Publication Cover
Sociological Spectrum
Mid-South Sociological Association
Volume 37, 2017 - Issue 2
10,770
Views
32
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Why are Depressive Symptoms More Prevalent Among The Less Educated? The Relevance of Low Cultural Capital and Cultural Entitlement

, &

ABSTRACT

Analyzing nationally representative survey data collected in the United States in 2014 (n = 1,932), we formulate and test a novel explanation for the educational gradient in depressive symptoms. We theorize that status as cultural capital drives this gradient in addition to well-established economic and social factors, via the feelings of cultural entitlement it inspires. Therefore, we use structural equation modeling to decompose the education effect. We demonstrate that in addition to economic (job security and income) and social factors (embeddedness in the neighborhood), cultural capital indeed accounts for the educational gradient in depressive symptoms via cultural entitlement. We conclude that for understanding social gradients in mental health it is vital to be sensitive for the cultural mechanisms that status as cultural capital can inspire. Based on our findings we propose suggestions for further research.

Introduction

It is clear that the study of depression does not exclusively belong to the domain of psychology: Because of the pronounced social stratification of depressive symptoms, it is a popular theme in sociology as well (see, e.g., Petersen Citation2011). The level of education especially plays a significant role in processes that affect mental health (Bjelland et al. Citation2008; Bracke, Pattyn, and Von dem Knesebeck Citation2013). Several studies have established that the more educated experience fewer depressive symptoms than the less educated (Bauldry Citation2015; Bjelland et al. Citation2008; for a meta-analysis, see Lorant et al. Citation2003), and a majority of longitudinal studies (e.g., Johnson et al. Citation1999) indicates that more education leads to less depressive symptoms, rather than vice versa. Scholars have thus acknowledged education as a “fundamental cause” of (mental) health disparities (Link and Phelan Citation1995; Pampel Citation2009). While, evidently, there has been a lot of attention on educational differences in health outcomes, the association still requires further explication (e.g., Bauldry Citation2015; Van Kippersluis et al. Citation2010).

As indicated by several authors, existing explanations traditionally focus on the economic privileges of the higher educated (e.g., Mirowsky and Ross Citation1998; Pampel, Krueger, and Denney Citation2010). In addition to the well-established economic causes for the mental health advantage of the more educated, it is argued that social factors also play an important role. Kawachi et al. (Citation1997), for instance, found that higher levels of educational attainment are positively associated with being embedded in cohesive social structures, and the mental health benefits of this embeddedness are widely acknowledged (see, e.g., Ferlander Citation2007). For a sociological understanding of the educational gradient in depressive symptoms it is therefore important to consider social factors next to economic ones. However, a sociological approach could make an additional contribution by focusing on the role of status as cultural capital (henceforth, cultural capital). Cultural capital indicates one’s affinity with elite culture, and level of education is one of its primary indicators (Bourdieu, Citation1984; Bourdieu and Passeron, Citation1977).

Building on earlier suggestions of the relevance of cultural capital for mental health issues (Abel, Citation2008; Pinxten and Lievens, Citation2014), we formulated and empirically tested a specific mechanism through which cultural capital could contribute to a better understanding of the relationship between education and depression. In short, we argue that an individual’s cultural capital shapes a sense of “cultural entitlement,” which entails a sense of being a relevant and legitimate citizen who matters in society. This is, for instance, illustrated by research by Spruyt, Van Droogenbroeck and Kavadias (Citation2015), who showed that individuals with lower levels of education more often experience a sense of futility. They feel, moreover, less entitled to participate in legitimate culture and institutions (Lamont, Beljean and Clair Citation2014; Laurison Citation2015), disenfranchised, and excluded (Myles Citation2008). As we explain in this article, such a low sense of cultural entitlement not only is shaped by educational level as an indicator for cultural capital but also could have important consequences for individual mental health.

By proposing a mechanism based on cultural entitlement, we aim to contribute to existing knowledge on the relationship between education and depression and provide an empirical basis for a novel, sociological interpretation of how cultural capital is relevant to depression. After explicating how this interpretation adds to existing notions of the role of economic and social factors, we empirically test hypotheses about the role of economic position, embeddedness in a cohesive social structure, and status as cultural capital using nationally representative survey data collected in the United States in 2014.

Positioning the role of status as cultural capital next to economic and social factors

Economic position

The positive relationship between education level and economic indicators such as income and employment is both well known and well documented (e.g., Psacharopoulos Citation2014). The resulting variations in economic circumstances can, in turn, be related to health inequalities, as Wilkinson and Pickett (Citation2010) pointed out.

Education may, for instance, influence depression through income. As Marmot and Wilkinson (Citation2001) highlighted, individual income can influence general health in two ways. First, income is directly related to health through the material conditions and the resources it provides to secure basic needs and, ultimately, health (Pinxten and Lievens Citation2014). Income also influences mental health through the social and emotional meaning it implies: Income shapes evaluations of relative social status, and this may influence factors like life satisfaction and depression (Diener et al. Citation1993:196). A related aspect of the economic privilege of the more educated is their relatively secure labor market position (Näswall and De Witte Citation2003). In general, insecurity about whether one will be able to maintain a job in the future leads to stress reactions and an overall reduction in psychological well-being (De Witte Citation1999), which may result in anxiety and depression (Orpen Citation1993).

An economic explanation of the relationship between education level and depression could thus be summarized as follows: The less educated are more likely to report depressive symptoms because they have a lower income (Hypothesis 1a) and experience more job insecurity (Hypothesis 1b).

Embeddedness in cohesive social structures

Next to a stronger economic position, social characteristics of the more educated could also play a role in their better mental health. The more prevalent embeddedness in heterogeneous networks among the higher educated (Ferlander Citation2007) has, for instance, been linked to assets like employment opportunities (Granovetter Citation1983; Small Citation2006). This aspect of social integration has already been discussed in the previous section, as such opportunities are associated with higher incomes and lower job insecurity and their potential role in the association between education and depression. In this section we focus on the psychosocial benefits of social integration instead.

