1,370
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Translation and Initial Psychometric Evaluation of Spanish Versions of Three Psychedelic Acute Effects Measures: Mystical, Challenging, and Insight Experiences

, PhD, , PhD, , BS, , MS, , MD, , BS, , PhD & , PhD show all
Received 27 Dec 2022, Accepted 04 May 2023, Published online: 14 Jul 2023

ABSTRACT

This study translated and tested the psychometric properties of acute psychedelic effects measures among Spanish-speaking people. The Psychological Insight Questionnaire (PIQ), Challenging Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ), and Mystical Experiences Questionnaire (MEQ) were translated before being incorporated into a web-based survey. We recruited native Spanish-speakers (N = 442; Mage = 30.8, SD = 10.9; Latino/Latina = 62%; Hispanic = 91.4%; male = 71.5%) to assess their previous experience with one of two psychedelics (LSD = 58.4%; Psilocybin = 41.6%) and their acute and enduring effects. Confirmatory factor analysis (confirming factor structure based on the English version) revealed a good fit for the MEQ, PIQ and the CEQ. Repeating our analysis in each drug subsample revealed consistency in factor structure for each assessment tool. Construct validity was supported by significant positive associations between the PIQ and MEQ, and between the PIQ and MEQ and changes in cognitive fusion and negative associations between changes in prosocial behaviors. As a signal of predictive validity, persisting effects (PEQ) were strongly related to scores on the MEQ and PIQ. Findings demonstrate that the Spanish versions of these measures can be reliably employed in studies of psychedelic use or administration in Spanish-speaking populations.

Introduction

In recent years, there have been increasingly more studies on the acute and enduring psychological and behavioral effects of psychedelic drugs in humans. Clinical studies administering psilocybin, lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), or ayahuasca in carefully monitored settings have shown preliminary safety and efficacy for such indications as Major Depressive Disorder (Carhart-Harris et al. Citation2021; Davis et al. Citation2021; Palhano-Fontes et al. Citation2019), nicotine use disorder (Johnson et al. Citation2014), and alcohol use disorder (Bogenschutz et al. Citation2015), as well as a variety of indications for psychological distress in the setting of cancer or life-threatening illnesses (Gasser, Kirchner, and Passie Citation2015; Griffiths et al. Citation2016; Ross et al. Citation2016), indicative that psychedelics may have transdiagnostic relevance (Kočárová, Horáček, and Carhart-Harris Citation2021). In addition, several dosing studies in healthy individuals have shown a range of impressive effects, including the induction of mystical, insight, and cathartic experiences with enduring positive sequelae (Griffiths et al. Citation2006, Citation2008, Citation2011; Uthaug et al. Citation2021).

In addition, online survey studies have investigated in cross-section or prospectively a wide variety of topics pertaining to psychedelic use. These include encounters with entities (Davis et al. Citation2020), God (Griffiths et al. Citation2016), metaphysical belief changes (Nayak and Griffiths Citation2022; Timmermann et al. Citation2021), and challenging psychedelic experiences (Carbonaro et al. Citation2016), among many others. However, both in-person dosing studies and online surveys have been plagued by a dearth of racial and linguistic diversity. As of 2018, 82% of participants in psychedelic studies were White (Michaels et al. Citation2018), with the majority of studies performed in English-speaking participants. Such lack of representation is problematic in the development of any clinical treatment, as effects may differ across cultural groups and fail to generalize.

This potential diversity of outcomes based on cultural differences may be especially problematic in psychedelic research, as psychedelic effects are highly context dependent (Carhart-Harris et al. Citation2018; Gukasyan and Nayak Citation2020; Hartogsohn Citation2017; Nayak and Johnson Citation2020, Sepeda et al., Citation2019). For example, researchers in an earlier wave of psychedelic research remarked on the striking differences in the experiences of peyote on Native American and White participants: “The responses described in clinical experiments on Whites are so different from the responses described by Indigenous Peyotists […] as to fall into completely different categories. They do not seem to be talking about the same thing” (Slotkin Citation1956). Thus, it is especially critical to expand psychedelic research beyond the confines of certain dominant cultural groups. However, there are several barriers to expanding research to lesser studied groups. For example, in dosing studies it is possible that the time and resource demands of very intensive studies skew toward more affluent populations. Yet, a great deal of psychedelic research takes the form of online surveys that could in principle reach anyone with internet access. However, this requires that commonly used measures be translated and validated into other languages. Indeed, even in-person dosing studies would require this.

