, such as decreased emotion recognition, can be ‘invisible’ (Citation13–15), ‘non-visible’ (Citation16), or misunderstood by community members, leading to disadvantage and marginalization of the injured person (Citation4,Citation17–19).

Prior research has investigated community-held views about TBI and if these could affect social interactions with people with TBI or reduce the availability of social support experienced by them. Experimental studies show that community members are more likely to ascribe negative attributes to a person with a brain injury than they are to a person with a different (non-brain) injury (Citation19). In a study of 323 students and community members, community members were more likely than students to associate negative attributes (such as untrustworthiness) with brain injury (Citation20). If such views function to worsen social interactions and TBI outcomes as proposed (Citation4), there is the potential for change through community education.

The idea of improving outcomes for persons with TBI through increased community awareness about this injury is not new (Citation21); however, an expanded approach that considers public awareness of the social effects of TBI is now needed. Past studies have focused on public TBI education to improve injury identification and management, and increase awareness of prevention and rehabilitation strategies (Citation22,Citation23). This has included studying the knowledge gaps, including in high injury-risk groups and if misconceptions can be changed (Citation4). shows a list of existing tools used for this purpose and their main features. The existing measures are often specific in their focus and target populations, highlighting some limitations of these tools. Existing TBI misconception tools have been strongly critiqued on several grounds (Citation4) including that they are not conceptually grounded, there are item-scaling limitations, and some lack psychometric evaluations, especially during their development. Most importantly, these tools have been critiqued because they overlook social factors, such as injury visibility and community attitudes toward persons with TBI (Citation29,Citation32,Citation33).

Table 1. Overview of Common TBI Misconception Measures, Their Development Process and Psychometric Properties in Chronological Order.

Given that there is no existing TBI misconception measure with questions about social attitudes toward persons with TBI (Citation33), this study sought to address this gap. The purpose of this study was to develop and psychometrically evaluate an item set to be added to the most established, well-researched and widely used measure of TBI misconceptions: the Common Misconceptions about TBI questionnaire (CM-TBI). Guided by best practice recommendations for test development (Citation34) a multi-stage process was employed, including item identification via expert (health professional) consultation and literature review, and item selection with expert input (phase one). Phase two evaluated the new items when administered as an extension to CM-TBI. The phase two aim was to determine the psychometric properties of the new items and the extended test (CM-TBI-II), including through investigations of internal consistency, test retest-reliability, and construct validity.

Materials and Methods

Participants

16 TBI experts across several Australian states were approached for phase one. Experts were defined as health professionals who were published in TBI. This pool included people in multidisciplinary fields, including neuropsychology, occupational therapy, and neuroscience. The experts were individually approached by e-mail in September 2021 and asked to complete an online survey [Qualtrics™, Provo, UT]. Seven experts evaluated the new items (44% response rate). This number satisfies the best practice recommendation that an initial item pool should be reviewed by five to seven experts (Citation34).

Initially, two hundred and nineteen people commenced phase two. These participants were recruited by word of mouth through the researchers’ network, and via electronic distribution of project information to staff and students at a large metropolitan university. Project information was also disseminated to a research participation pool, and via a community volunteer database. Eligible participants were aged ≥18 years. After screening for compliance with the age requirement, and screening for ‘invalid’ responses (see screening process below), 158 participants completed session one of phase two (T1), and 46 participants returned for session two (T2); the retest survey, administered one week later.

Measures

Shortened Common Misconceptions about TBI (CM-TBI) and CM-TBI-II

The CM-TBI is a 40-item measure of TBI knowledge gaps with good psychometric properties (e.g (Citation26,Citation35). The CM-TBI records responses on a 4-point scale (true, probably true, probably false, false). Several variants have been used to study TBI misconceptions in various populations (Citation25,Citation36–41), including educational professionals (Citation42) and probation board members (Citation35). A 20-item (shortened) version was developed using principal components analysis, and this identified four subscales: recovery, sequelae, hidden injury, and insight (Citation26). This shortened variant assessed pediatric TBI and used a modified (5-point) Likert response scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree) with a ‘don’t know’ mid-point. An example item is: It is good advice to remain completely inactive during recovery from a brain injury. The 20-item version was subsequently used in a study of adult TBI (Citation39). For this study, we used the 20-item adult version (Citation39), with a new mid-point (‘neither agree nor disagree’) to improve scale continuity (Citation33). Items were also added to the measure, based on phase one. The added 13 items assessed beliefs about the social experience of persons with TBI, including attitudes about social functioning or behavior, and broader environmental factors (e.g., opportunity for community participation). After score reversals, a total agreement score was calculated (possible score range: 20–100), with higher scores indicating stronger misconceptions. Thus, the possible score range for the new items was 13 to 65, and for the expanded measure (i.e., new plus existing items [CM-TBI-II]), the possible score range was 33 to 165.