In general, the more educated are more embedded in cohesive social structures than less educated individuals (Kawachi et al. Citation1997). In line with work of classical sociologists such as Durkheim on the beneficial effects of social integration, current studies show that the strong and intimate relationships that constitute social structures like the family are crucial to mental health (Ferlander Citation2007). When it comes to the family domain, the more educated more often have a stable relationship with a significant other (Ono Citation2009), which serves as a rich source of social support (Ferlander Citation2007) and as such mitigates depressive symptoms (Dahl and Malmberg-Heimonen Citation2010). Other facets of family life, like having children, are argued to play a role as well (Helbig et al. Citation2006), but the relationship between education and parenthood is not straightforward (see, e.g., Liefbroer and Corijn Citation1999).

The more educated also have access to a rich potential of social support in their local community, namely, the neighborhood. Several studies have found more educated individuals to be more embedded in a cohesive neighborhood community, which is considered to reduce individual depression through feelings of security and mutual trust (Ross Citation2000).

In short, there are two ways in which more embeddedness in cohesive social structures could underlie greater prevalence of depressive symptoms among the less educated: less access to social support from a significant other and weaker integration in a cohesive neighborhood. We thus anticipate the following: The less educated are more likely to report depressive symptoms, because they less often have a partner (Hypothesis 2a) and are less strongly embedded in a cohesive neighborhood (Hypothesis 2b).

Status as cultural capital and cultural entitlement

Next to the discussed economic and social factors, Bourdieusian cultural capital could also play an important role in explaining why the better educated experience less depressive symptoms. Cultural capital denotes the extent to which one appreciates, demonstrates, and masters the lifestyle of the cultural elite (shorthand: one’s affinity with elite culture, or affinity with legitimate culture; Bourdieu Citation1984; Bourdieu and Passeron Citation1977) and indicates one’s status: one’s standing on the cultural ladder. It results from socialization within the family and educational institutions.

We are certainly not the first to claim that cultural capital is relevant for understanding social gradients in mental health (see, e.g., Abel Citation2008; Pinxten and Lievens Citation2014), nor do we claim that the mechanism we formulate is the only way in which cultural capital might be relevant. After all, affinity with elite or legitimate culture is more encompassing than what its standard indicators in stratification research—level of education (often referred to as “institutionalized cultural capital”) and highbrow cultural consumption (indicating one’s “embodied cultural capital”; DiMaggio Citation1982; Jæger and Breen Citation2016)—account for. Cultural capital might affect mental health in various ways. (We elaborate on this in the Discussion section.) However, our main goal is to formulate, and subsequently test, a specific mechanism by means of which cultural capital might account for the educational gradient in mental health, in addition to economic and social factors.

The mechanism we propose revolves around what we label “cultural entitlement”—a “sense of ease” (Laurison Citation2015:926) because of the feeling that one matters, is a relevant, valuable, and legitimate part of society, and is entitled to participate in its dominant institutions such as education and politics. The low affinity with elite or legitimate culture and dominant institutions among the less educated is likely to be accompanied with a low sense of cultural entitlement. Research indeed demonstrates that the low status accompanying lower levels of educational attainment decreases people’s sense of cultural entitlement (see, e.g., Lareau, Citation2015; Spruyt et al. Citation2015). The less educated do not perceive themselves as legitimate actors in society, leading to withdrawal from dominant institutions (Kuppens et al. Citation2015; Lamont et al. Citation2014; Laurison Citation2015). In addition, the less educated are excluded from dominant discourse because their dispositions are not considered legitimate or appropriate (see, e.g., Myles Citation2008).

When it comes to its relevance to mental health, cultural entitlement could influence depressive symptoms in at least three ways. Determining through which of these three pathways cultural entitlement influences depressive symptoms goes beyond the scope of this study and is for future research to decide. First, because of the negative identity that is tied to their low-status position (Kuppens et al. Citation2015; Spruyt and Kuppens Citation2015), groups with less education experience a sense of being looked down on (also called “perceived contempt”; Spruyt et al. Citation2015). As is commonly acknowledged, perceived stigmatization is negatively related to mental health outcomes (Kessler, Mickelson, and Williams Citation1999). A lower sense of cultural entitlement could also influence depression through internalization of this negative image: Low levels of self-esteem are related to higher chances on depression (Orth, Robins, and Roberts Citation2008). Finally, not feeling entitled to participate in legitimate culture and institutions relates to low feelings of control and efficacy (Laurison Citation2015:928). Such feelings play an important role in mental health because they are crucial to active problem solving and coping (Mirowsky and Ross Citation1998), which aids prevention and solving of health issues (Pudrovska et al. Citation2005).

While we do not aim to disentangle the specific pathways through which low cultural entitlement associates with depressive symptoms in the present study, the aforementioned clearly indicates that lower cultural entitlement is a potential mechanism through which a lower level of education and higher prevalence of depressive symptoms are connected. In short, we expect that their smaller amount of cultural capital explains why the less educated experience more depressive symptoms, because it inspires a weaker sense of cultural entitlement (Hypothesis 3).

Data and measurements

We use data gathered on our behalf by the GfK Group (formerly Knowledge Networks) in 2014 (Van der Waal, Achterberg, and De Koster, Citation2014). Contrary to other surveys used to scrutinize social gradients in mental health, this one allows us to incorporate both the standard measure for embodied cultural capital and an indicator for cultural entitlement. There were 3,966 people sampled from a probability-based panel carefully designed to be representative of noninstitutionalized adults age 18 or older residing in the United States. There were 2,062 respondents who completed the survey (response rate = 52%).Footnote1 Respondents who completed the survey in 10 minutes or less, which is not enough time to provide serious answers, were excluded from our analyses. This leaves a data set of 2,006 respondents.

To measure depressive symptoms, we used the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff Citation1977), commonly used in research on education and depression (see, e.g., Miech and Shanahan Citation2000). Following studies demonstrating that shorter, four- or five-item versions of the CES-D are as sensitive and specific as the full version (see, e.g., Fendrich et al. Citation1989; Lewinsohn et al. Citation1997), a five-item version of the CES-D was used. We follow Van de Velde, Bracke, and Levecque (Citation2010) and Radloff (Citation1977) by using the CES-D as an interval-level measure indicating the frequency and severity of depressive symptoms and not as a diagnostic tool with a specific cutoff point. For uniformity with the rest of the survey, answer categories for the items range from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 10 (most or all of the time). Respondents were asked how often they experienced the following during the previous week: “I felt depressed,” “My sleep was restless,” “I felt lonely,” “I had a crying spell,” and “I could not get going.” A factor analysis produced a first factor with an eigenvalue of 3.25, explaining 65% of the variance. We constructed a scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.86) by calculating the mean score for respondents with valid scores on at least four of five items. Higher scores indicate greater prevalence and persistence of depressive symptoms.

Education level was measured using the highest degree received, ranging from 1 (less than high school) to 4 (bachelor’s degree or higher).

To measure income, the respondents were asked to indicate which of 19 categories their annual household income fell into, ranging from 1 (less than $5,000) to 19 ($175,000 or more).

To measure job insecurity, we combined two items. Employed respondents were asked how worried they were about losing their job, ranging from 0 (not much at all) to 10 (a lot). Using the same answer categories, all other respondents were asked how worried they were about not being able to find a job in the near future. Combining the two, we have created a variable where higher scores indicate higher levels of job insecurity.

The measurement of respondents’ sense of cultural entitlement was informed by research on expressions of not feeling entitled among the lower educated. According to Kuppens et al. (Citation2015), a potential reaction of the less educated to having a negative group identity is to “withdraw from areas associated with the low-status attribute” (p. 1261). Laurison (Citation2015) similarly discussed how the less educated do not see themselves as sufficiently competent, and therefore do not feel entitled to participate in legitimate culture. Laurison (Citation2015) indicated that this is illustrated by the greater frequency of answering “don’t know” in a political survey among the less educated, who withdraw from the arena of politics because they do not feel entitled to participate.

Following relevant research on this topic (e.g., Berinsky Citation2004; Bourdieu Citation1984; Laurison Citation2015), we measure low cultural entitlement by counting the “don’t know” and nonanswers (missings) provided by respondents on questions on five political issues (derived from Delli Carpini and Keeter Citation1996:304–306): “Do you happen to know what job or political office is now held by Joe Biden?” “Whose responsibility is it to determine if a law is constitutional or not?” “How much of a majority is required for the U.S. Senate and House to override a presidential veto?” “Do you happen to know which party had the most members in the House of Representatives in Washington before the election in 2012?” and “Would you say that one of the parties below is more conservative than the other at the national level?” A factor analysis produced a first factor with an eigenvalue of 2.53, explaining 51% of the variance. A scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.75) was constructed by summating the don’t knows and missing values on the five items.

We measure embodied cultural capital by means of the standard indicator in the status attainment literature: interest and participation in highbrow cultural activities (e.g., DiMaggio Citation1982; Jæger and Breen Citation2016). Respondents were asked how frequently they go to a live opera, ballet, or classical music performance; visit an art museum or gallery, historic park or monument, or neighborhoods for their historic or design value; and read books (with the exception of books required for school or work). They were also asked about the extent to which they consider themselves to be a lover of the arts and culture. All items had response categories varying from 0 (never or not at all) to 10 (very often or very much). A factor analysis produced a first factor with an eigenvalue of 2.41, explaining 60% of the variance. The mean score was calculated for the respondents with valid scores on at least three out of four items, producing a reliable scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.75). Higher scores represent more embodied cultural capital.

Embeddedness in cohesive social structures was measured with two indicators. First, inspired by research on the health effects of social relationships (see, e.g., Frech and Williams Citation2007), we include a measure for having a partner (people who are married or cohabiting coded as 1, others coded as 0). Second, inspired by research on the mental health effects of local communities (e.g., Latkin and Curry Citation2003), we include a measure of embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood. We used the following items: “People around here are willing to help their neighbors,” “This is a close-knit neighborhood,” and “People in this neighborhood can be trusted” (taken from Oh and Kim Citation2009; Sampson and Raudenbush Citation1999). The response categories ranged from 0 (strongly disagree) to 10 (strongly agree). A factor analysis confirmed that these three items represent one underlying dimension of embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood, with an eigenvalue of 2.47, explaining 82% of the variance. A scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.89) was constructed by calculating the mean for respondents with valid scores on all three items. Higher scores indicate more embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood.

Age (in years), gender (coded 0 for male and 1 for female), and parenthood (coded 0 for no children and 1 for one or more children) are included as controls, as they are related to depressive symptoms (e.g., Helbig et al. Citation2006; Nolen-Hoeksema Citation2001; Wade and Cairney Citation1999), as is race/ethnicity (e.g., Turner Citation2013), which was measured using five dichotomous variables, indicating whether the respondents considered themselves to be non-Hispanic white (reference category), non-Hispanic black, Hispanic, non-Hispanic multiracial, or non-Hispanic of another race. We also control for the region where the respondents reside (Northeast, Midwest, South, and West) and whether they live in a metropolitan area (No = 0, Yes = 1). Finally, as the panel utilized includes both online and offline populations, we include a variable indicating whether (No = 0, Yes = 1) respondents had household Internet access at the time of recruitment. (Those who had not were provided with the digital means and assistance necessary for completing online questionnaires.)