Community Attitudes toward the Mentally Ill (CAMI) – TBI modification (CAMI-TBI)

The CAMI is a well-established measure of community attitudes toward individuals with a mental illness (Citation43). A previously modified 40-item version of the CAMI was employed in this study as a measure of validity for the new items (Citation44). In this version the term ‘mental illness’ is replaced with ‘brain injury’ (i.e., CAMI-TBI) (Citation44). The CAMI-TBI has been used to measure outcomes from a school-based intervention for reducing stigmatization of acquired brain injury (Citation45). An example CAMI-TBI item is: Individuals with a brain injury are far less of a danger than most people suppose. The same 5-point agreement scale was used as for the CM-TBI-II. The CAMI-TBI has a multi-component structure (10 items per component), and in its original form, the subscales have satisfactory reliability [10]. The CAMI-TBI is scored by re-scaling the reversed items, calculating item ratings, and summing the item ratings for each of the four component scores (authoritarianism, social restrictiveness, benevolence and community ideology) (Citation44). Higher authoritarian and social restrictiveness scores are indicative of more unfavorable attitudes toward persons with TBI, whereas higher benevolence and community ideology signify more inclusive and supportive attitudes. The CAMI-TBI items from this study are available on request.

Previous experience with TBI

Previous experience with TBI was assessed with a single multiple-choice item focused on the most common form of injury (mild TBI). We asked: Do you have any previous experience with a mild traumatic brain injury? The three response options were converted to a binary score to create a group with (TBI-Experience) or without previous experience (TBI-No Experience). The group with experience was a composite of those who responded that they or someone they knew had experienced TBI.

Validity measures: Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale and Instructional

The 10-item Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale (M-C 1 (10)) was administered as a control for a potential confound (Citation46). This scale has a binary response format (true = 1, false = 2) and with reverse scoring of items with a ‘true’ response (Citation47). Total scores on the M-C 1(10) can range from 10–20, with higher scores indicating higher levels of socially desirable responding (Citation46,Citation47).

A three-item instructional manipulation check (IMC) was deployed at T1, and one IMC at T2. IMCs can provide an indication of invalidity (e.g., inattentiveness) in online surveys (Citation48,Citation49). IMCs tell the participant which response to select (e.g., ‘please select agree’), and are scored as correct or incorrect. A lower total IMC score indicates a higher risk of invalidity. At T1 an IMC cut off at least 50% correct was applied to identify valid protocols (low inattentiveness risk); at T2 the IMC cut off was 100% correct.

Procedure

This research was part of a larger study. Applications for ethical (identification number: 4414) and occupational health and safety clearance (identification number: 1782) were approved by the Queensland University of Technology (QUT). In phase one, the experts were told that the study aimed to modify the CM-TBI to include items about social functioning and community attitudes toward persons with TBI. They were shown the original CM-TBI and twenty-four pilot items identified by the authors from a review of literature examining the social experiences of persons with TBI and public misconceptions about TBI that might impact those social experiences (Citation6,Citation7,Citation20,Citation44,Citation50), plus related social attitude scales, such as the TBI adaptation of the Community Attitudes toward the Mentally Ill (Citation44). The pilot items were modelled to fit with existing CM-TBI items (e.g., by matching stems, where possible, and other key referents (e.g., injury terminology)). The pilot items were then evaluated by the experts (value-add, relevance, understandable, ambiguity) and rated (top 3 ‘worst’ and ‘best’ items, respectively), and they provided input at a granular- (e.g., is the item unambiguous?) and global- level (e.g., is a revised scale needed?). Experts’ views were also sought about the community attitudes toward TBI and any additional recommended items, and whether the pilot items provided adequate coverage of this aspect.

In phase two, the study was advertised with materials presented online (Qualtrics™). Survey access was controlled via an opt-in method (activating a link, scanning a quick response (QR) code). A full consent form was shown including information regarding risks and benefits of participation, privacy, and complaint procedures. A forced response was required to proceed (click ‘agree’) or exit (‘disagree’). Conditional logic directed consenting participants through the remaining questionnaire (non-forced responses). Participants generated a unique identifier to link anonymized responses, then answered demographic questions, followed by the CM-TBI-II, and the CAMI-TBI (with one embedded IMC). The CM-TBI-II was administered in two fixed blocks, each with one embedded IMC, plus the new items (block 1), or the original CM-TBI items (block 2). One week later an automated e-mail with a T2 survey link comprising block 1 items was sent to participants who requested it. Exiting participants either quit the study immediately, or after they entered their contact details into a separate database held for administrative purposes (e.g., token distribution). Two tokens of appreciation were offered: a prize draw entry (one chance to win one of two $AUD50 gift cards) or 1% academic credit, if eligible. 78 participants received a study summary and two participants received prizes (drawn May 2022).