Results

Before inspecting the role of status as cultural capital in the educational gradient in depressive symptoms, we provide the descriptive statistics and the bivariate relationship to education for all variables in . This table shows that there is a very modest number of missing values on some variables. After listwise deletion, 1,932 respondents (96.3%) remain, and are included in the structural equation models.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and bivariate relationship to education.

provides more in-depth insight into the bivariate relationship between education and depression and shows that the relationship between our measures of education and depressive symptoms does not appear to show signs of nonlinearity. This is furthermore supported by the linearity check we performed, which produced insignificant results (p = .29), indicating linearity assumptions are not violated.

Figure 1. Bar chart of relationship between education and depressive symptoms.

Figure 1. Bar chart of relationship between education and depressive symptoms.

Moving on to the central issue of this article, we test our hypotheses using structural equation modeling (SEM), which allows us to disentangle the different paths through which education is associated with depressive symptoms. To handle the non-normality of the dichotomous variables included in our model, we use quasi-maximum likelihood to fit the standard errors. This method relaxes the normality assumptions of the default maximum likelihood estimation and produces robust standard errors (Klein and Muthén, Citation2007). Because the standard chi-square test statistic does not follow a chi-square distribution when data are non-normal, we use the Satorra–Bentler scaled chi-square to test the fit of our model (Satorra and Bentler, Citation1994). The Satorra–Bentler scaled chi-square, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and comparative fit index (CFI) indicate that our model fits the data well (Satorra–Bentler scaled χ2 = 49.78, p = .14; RMSEA = 0.01, pclose = 1; CFI = 0.996). The estimated path model is presented in .Footnote2,Footnote3

Figure 2. Structural equation model for testing hypotheses on the relationship between educational level and depressive symptoms. Note. n = 1,932. Standardized coefficients shown, controlling for concomitants age, gender, parenthood, race, region, Internet access and metropolitan status. All values are significant at the .05 level unless indicated otherwise.

Figure 2. Structural equation model for testing hypotheses on the relationship between educational level and depressive symptoms. Note. n = 1,932. Standardized coefficients shown, controlling for concomitants age, gender, parenthood, race, region, Internet access and metropolitan status. All values are significant at the .05 level unless indicated otherwise.

To check the robustness of our results, we estimated two additional models. First, to take into account that one of our mediators, having a partner, is a binary variable, we estimated our final model using generalized SEM (GSEM). GSEM allows for specification of various types of responses and link functions, including binary variables and logit links. Despite these advantages of GSEM, we chose the SEM model presented in because no goodness-of-fit indicators and postestimation commands to estimate direct and indirect effects are available when using GSEM. Since all paths that are significant in the SEM model presented in are also significant in the alternative GSEM model, our findings appear robust.

Second, we estimated a model that included a correlation between education and embodied cultural capital instead of a one-way path from the former to the latter. We did this because such a model fits well with the theoretical idea that education and embodied cultural capital are both forms of cultural capital. It is, however, not possible to estimate the proportion of the relation between education and depression that is mediated (by each mediating variable and in total) when using such a model, which is why we chose to specify our model as presented in . Again, this additional analysis indicates that our results are robust: The same paths are significant as in the model depicted in .

demonstrates that our expectations regarding economic and social factors are largely confirmed. It shows that both income and job insecurity play a significant role in explaining why the less educated experience more depressive complaints, as stated in Hypotheses 1a and b. Embeddedness in the neighborhood plays an additional significant role, corroborating Hypothesis 2b. Contrary to our expectations, having a partner does not affect the frequency and severity of experiencing depressive symptoms, refuting Hypothesis 2a. The more educated thus experience less depressive symptoms partly because they have higher incomes, have less job insecurity, and are more embedded in a cohesive neighborhood.

Turning to the main point of our article, shows that status as cultural capital and cultural entitlement are indeed of additional relevance next to economic and social factors for explaining the relationship between education and depression. In line with extant stratification research, institutionalized cultural capital (level of education) is substantially positively related to embodied cultural capital (β = 0.30), and both inspire a sense of cultural entitlement (β = −0.28 and −0.11, respectively). Finally, a weaker sense of cultural entitlement relates to more depressive symptoms (β = 0.07). These results support our expectation that a smaller amount of cultural capital explains why the less educated experience more depressive symptoms, because it inspires a weaker sense of cultural entitlement.

To examine the proportion of the association between education and depression that is mediated, we calculated the strength and significance of direct and indirect effects (see ). As shows, cultural capital plays a significant role next to the included economic and social factors. Income and job insecurity both account for sizeable amounts of the total effect of education, respectively, 21.5% and 23.5%, whereas embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood accounts for 4.6% of the total effect. With 12.1% and 1.4%, cultural entitlement proves to be a significant way in which both forms of cultural capital are related to feelings of depression. The fact that the less educated experience more depressive symptoms is partly accounted for by their weaker sense of cultural entitlement inspired by their lower level of cultural capital.

Table 2. Extent of mediation.

As both and show, a direct effect of education on depression remains after taking the hypothesized mechanisms into account (β = −0.07). We offer an interpretation of this remaining education effect in the discussion.

Discussion

Our results show that status as cultural capital plays a significant role in the negative relationship between education and depressive symptoms. Furthermore, our analyses indicate that what we call “cultural entitlement” is central to understanding how it plays a role. As our analyses show, a higher level of education, indicative of a higher level of institutionalized cultural capital, relates to a sense of cultural entitlement. The same goes for one’s amount of embodied cultural capital. Our findings resonate with highly influential sociological literature stressing the intrinsic connection between educational attainment and cultural capital (see, e.g., Bourdieu Citation1984) and expands on studies suggesting that cultural capital might play an important role in mental health issues (see, e.g., Abel Citation2008; Pinxten and Lievens Citation2014) by offering a specific interpretation and empirical test of this role.

Our analyses show that cultural entitlement is more strongly related to institutionalized cultural capital than to embodied cultural capital. This resonates with arguments that education is nowadays crucially important for the cultural stratification ladder in Western societies (see Spruyt and Kuppens Citation2014; Stubager 2009): Not only does it affect how individuals see their place in society because it provides individuals with affinity with elite culture, it is also directly related to perceptions of entitlement and “mattering.”

Although several authors suggest that embodied cultural capital has a negative effect on depression (see, e.g., Abel Citation2008; Khawaja and Mowafi Citation2006), we find no significant direct effect while utilizing its standard indicator (i.e., highbrow cultural consumption). This suggests that different, cross-pressuring mechanisms may underlie the relationship between embodied cultural capital and depression. We conducted an additional analysis that also points in that direction. As depressed individuals might participate less in cultural activities, we performed a robustness check by also conducting the analyses measuring embodied cultural capital without actual participation (i.e., only using the item on the extent to which respondents consider themselves to be a lover of the arts and culture). The resulting model shows a modestly significant positive effect on depressive symptoms (β = 0.05, p = .03; n = 1,921). When accounting for the possibility of reverse causality in the relationship between depressive symptoms and the indicator for embodied cultural capital, a moderate positive association between embodied cultural capital and depression thus remains.

This indicates that the theorized negative effects of embodied cultural capital on depressive symptoms (Abel Citation2008), for instance, through stimulating active help-seeking (Mirowsky and Ross Citation1998), may be cross-pressured by mechanisms that link embodied cultural capital to higher levels of those symptoms. A potential explanation can be found in the tendency to ruminate. The high level of reflexivity that accompanies embodied cultural capital (e.g., Threadgold and Nilan Citation2009) could in its extreme guise lead to excessive self-monitoring and rumination, which in turn could lead to more depressive symptoms (Crowe Citation2002). Such a cross-pressuring mechanism could explain why there is no association between embodied cultural capital and depression (main analyses), or even a small positive one (robustness check), while that capital is broadly conceived as beneficial for mental health.

This illustrates an issue we touched on in a previous section: Next to the specific one we analyzed here, cultural capital could also associate with depressive symptoms through other mechanisms. Abel (Citation2008) for instance, argued that education stimulates the active construction of a healthy lifestyle. For him, cultural capital is mainly related to health outcomes because it offers certain values and knowledge, which are incorporated in a healthy lifestyle. Others link cultural capital to a general “know-how” and institutional knowledge, providing the more educated with tools to find their way in complex institutions (Lareau Citation2015). More specifically, the more educated are argued to be more able to navigate medical institutions (Heimer and Staffen Citation1998) and have a higher ability to understand, judge, and act upon health care information (Missinne, Neels and Bracke Citation2014). Taking such mechanisms into account might help explain the remaining direct effect of education.

To determine whether and to what extent these additional mechanisms play a role, future research could simultaneously include various more detailed measurements. For instance, we recommend incorporating multiple knowledge indicators. Specific measures of health knowledge are useful for assessing to what extent the ability to understand and act upon health care information explains the remaining education effect. In addition, one could include indicators of general institutional knowledge, which would aid examining if the ability of the more educated to find their way in contemporary institutions also inspires less feelings of depression. Similarly, including measures for active help-seeking behavior alongside indicators for self-monitoring and rumination could be valuable for further examining the potentially cross-pressuring mechanisms through which cultural capital affects feelings of depression. In addition, it would be useful to include specific measurements that could help determining how exactly cultural entitlement is related to depressive symptoms. As we discussed, a sense of cultural entitlement could shape depression in different ways, ranging from having a sense of control and active problem solving, to the consequences of feeling stigmatized. Future studies using additional indicators could unravel which of these mechanisms are relevant to the association between cultural entitlement and depression.

Finally, note that with regards to cultural entitlement and the rationale behind it, the word “status” may bring to mind theories of “status anxiety” (see, e.g., Wilkinson and Pickett Citation2010). In line with the literature we build on, our approach is, however, not based on economic or material notions of status: Cultural entitlement does not refer to a sense of being more economically successful. It refers to perceptions of being entitled to participate in legitimate culture and institutions based on one’s standing on the cultural ladder of stratification. However, our findings do suggest that an economic form of “status anxiety” also plays a role in addition to the cultural one formulated and tested in this study, since job security substantially matters even when controlling for the influence of income. Our study thus shows that a lower level of education is related to both the perception of one’s economic position and one’s sense of cultural entitlement, and in turn both matter significantly to the experience of depressive symptoms.

Conclusion

We aimed to develop and test a specific interpretation of the role of cultural capital in the negative association between education and depression, next to well-established economic and social factors. Inspired by recent studies in other fields that address level of education and social status, we proposed and tested a novel mechanism based on the notion of “cultural entitlement.” Our analyses confirm our expectation that the lower level of cultural capital of the less educated underlies the greater prevalence of depressive symptoms among this group, because cultural capital provides a sense of cultural entitlement. This resonates well with studies showing that the less educated experience feelings of futility (Spruyt et al. Citation2015) and studies tying these feelings to participation in legitimate culture and institutions (see Laurison Citation2015; Myles Citation2008).

Because of the widespread prevalence of depressive symptoms, the findings of this study are relevant to a relatively large group. We are, however, cautious with claims regarding other mental health outcomes. Seeing as mental disorders like specific phobia or impulse-control disorders do not necessarily bear much resemblance to depression, and they are not categorized as mood disorders like depression (The WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium Citation2004), we do not expect them to be related to cultural capital and cultural entitlement in exactly the same way as depressive symptoms. For more general measures of mental well-being like life satisfaction and happiness, on the other hand, we expect that comparable or even stronger effects of cultural capital could be found, as they are most likely more strongly socially shaped than a “mental illness” like depression (Keyes Citation2002). Future research that simultaneously takes into account various mental health outcomes could establish whether this is the case indeed.