Data analysis

The data were transferred for analysis into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (IBM SPSS Statistics version 28). The phase one results (item- and scale-evaluations) were examined using descriptive statistics, with counts employed in item decisions (e.g., retention, revision, removal). The qualitative results were used for item refinement. The phase two data was screened for missing results (>10%) and patterns (Little’s MCAR, p’s < .05), and invalid attempts. The social desirability scores on the M-C 1(10) were examined with reference to conventional standards (Citation48,Citation49) and did not indicate socially desirable responding. Intercorrelations between new item pairs were inspected to explore interrelationships (Citation51). Based on precedent (Citation34), potentially problematic items were flagged using an a priori standard (i.e., bivariate intercorrelation < .3). Internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) was explored for the CM-TBI, new item set, and the CM-TBI-II. A one-week test-retest intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was generated for the new items (Citation52). Validity was examined through the CM-TBI-II and CAMI-TBI intercorrelations. Independent-samples t-tests examined differences in CM-TBI-II scores for people based on previous education and experience of TBI, respectively, and effect size was calculated with Cohen’s d. A p-value of .05 was used for statistical significance, unless otherwise stated.

Results

Phase 1: Expert review

The expert feedback was tabulated and descriptive statistics calculated where appropriate. Thirteen items were retained because six or more experts deemed them value-adding or relevant to community attitudes. For the eleven items that were removed, three or more experts did not rate them highly (low value-add or relevance), or they received at least two votes as the ‘worst’ item from the pool. A selection of the qualitative feedback is shown in . This table shows how the feedback was used, including via a cross-check with the empirical literature. Out of a maximum score of 5, the average importance of assessing the social aspects of TBI was 4.43 (SD = 0.49) [very important] and average the item-domain fit was 3.86 (SD = 0.35) [very well].

Table 2. Summary of the item development process showing use of the literature and expert feedback to form and refine new CM-TBI-II items.

Phase 2: Community sample

The flowchart in explains the participant selection, and displays the sample characteristics (N = 158). The average age of the participants was approximately 29 years (SD = 15). Seventy-eight percent of the sample (n = 124) identified as female and 41% self-reported previous experience with TBI (either personally or indirectly, n = 64). Approximately half of the sample had completed their school education (n = 77) with the other half having also completed a post-school qualification (n = 81).

Figure 1. The selection of community participants across two data collection periods. Notes: T1 = time 1; T2 = time 2. All duplicate attempts were removed from the dataset. § Duplicate attempts screened first, with removal f all attempts by a duplicate responder.** Evaluated via Instructional Manipulation Checks (IMC)s. At T1, participants excluded for fail ure on two or more out of three IMCs. At T2, participants were excluded if the on e IMC was failed. † Unlinked codes included 12 novel T2 codes, and 6 codes that could not be linked after T1 screening.

Figure 1. The selection of community participants across two data collection periods. Notes: T1 = time 1; T2 = time 2. All duplicate attempts were removed from the dataset. § Duplicate attempts screened first, with removal f all attempts by a duplicate responder.** Evaluated via Instructional Manipulation Checks (IMC)s. At T1, participants excluded for fail ure on two or more out of three IMCs. At T2, participants were excluded if the on e IMC was failed. † Unlinked codes included 12 novel T2 codes, and 6 codes that could not be linked after T1 screening.

Table 3. Characteristics of the main study sample.

CM-TBI-II-Social subscale: item reduction

Intercorrelations and reliability analyses were performed on the new CM-TBI-II social subscale items. This information was used to identify items with weak inter-item associations (<. 3) (Citation34) and/or whether their removal would increase the scale alpha. An iterative process was used to remove items until the values in the intercorrelation matrix approximated or exceeded .3 and the reliability analysis showed that the removal of further items would not significantly increase the scale alpha. Three items were excluded in this process. The intercorrelations for the remaining 10 CM-TBI-II items are shown in (lower diagonal). These correlations ranged from .13 (small) to .55 (medium (Citation54)), and all were statistically significant bar one correlation. The final set of items had an alpha of .82 at T1 and .87 at T2 ().