Addressing an additional issue of generalizability, we would like to point out that this study has been conducted in a specific context, namely, the United States. Socioeconomic inequality, and therefore the salience of economic position, is relatively major in the United States as compared to other Western countries (Thewissen et al., Citation2015). It is therefore to be expected that economic factors will play a less prominent role in the education gap in the latter. On the other hand, we expect the role of cultural capital and cultural entitlement in the educational gradient in depression to be greater in contexts where the status order is more insistent, such as France. To unravel whether and how different country characteristics play a role in shaping how cultural capital is related to educational differences in depressive symptoms, future research could apply a cross-national approach.

Acknowledgments

This article was presented at the annual meeting of the Dutch and Flemish Sociological Assocations (Dag van de Sociologie) and the PhD Day of Erasmus University’s Department of Public Administration and Sociology in 2015. We thank all participants who provided feedback. In addition, we are especially grateful for very valuable constructive comments made by three anonymous reviewers.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Josje ten Kate

Much of Josje ten Kate’s research aims to explain how social factors play a role in mental health disparities. In addition, she studies how social inequalities in mental health are shaped by cultural, economic, and institutional country characteristics.

Willem de Koster

Much of Willem de Koster’s research addresses the genesis, manifestations, and consequences of cultural conflict in Western countries. For more information, see www.willemdekoster.nl.

Jeroen van der Waal

Much of Jeroen van der Waal’s research aims to explain why social stratification is linked to value orientations, voting behavior, and health disparities in Western societies. In addition he studies the consequences of globalization and cultural change for inequalities, value orientations, and vote choice. For more information, see www.jeroenvanderwaal.com.

Notes

1The response rate is not high, but comparable to recent waves of widely used large-scale survey programs such as ANES (Citation2014:31) and ISSP (Citation2014, Appendix A). Detailed information about the panel is available through http://www.knowledgenetworks.com/ganp/docs/KnowledgePanel(R)-Design-Summary.pdf and http://www.knowledgenetworks.com/ganp/docs/KnowledgePanelR-Statistical-Methods-Note.pdf.

2The results presented in are controlled for the influence of age, gender, ethnicity, parenthood, region, living in a metropolitan area, and Internet access. A full model containing paths between the control variables and all other variables was trimmed by stepwise removal of nonsignificant paths, each time checking whether the Akaike information criterion (AIC) and Bayesian information criterion (BIC) decreased. This resulted in a more elegant model (AIC and BIC of full model are 66,258.32 and 66,926.27, respectively, and AIC and BIC of trimmed model are 66,241.42 and 66,720.12). We removed the paths from gender to depression, job insecurity, having a partner, and embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood; from ethnicity to depression, embeddedness in a cohesive neighborhood, and highbrow orientations; from region to depression, cultural entitlement, and job insecurity; from living in a metropolitan area to depression, cultural entitlement, job insecurity, and having a partner; from having Internet access to depression, job insecurity, and highbrow orientations; and, finally, the paths from parenthood to cultural entitlement, job insecurity, and depression.

3The estimated model also includes correlated error terms. More specifically, covariance between the errors of all mediator variables is included except for covariance between the errors of cultural entitlement and job insecurity, because it was not significant. Removing this covariance improved the model (AIC and BIC for the model including all correlated error terms are 66,241.97 and 66,726.24 respectively; AIC and BIC for the model without the covariance just discussed are 66,241.42 and 66,720.12). For a clear presentation, we do not display the correlated error terms in .

References

  • Abel, Thomas. 2008. “Cultural Capital and Social Inequality in Health.” Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 62(7):e13.
  • ANES. 2014. User’s Guide and Codebook for the ANES 2012 Time Series Study. Ann Arbor and Palo Alto: University of Michigan and Stanford University.
  • Bauldry, Shawn. 2015. “Variation in the Protective Effect of Higher Education Against Depression.” Society and Mental Health 5(2):145–161.
  • Berinsky, Adam J. 2004. Silent Voices: Public Opinion and Political Participation in America. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  • Bjelland, Ingvar, Steinar Krokstad, Arnstein Mykletun, Alv A. Dahl, Grethe S. Tell, and Kristian Tambs. 2008. “Does a Higher Educational Level Protect Against Anxiety and Depression? The HUNT Study.” Social Science & Medicine 66(6):1334–1345.
  • Bourdieu, Pierre. 1984. Distinction: A Social Critique of the Judgment of Taste. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
  • Bourdieu, Pierre and Jean-Claude Passeron. 1977. Reproduction in Education, Society, and Culture. Translated by R. Nice. Beverly Hills: Sage.
  • Bracke, Piet, Elise Pattyn, and Olaf von dem Knesebeck. 2013. “Overeducation and Depressive Symptoms: Diminishing Mental Health Returns to Education.” Sociology of Health & Illness 35(8):1241–1259.
  • Crowe, Marie. 2002. “Reflexivity and Detachment: A Discursive Approach to Women’s Depression.” Nursing Inquiry 9(2):126–132.
  • Dahl, Espen and Ira Malmberg-Heimonen. 2010. “Social Inequality and Health: The Role of Social Capital.” Sociology of Health & Illness 32(7):1102–1119.
  • Delli Carpini, Michael X. and Scott Keeter. 1996. What Americans Know About Politics and Why it Matters. New Haven: Yale University Press.
  • De Witte, Hans. 1999. “Job Insecurity and Psychological Well-Being: Review of the Literature and Exploration of Some Unresolved Issues.” European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology 8(2):155–177.
  • Diener, Ed, Ed Sandvik, Larry Seidlitz, and Marissa Diener. 1993. “The Relationship Between Income and Subjective Well-Being: Relative or Absolute?” Social Indicators Research 28(3):195–223.
  • DiMaggio, Paul. 1982. “Cultural Capital and School Success: The Impact of Status Culture Participation on the Grades of U.S. High School Students.” American Sociological Review 47(2):189–201.
  • Fendrich, Michael, Myrna M. Weissman, and Virginia Warner. 1989. “Screening for Depressive Disorder in Children and Adolescents: Validating the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale for Children.” American Journal of Epidemiology 131(3):538–551.
  • Ferlander, Sara. 2007. “The Importance of Different Forms of Social Capital for Health.” Acta Sociologica 50(2):115–128.
  • Frech, Adrianne and Kristi Williams. 2007. “Depression and the Psychological Benefits of Entering Marriage.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 48(2):149–163.
  • Granovetter, Mark. 1983. “The Strength of Weak Ties: A Network Theory Revisited.” Sociological Theory 1(1):122–131.
  • Heimer, Carol and Lisa R. Staffen. 1998. For the Sake of the Children: The Social Organization of Responsibility in the Hospital and the Home. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Helbig, Sylvia, Thomas Lampert, Michael Klose, and Frank Jacobi. 2006. “Is Parenthood Associated With Mental Health? Findings From an Epidemiological Community Survey.” Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 41(11):889–896.
  • ISSP. 2014. ISSP 2012: Family and Changing Gender Roles IV Variable Report. Köln: GESIS – Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences.
  • Jæger, Mads M. and Richard Breen. 2016. “A Dynamic Model of Cultural Reproduction.” American Journal of Sociology 121(4):1079–1115.
  • Johnson, Jeffrey G., Patricia Cohen, Bruce P. Dohrenwend, Bruce G. Link, and Judith S. Brook. 1999. “A Longitudinal Investigation of Social Causation and Social Selection Processes Involved in the Association Between Socioeconomic Status and Psychiatric Disorders.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108(3):490–499.
  • Kawachi, Ichiro, Bruce P. Kennedy, Kimberly Lochner, and Deborah Prothrow-Stith. 1997. “Social Capital, Income Inequality, and Mortality.” American Journal of Public Health 87(9):1491–1498.
  • Kessler, Ronald C., Kristin D. Mickelson, and David R. Williams. 1999. “The Prevalence, Distribution, and Mental Health Correlates of Perceived Discrimination in the United States.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 40(3):208–230.
  • Keyes, Corey L. M. 2002. “The Mental Health Continuum: From Languishing to Flourishing in Life.” Journal of Health and Social Research 43:207–222.
  • Khawaja, Marwan and Mona Mowafi. 2006. “Cultural Capital and Self-Rated Health in Low-Income Women: Evidence from the Urban Health Study, Beirut, Lebanon.” Journal of Urban Health 83(4):44–58.
  • Klein, Andreas G. and Bengt O. Muthén. 2007. “Quasi-Maximum Likelihood Estimation of Structural Equation Models With Multiple Interaction and Quadratic Effects.” Multivariate Behavioral Research 42(4):647–673.
  • Kuppens, Toon, Matthew J. Easterbrook, Russell Spears, and Antony S. R. Manstead. 2015. “Life at Both Ends of the Ladder: Education-Based Identification and Its Association With Well-Being and Social Attitudes.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(9):1260–1275.
  • Lamont, Michèle, Stefan Beljean, and Matthew Clair. 2014. “What Is Missing? Cultural Processes and Causal PathwayS to Inequality.” Socio-Economic Review 12(3):573–608.
  • Lareau, Annette. 2015. “Cultural Knowledge and Social Inequality.” American Sociological Review 80(1):1–27.
  • Latkin, Carl A. and Aaron D. Curry. 2003. “Stressful Neighborhoods and Depression: A Prospective Study of the Impact of Neighborhood Disorder.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 44(1):34–44.
  • Laurison, Daniel. 2015. “The Willingness to State an Opinion: Inequality, Don’t Know Responses, and Political Participation.” Sociological Forum 30(4):925–948.
  • Lewinsohn, Peter M., John R. Seeley, Robert E. Roberts, and Nicholas B. Allen. 1997. “Center of Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) as a Screening Instrument for Depression Among Community-Residing Older Adults.” Psychology and Aging 12(2):277–287.
  • Liefbroer, Aart C. and Martine Corijn. 1999. “Who, What, Where, and When? Specifying the Impact of Educational Attainment and Labour Force Participation on Family Formation.” European Journal of Population 15(1):45–75.
  • Link, Bruce G. and Jo Phelan. 1995. “Social Conditions as Fundamental Causes of Disease.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 51(1):80–94.