Table 4. Intercorrelations (Pearson’s R) for CM-TBI-II Block 1 (new) items, 10-item social subscale (lower diagonal), and between the CM-TBI-II social subscale and CAMI-TBI subscales (upper diagonal).

Table 5. Descriptive Statistics, Reliability (Internal Consistency), and Group Differences (School versus Post-School) for CM-TBI-II and CAMI-TBI Summary Scores.

CM-TBI-II Internal Consistency and Test-Retest Reliability of the CM-TBI-Social subscale

shows the scale reliability for CM-TBI-II subscale scores. Adding the new items to the original CM-TBI did not substantially change the scale’s internal consistency (.71 CM-TBI-II vs .72 CM-TBI) nor alter its acceptability (Citation55). The CM-TBI-II social subscale stability was examined using a one-week test-retest correlation for the new CM-TBI-II items. The test-retest reliability (ICC) for the new subscale was .83 (95% CI [.68, .91], ).

CM-TBI-II validity

The validity of the CM-TBI-II social subscale was examined through intercorrelations with existing CM-TBI subscales, and the CAMI-TBI subscales, respectively. shows selected correlations for the CM-TBI-II social subscale and CAMI-TBI subscales (upper diagonal). The new CM-TBI-II social subscale was significantly correlated (p < 0.05) with one existing CM-TBI subscale (i.e., sequalae, r = −.37), and with all CAMI-TBI subscales. The CM-TBI-II social and CAMI-TBI subscales were moderately correlated (r = .32 [benevolence] to − .46 [social restrictiveness], all p’s < .01). These correlations were larger than the correlations between the new CM-TBI-II subscale and existing CM-TBI subscales.

Misconceptions in the community and the effect of individual differences on item and summary scores

Measures of central tendency and dispersion for the average agreement on CM-TBI-II and CAMI-TBI subscales are shown in (full sample and grouped by education). A series of repeated measures t-tests compared the average score on the CM-TBI-II social subscale (M = 3.85, SD = .50), paired with an existing subscale (M = 3.17 to 4.17). The mean score for the social subscale was significantly higher than the sequalae and insight subscales (p’s<.001), and significantly lower than the recovery (p=0.024) and insight subscales (p < 0.001).

Independent-samples t-tests were used to examine education- or experience-related group differences on the CM-TBI-II. There was no significant difference based on previous or no previous TBI experience for any item, subscale, or total score (all p’s > 0.05). There were education group differences, but this depended on the score type (item, subscale or total), and when observed they were in the same direction (post-school > school). Specifically, education-group differences were found at the item-level for two new (social) items (Item 1 and 7) and four original items (Items 2, 7, 11 and 14; all ps < .05, ds >.8, large effects (Citation54)). Subscale differences were not found, with the exception of a trend for the social subscale, school: M = 37.75, SD = 5.32 vs post-school: M = 39.28, SD = 4.67, t(156)= −1.92, p = 0.056, 95% CI [−3.10, .04]. There was no effect of education on the CM-TBI total score t(156)= −1.40, p=0.164, 95% CI [−3.41, .59]; but there was an effect on the CM-TBI-II total score, school: M = 111.36, SD = 7.32 vs post-school education: M = 114.31, SD = 8.29, t(156)= −2.36, p=0.019, 95% CI [−5.41, −.48], d = 7.83. On average, participants with a post-high school education demonstrated more accurate TBI knowledge and scored 2.95 points higher on the CM-TBI-II than participants with a high school education.

Discussion

This study responds to a call from the field for a revised measure of TBI misconceptions that considers the social experience post TBI (Citation33). The capacity to evaluate and change misconceptions about the social experience of persons with TBI has arguably been limited by the absence of a suitable tool for this purpose (Citation12). Consistent with this idea (Citation12), none of our experts knew of a measure for this purpose, but all recognized this need as very important. Because existing TBI misconceptions measures have also been critiqued for insufficient psychometric evaluation when under development (Citation33), the current study aimed to build and psychometrically evaluate a ‘social extension’ for an existing misconceptions measure with known properties (i.e., the CM-TBI).

To establish a basis for comparison, this study first replicated the psychometric properties for the existing CM-TBI subscales. Consistent with past reports, this study found acceptable (>.70) internal consistency for some CM-TBI scores (total score, recovery subscale (Citation26,Citation35)); but not for others (Insight (Citation35)). O’Rourke et al (Citation35). found that the sequalae subscale was internally consistent (α = .81), whereas this result was not replicated (α = .69). The new social subscale had an internal consistency of .81, and when added to the CM-TBI – to create the CM-TBI-II – the alpha for the CM-TBI-II was acceptable (above .70) (Citation34). Importantly, the new social subscale also had acceptable one-week test-retest reliability (>.80) signifying stable measurement. Taken together, this investigation suggests that the existing CM-TBI subscales could be further developed (for example via item addition for some subscales), and the new scale performed relatively well.

The relationship between attitudes toward social aspects of TBI, and existing TBI misconceptions was explored. If the new scale is a valid measure, it should be related to existing misconceptions. These patterns were indeed evident; for example, there was a significant positive correlation between the social and recovery subscales, suggesting that with increased knowledge of the social effects of injury, there is increased knowledge of general TBI recovery. However, social knowledge was not significantly correlated with other misconceptions (insight or hidden injury), and further, it was significantly negatively correlated with an understanding of TBI sequalae. This significant negative correlation could indicate that prosocial attitudes toward a person with TBI can be high, even when symptoms are not fully comprehended (Citation33). Overall, while these CM-TBI-II subscale analyses require cautious interpretation due to the low internal consistency of the existing subscales, it is promising that the new social subscale was significantly associated with the most reliable of the existing subscales. A suggestion for future research is to trial the new subscale in the full 40-item CMI-TBI, or in a modification of the shortened version with added or improved items for the two weakest CM-TBI subscales (hidden injury, insight).

The pattern of correlations between the CM-TBI-II social subscale and CAMI-TBI subscales was explored to test for construct validity. Specifically, this determined if the new items were related to conceptually-similar items from a measure of community attitudes toward people with TBI (Citation44). Consistent with expectations, there were significant, moderate, positive correlations between the new social subscale and two of the four CAMI-TBI subscales (Benevolence and Community Ideology) and significant negative correlations with the others (Authoritarianism and Social Restrictiveness). The negative correlations suggest that stronger prosocial TBI attitudes are associated with weaker attitudes about the control of, or social restrictions for, persons with TBI (i.e., lower stigma and discrimination). The positive correlations suggest that as prosocial attitudes increase so too do attitudes that reflect a humanistic view of TBI and a valuing of community-integrated supports (i.e., higher social acceptance and understanding). This suggests that prosocial attitudes, such as a willingness to engage with a person with TBI, extends the support for community-integrated services and a more benevolent injury appraisal. Overall, these results support the hypothesized relations between the new subscale and an established measure, thus displaying convergent validity.

As a further validation, this study tested for differences in TBI misconceptions based on education (school versus post-school) or TBI experience (previous vs no previous TBI experience) since studies have shown that knowledge can be influenced by such factors (Citation4). Contrary to expectations, this study did not find a statistical difference in TBI misconceptions due to experience; but it did find an effect for education. The experience effect is inconsistent with a prior study that showed that TBI misconceptions were reduced by previous TBI experience (Citation35). However, on closer inspection of that prior study, the experience effect was quite limited (i.e., effects evident on two of four subscales [sequalae, hidden injury], and one of two measures (‘having knowledge of someone with a brain injury’ but not having ‘experience of working with a person with a brain injury,’ p.1121) (Citation35)). Taken together, the role of TBI experience on misconceptions – including those measured via existing subscales – is not clear, potentially because of different measures.

Consistent with the education hypothesis, this study found an effect of general education on the CM-TBI-II total score (Cohen’s d = 7.83, large effect). This is consistent with past studies showing that higher levels of education are associated with fewer TBI misconceptions (Citation4,Citation38,Citation56). However, an education effect was not found for any of the subscales (albeit that, the difference approached significance for the new subscale, p= 0.056). This finding could temper conclusions about subscale validity or interpretability and requires replication. If the result stands, it suggests that formal education is unlikely to provide an avenue for supporting people to hold more accurate views about social functioning post-TBI, and that other strategies will be needed.

This study has several limitations. Online surveys carry a risk of bias, as do attitude surveys (e.g., socially desirable responding (Citation48,Citation49)). To address this issue, this study employed data quality checks and excluded cases for reasons such as inattentive responding. Despite this, the findings may carry a risk of bias. Second, these findings have limited generalizability because the sample was small (especially at retest), unrepresentative of the community (comprised of mostly women volunteers, with almost half of the sample holding a post-school qualification), and some effects were small. In a sample that is more representative of the general public, it is speculated that additional misconceptions would be revealed. Third, while the psychometrics for the new scale were promising, the comparative measures and their subscales (the gold standard) have known weaknesses, some of which were replicated in this study. The CAMI-TBI has only been used in a handful of studies, and the validity and interpretability of CM-TBI-II scores (total and subscale) requires further investigation in a larger study with structural analyses (e.g., factor analysis). Until this occurs, it cannot be concluded that the new subscale taps into a unique dimension or that it can be interpreted alone. Future studies can build on this research to address these limitations, such as employing alternate measures of a range of variables, including TBI experience. Future research should also investigate community attitudes about social functioning in people with mild versus severe TBI.