  • Lorant, Vincent, Denise Deliège, William Eaton, Annie Robert, Pierre Philippot, and Marc Ansseau. 2003. “Socioeconomic Inequalities in Depression: A Meta-Analysis.” American Journal of Epidemiology 157(2):98–112.
  • Marmot, Michael and Richard G. Wilkinson. 2001. “Psychosocial and Material Pathways in the Relation Between Income and Health: A Response to Lynch et al.” British Medical Journal 322(7296):1233–1236.
  • Miech, Richard A. and Michael J. Shanahan. 2000. “Socioeconomic Status and Depression Over the Life Course.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 41(2):162–176.
  • Mirowsky, John and Catherine E. Ross. 1998. “Education, Personal Control, Lifestyle and Health.” Research on Aging 20(4):415–449.
  • Missinne, Sarah, Karel Neels, and Piet Bracke. 2014. “Reconsidering in Equalities in Preventive Health Care: An Application of Cultural Health Capital Theory and the Life-Course Perspective to the Take-Up of Mammography Screening.” Sociology of Health & Illness 36(8):1259–1275.
  • Myles, John. 2008. “Making Don’t Knows Make Sense: Bourdieu, Phenomenology and Opinion Polls.” The Sociological Review 56(1):102–116.
  • Näswall, Katharine and Hans de Witte. 2003. “Who Feels Insecure in Europe? Predicting Job Insecurity From Background Variables.” Economic and Industrial Democracy 24(2):189–215.
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, Susan. 2001. “Gender Differences in Depression.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 10(5):173–176.
  • Oh, Joong-Hwan and Sangmoon Kim. 2009. “Aging, Neighborhood Attachment, and Fear of Crime: Testing Reciprocal Effects.” Journal of Community Psychology 37(1):21–40.
  • Ono, Hiromi. 2009. “Husbands’ and Wives’ Education and Divorce in the United States and Japan, 1946–2000.” Journal of Family History 34(3):292–322.
  • Orpen, Christopher. 1993. “Correlations between job insecurity and psychological well-being among white and black employees in South Africa.” Perceptual and Motor skills 76(3):885–886.
  • Orth, Ulrich, Richard W. Robins, and Brent W. Roberts. 2008. “Low Self-Esteem Prospectively Predicts Depression in Adolescence and Young Adulthood.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95(3):695–708.
  • Pampel, Fred C. 2009. “The Persistence of Educational Disparities in Smoking.” Social Problems 56(3):526–542.
  • Pampel, Fred. C., Patrick Krueger, and Justin T. Denney. 2010. “Socioeconomic Disparities in Health Behaviors.” Annual Review of Sociology 36:349–370.
  • Petersen, Anders. 2011. “Authentic Self-Realization and Depression.” International Sociology 26(1):5–24.
  • Pinxten, Wouter and John Lievens. 2014. “The Importance of Economic, Social and Cultural Capital in Understanding Health Inequalities: Using a Bourdieu-Based Approach in Research on Physical and Mental Health Perceptions.” Sociology of Health & Illness, 36(7):1095–1110.
  • Psacharopoulos, George ed. 2014. Economics of education: Research and Studies. New York: Elsevier.
  • Pudrovska, Tetyana, Scott Schieman, Leonard I. Pearlin, and Kim Nguyen. 2005. “The Sense of Mastery as a Mediator and Moderator in the Association Between Economic Hardship and Health in Late Life.” Journal of Aging and Health 17(5):634–660.
  • Radloff, Lenore S. 1977. “The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population.” Applied Psychological Measurement 1(3):385–401.
  • Ross, Catherine E. 2000. “Neighborhood Disadvantage and Adult Depression.” Journal of Health and Social Behavior 41(2):177–187.
  • Sampson, Rovert J. and Stephen W. Raudenbush. 1999. “Systematic Social Observation of Public Spaces: A New Look at Disorder in Urban Neighborhoods.” American Journal of Sociology 105(3):603–651.
  • Satorra, Albert and Peter M. Bentler. 1994. “Corrections to Test Statistics and Standard Errors in Covariance Structure Analysis.” Pp. 399–419 in Latent Variables Analysis: Applications for Development Research, edited by A. Von Eye and C. C. Clogg. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Small, Mario L. 2006. “Neighborhood Institutions as Resource Brokers: Childcare Centers, Interorganizational Ties, and Resource Access Among the Poor.” Social Problems 53(2):274–292.
  • Spruyt, Bram and Toon Kuppens. 2014. “Warm, Cold, Competent or Incompetent? An Empirical Assessment of Public Perceptions of the Higher and Less Educated.” Current Sociology 63(7):1–20.
  • Spruyt, Bram and Toon Kuppens. 2015. “Education-Based Thinking and Acting? Towards an Identity Perspective for Studying Education Differentials in Public Opinion and Political Participation.” European Journal of Cultural and Political Sociology 2(3–4):291–312.
  • Spruyt, Bram, Filip Van Droogenbroeck, and Dimokritos Kavadias. 2015. “Educational Tracking and Sense of Futility: A Matter of Stigma Consciousness?” Oxford Review of Education 41(6):747–765.
  • Stubager, Rune. 2009. “Education-Based Group Identity and Consciousness in the Authoritarian-Libertarian Value Conflict.” European Journal of Political Research 48(2):204–233.
  • Thewissen, Stefan, Lane Kenworthy, Brian Nolan, Max Roser, and Tim Smeeding. 2015. “Rising Income Inequality and Living Standards in OECD Countries: How Does the Middle Fare?” LIS Working Paper Series 656.
  • Threadgold, Steven and Pam Nilan. 2009. “Reflexivity of Contemporary Youth, Risk and Cultural Capital.” Youth, Culture and Risk 57(1):47–68.
  • Turner, R. Jay. 2013. “Understanding Health Disparities: The Relevance of the Stress Process Model.” Society and Mental Health 3(3):170–186.
  • Van de Velde, Sarah, Piet Bracke, and Katia Levecque. 2010. “Gender Differences in Depression in 23 European Countries. Cross-National Variation in the Gender Gap in Depression.” Social Science & Medicine 71(2):305–313.
  • Van der Waal, Jeroen, Peter Achterberg, and Willem de Koster. 2014. Residential Preferences, Institutional Trust, Fear of Crime, Value Orientations, and Voting Behavior in the United States. Dataset, Washington/Palo Alto: GfK Custom Research.
  • Van Kippersluis, Hans, Owen O’Donnell, Eddy van Doorslaer, and Tom van Ourti. 2010. “Socioeconomic Differences in Health Over the Life Cycle in an Egalitarian Country.” Social Science & Medicine 70(3):428–438.
  • Wade, Terrance J. and John Cairney. 1999. “The Effect of Sociodemographics, Social Stressors, Health Status and Psychosocial Resources on the Age–Depression Relationship.” Canadian Journal of Public Health 91(4):307–312.
  • The WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium. 2004. “Prevalence, Severity, and Unmet Need for Treatment of Mental Disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys.” Journal of the American Medical Association 291(21):2581–2590.
  • Wilkinson, Richard G. and Kate E. Pickett. 2010. The Spirit Level: Why Equality Is Better for Everyone. London: Penguin.