The social experience of persons with TBI can be debilitating and can reduce community participation (Citation6,Citation7). Modelling suggests that many factors contribute to this problem, including the role played by the community via beliefrs or attitudes they may hold (Citation4,Citation10). This study filled a gap in the literature through the development of a social extension for a well-established measure of community misconceptions about TBI. If this measure is deployed in future research it could assist in understanding community views about the social engagement of persons with TBI. Further, it could prove a useful tool for the evaluation of programs aimed at improving the social experience and community participation of persons with TBI. While additional testing is needed for the social subscale developed in this study, including via independent replication and extension, the findings strongly suggest that this new subscale has promising psychometric properties.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank study participants for volunteering their time to participate in this study, and the expert reviewers who kindly contributed feedback on the initial items. This project was approved by the (HREC approval number: A health and safety risk assessment for this project was approved by (approval number:). Both authors contributed to the conception, execution, and write up of this research. The funded the gift cards for this project.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

The author(s) reported there is no funding associated with the work featured in this article.

References

  • Nguyen R, Fiest KM, McChesney J, Kwon CS, Jette N, Frolkis AD, Atta C, Mah S, Dhaliwal H, Reid A, et al. The international incidence of traumatic brain Injury: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Can J Neurol Sci. 2016;43(6):774–85. doi:10.1017/cjn.2016.290.
  • Polinder S, Cnossen MC, Real RGL, Covic A, Gorbunova A, Voormolen DC, Master CL, Haagsma JA, Diaz-Arrastia R, von Steinbuechel N. A multidimensional approach to post-concussion symptoms in mild traumatic brain injury. Front Neurol. 2018;9(1113). doi:10.3389/fneur.2018.01113.
  • Temkin NR, Corrigan JD, Dikmen SS, Machamer J. Social functioning after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma Rehab. 2009;24(6):460–67. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181c13413.
  • Block CKP, West SEP, Goldin YP. Misconceptions and misattributions about traumatic brain injury: An integrated conceptual framework. Phys Med Reh. 2016;8(1):58–68.e4. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2015.05.022.
  • Hennessy MJ, Sullivan KA. A ‘network of understanding and compassion’: A qualitative study of survivor perspectives on unmet needs after traumatic brain injury (TBI) in regional communities. Brain Impair. 2021;24(1):1–12. doi:10.1017/BrImp.2021.18.
  • Roscigno CI, Van Liew K. Pushed to the margins and pushing back: a case study of one adult’s reflections on social interactions after a traumatic brain injury sustained as an adolescent. J Neurosci Nurs. 2008;40(4):212–21. doi:10.1097/01376517-200808000-00005.
  • Linden MA, Boylan AM. ‘To be accepted as normal’: Public understanding and misconceptions concerning survivors of brain injury. Brain Inj. 2010;24(4):642–50. doi:10.3109/02699051003601689.
  • Freeman A, Adams M, Ashworth F. An exploration of the experience of self in the social world for men following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2014;25(2):1–27. doi:10.1080/09602011.2014.917686.
  • Üstün TB, Chatterji S, Bickenbach J, Kostanjsek N, Schneider M. The International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: A new tool for understanding disability and health. Disabil Rehabil. 2003;25(11–12):565–71. doi:10.1080/0963828031000137063.
  • Kenzie ES, Parks EL, Bigler ED, Lim MM, Chesnutt JC, Wakeland W. Concussion as a multi-scale complex system: An interdisciplinary synthesis of current knowledge. Front Neurol. 2017;8(513):513. doi:10.3389/fneur.2017.00513.
  • Ownsworth T, McKenna K. Investigation of factors related to employment outcome following traumatic brain injury: a critical review and conceptual model. Disabil Rehabil. 2004;26(13):765–83. doi:10.1080/09638280410001696700.
  • Sullivan KA. Recovery after traumatic brain injury: An integrative review of the role of social factors on postinjury outcomes. Appl Neuropsychol-Adul. 2022;1–8. doi:10.1080/23279095.2022.2070021.
  • Childers C, Hux K. Invisible injuries: The experiences of college students with histories of mild traumatic brain injury. J Postsecondary Educ Disabil. 2016;29(4):389–405.
  • McKinlay A, McLellan T, Daffue C, Patrick PD, Savage RC. The invisible brain injury: The importance of identifying deficits following brain injury in children with intellectual disability. Neuro Rehabil. 2012;30(3):183–87. doi:10.3233/NRE-2012-0743.
  • Dams-O’Connor K, Landau A, Hoffman J, St De Lore J. Patient perspectives on quality and access to healthcare after brain injury. Brain Inj. 2018;32(4):431–41. doi:10.1080/02699052.2018.1429024.
  • United Kingdom Government CO, Disability Unit Living with Non-Visible Disabilities United Kingdom (UK): UK Government; 2020 [accessed 2022 June 3]. https://disabilityunit.blog.gov.uk/2020/12/17/living-with-non-visible-disabilities/.
  • McClure J. The role of causal attributions in public misconceptions about brain injury. Rehabil Psychol. 2011;56(2):85–93. doi:10.1037/a0023354.
  • Ralph A, Derbyshire C. Survivors of brain injury through the eyes of the public: A systematic review. Brain Inj. 2013;27(13–14):1475–91. doi:10.3109/02699052.2013.823653.
  • McLellan T, Bishop A, McKinlay A. Community attitudes toward individuals with traumatic brain injury. J Int Neuropsych Soc. 2010;16(4):705–10. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000524.
  • Linden MA, Crothers IR. Violent, caring, unpredictable: Public views on survivors of brain injury. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2006;21(8):763–70. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.08.010.
  • Caron JG, Bloom GA, Falcão WR, Sweet SN. An examination of concussion education programmes: A scoping review methodology. Inj Prev. 2015;21(5):301–08. doi:10.1136/injuryprev-2014-041479.
  • Mallory KD, Saly L, Hickling A, Colquhoun H, Kroshus E, Reed N. Concussion education in the school setting: A scoping review. J School Health. 2022;92(6):605–18. doi:10.1111/josh.13156.
  • Mylabathula S, Macarthur C, Guttmann A, Colantonio A, Tator C. Development of a concussion public policy on prevention, management and education for schools using expert consensus. Inj Prev. 2022;28(5):453–58. doi:10.1136/injuryprev-2021-044395.
  • Gouvier WD, Prestholdt PH, Warner MS. A survey of common misconceptions about head injury and recovery. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 1988;3(4):331–43. doi:10.1093/arclin/3.4.331.
  • Springer JA, Parmer JE, Bouman DE. Common misconceptions about traumatic brain injury among family members of rehabilitation patients. J Head Trauma Rehab. 1997;12(3):41–50. doi:10.1097/00001199-199706000-00005.
  • Linden MA, Braiden H-J, Miller S. Educational professionals’ understanding of childhood traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2013;27(1):92–102. doi:10.3109/02699052.2012.722262.
  • Hux K, Schram CD, Goeken T. Misconceptions about brain injury: A survey replication study. Brain Inj. 2006;20(5):547–53. doi:10.1080/02699050600676784.
  • Ono M, Ownsworth T, Walters B. Preliminary investigation of misconceptions and expectations of the effects of traumatic brain injury and symptom reporting. Brain Inj. 2011;25(2):237–49. doi:10.3109/02699052.2010.541893.
  • Rosenbaum AM, Arnett PA. The development of a survey to examine knowledge about and attitudes toward concussion in high-school students. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2010;32(1):44–55. doi:10.1080/13803390902806535.
  • Sefton JM An examination of factors that influence knowledge of and reporting of head injuries in college football [Unpublished Master’s thesis]: Central Connecticut State University; 200.
  • Simonds CB Development of a questionnaire to assess knowledge and attitudes about concussion and return to play criteria in college athletes [Doctoral dissertation]: LaSalle University; 2004.
  • Oyesanya TO, Turkstra LS, Brown RL. Development, reliability, and validity of the Perceptions of Brain Injury Survey. J Nurs Meas. 2020;28(2):229–58. doi:10.1891/JNM-D-19-00007.
  • Bryant E, Williams C, Horry R, Worthington A. Measuring misconceptions about traumatic brain injury: Are existing scales misconceived? Brain Inj. 2020;34(9):1150–58. doi:10.1080/02699052.2020.1795721.
  • Boateng GO, Neilands TB, Frongillo EA, Melgar-Quiñonez HR, Young SL. Best practices for developing and validating scales for health, social, and behavioral research: A primer. Front Pub Health. 2018;6. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2018.00149.
  • O’Rourke C, Linden MA, Lohan M. Misconceptions about traumatic brain injury among probation services. Disabil Rehabil. 2018;40(10):1119–26. doi:10.1080/09638288.2017.1288274.
  • Michelle W, James D. Knowledge of mild traumatic brain injury: Effects of age, locality, occupation, media and sports participation. Front Psychol. 2015;6. doi:10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2015.66.00001.
  • Gurusamy J, Gandhi S, Amudhan S, Veerabhadraiah KB, Narayanasamy P, Sreenivasan ST, Palaniappan M. Misconceptions about traumatic brain injury among nursing students in India: Implications for nursing care and curriculum. BMC Nurs. 2019;18(1):64–. doi:10.1186/s12912-019-0388-1.
  • Pappadis MR, Sander AM, Struchen MA, Leung P, Smith DW. Common misconceptions about traumatic brain injury among ethnic minorities with TBI. J Head Trauma Rehab. 2011;26(4):301–11. doi:10.1097/HTR.0b013e3181e7832b.
  • Whiting DL, Chuah SL, Simpson GK, Deane FP, Reynolds J. Video-consulting to address mental health needs after traumatic brain injury: evaluation of a training workshop to build capacity among psychologists. Brain Inj. 2021;35(9):1065–74. doi:10.1080/02699052.2021.1953594.
  • Ernst WJ, Gallo AB, Sellers AL, Mulrine J, MacNamara L, Abrahamson A, Kneavel M. Knowledge of traumatic brain injury among educators. Exceptionality. 2016;24(2):123–36. doi:10.1080/09362835.2015.1107832.
  • Schellinger SK, Munson B, Kennedy MRT. Public perceptions of traumatic brain injury: predictors of knowledge and the effects of education. Brain Inj. 2018;32(11):1377–85. doi:10.1080/02699052.2018.1492737.
  • McKinlay A, Buck K. Misconceptions about traumatic brain injury among educators: Has anything changed over the last 20 years? Disabil Rehabil. 2019;41(12):1419–26. doi:10.1080/09638288.2018.1429500.
  • Taylor SM, Dear MJ. Scaling community attitudes toward the mentally ill. Schizophrenia Bull. 1981;7(2):225–40. doi:10.1093/schbul/7.2.225.
  • Linden MA, Rauch RJ, Crothers IR. Public attitudes towards survivors of brain injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19(12):1011–17. doi:10.1080/02699050500110314.
  • Irwin LG, Fortune DG. Schools-based interventions for reducing stigmatization of acquired brain injury: the role of interpersonal contact and visible impairment. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2014;29(2):194–205. doi:10.1093/arclin/act118.
  • Strahan R, Gerbasi KC. Short, homogenous versions of the Marlow [sic]-Crowne Social Desirability scale. J Clin Psychol. 1972;28(2):191–93. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197204)28:2<191:AID-JCLP2270280220>3.0.CO;2-G.
  • Crowne DP, Marlowe D. A new scale of social desirability independent of psychopathology. J Consult Psychol. 1960;24(4):349–54. doi:10.1037/h0047358.
  • Oppenheimer DM, Meyvis T, Davidenko N. Instructional manipulation checks: Detecting satisficing to increase statistical power. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2009;45(4):867–72. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.03.009.
  • McKibben WB, Silvia PJ. Evaluating the distorting effects of inattentive responding and social desirability on self‐report scales in creativity and the arts. J Creat Behav. 2017;51(1):57–69. doi:10.1002/jocb.86.
  • McKinlay A, Bishop A, McLellan T. Public knowledge of ‘concussion’ and the different terminology used to communicate about mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI). Brain Inj. 2011;25(7–8):761–66. doi:10.3109/02699052.2011.579935.
  • Norman G. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the “laws” of statistics. Adv Health Sci Educ. 2010. doi:10.1007/s10459-010-9222-y.
  • Leppink J, Pérez-Fuster P. We need more replication research – a case for test-retest reliability. Perspectives Med Educ. 2017;6(3):158–64. doi:10.1007/s40037-017-0347-z.
  • Humphries TJ, Ingram S, Sinha S, Lecky F, Dawson J, Singh R. The effect of socioeconomic deprivation on 12 month Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) outcome. Brain Inj. 2020;34(3):343–49. doi:10.1080/02699052.2020.1715481.
  • Schäfer T, Schwarz MA. The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: Differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases. Front Psychol. 2019;10. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00813.
  • Taber KS. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res Sci Ed. 2017;48(6):1273–96. doi:10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2.
  • Merz ZC, Van Patten R, Lace J. Current public knowledge pertaining to traumatic brain injury: Influence of demographic factors, social trends, and sport concussion experience on the understanding of traumatic brain injury sequelae. Arch Clin Neuropsych. 2017;32(2):155–67. doi:10.1093/arclin/acw092.