Psychedelic studies, whether clinical studies or online surveys, increasingly draw from some assessments that have become standard in the field. These include mystical experiences (Mystical Experience Questionnaire; MEQ30), challenging experiences (Challenging Experiences Questionnaire; CEQ), psychologically insightful experiences (Psychological Insight Questionnaire; PIQ) and a range of enduring effects (Persisting Effects Questionnaire; PEQ), among others (Barrett et al. Citation2016; Barrett, Johnson, and Griffiths Citation2015; Davis et al. Citation2021; Griffiths et al. Citation2006, Citation2011, Citation2016; Nour et al. Citation2016). However, there have been scant efforts to translate these questionnaires into other languages. The MEQ30 has been translated into Brazilian Portuguese, French, and Finnish (Fauvel et al. Citation2022; Kangaslampi, Hausen, and Rauteenmaa Citation2020; Schenberg et al. Citation2017). The CEQ has been translated into German (Dworatzyk et al. Citation2022). Yet to our knowledge, no commonly used psychedelic scales have been translated into and validated in Spanish.

There are several reasons to expand psychedelic assessment tools into the Spanish language. First, Spanish is among the most spoken languages in the world (Eberhard, Simons, and Fenning Citation2019). Second, several Central and South American Spanish-speaking countries are home to various indigenous traditions of classic psychedelic use dating hundreds, and in some cases, possibly thousands of years (Glass-Coffin Citation2010; Guerra-Doce Citation2015). Third, in the United States (U.S.), Spanish is the second most spoken language after English (American FactFinder Citation2020). Thus, it is important to expand clinical investigations to Spanish-speaking populations. Given the growing proportion of Spanish-speaking individuals in the U.S (Ennis, Rios-Vargas, and Albert Citation2011). and the need to ensure access to these psychedelic studies, it is critical that the assessment tools developed in English-speaking populations be translated and validated to increase inclusion in psychedelic research. Therefore, the present study tested the psychometric properties of the Spanish translation of measures in psychedelic research to remove barriers to conducting research with more diverse populations. This study represents an important step in characterizing the phenomenology of psychedelic experiences among Spanish-speaking individuals, and study findings could provide reliable and valid measures of such experiences to be used in future research in these populations.

Method

Measure translation

We first translated all measures using the forward- and back-translation technique. Translations were performed by team members fluent in English and native Spanish speakers. The survey was first created in English then translated into Spanish by CT. This Spanish version was then translated back into English by AMOB. RL then compared this “back-translation” to the original English questionnaires to ensure the translation process had not altered the meaning of the questionnaires. In the event of discrepancies or confusion, individual phrases were retranslated to ensure appropriate clarity and meaning. The following measures were translated: Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ), Challenging Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ), Psychological Insight Questionnaire (PIQ), Persisting Effects Questionnaire (PEQ), Ego Dissolution Inventory (EDI), Nature Relatedness scale, Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire and Prosocial Behavioral Intentions Scale. All measures are available in the supplementary materials (Tables S1 - S8).

Procedure

Respondents were invited to complete an anonymous online survey of Spanish speakers who had a memorable psychedelic experience. Participants were recruited via a variety of methods including posts on Spanish language Facebook groups, e-mails to prominent psychedelic organizations, social media advertisements, the study website, and word of mouth. Interested individuals were referred to our survey website (www.qualtrics.com), where they were presented with our study flyer and informed consent document describing inclusion criteria (i.e., being at least 18 years old, able to read, write, and speak Spanish fluently, having taken a dose of mescaline/Peyote/San Pedro, psilocybin/psilocybin mushrooms, LSD, or 5-MeO-DMT). We chose to focus on 4 psychedelic substances either because they are widely used (psilocybin/psilocybin mushrooms and LSD) or because of their use specifically among Spanish-speaking communities where they are grown/found (mescaline/Peyote/San Pedro and 5-MeO-DMT). After completing consent, respondents completed survey questionnaires. The study was approved by the Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board. No incentives were offered.

Participants

Recruitment occurred from June 2020 to June 2021. A total of 6,283 people clicked the survey link, and 2,458 consented and started taking the survey. Analyses for the current report were restricted to psilocybin and LSD sub-samples to explore the psychometric properties of the questionnaires for the two most prevalent psychedelics used (Krebs, Johansen, and Lu Citation2013), thus 541 responses were excluded from the current analysis. Of the remaining 1,917 respondents, 458 completed the survey (184 and 258 in the psilocybin and LSD groups, respectively). Of these, 16 more were excluded (4: indicated their responses were inaccurate, 2: duplicate IP addresses, and 10: younger than 18). Therefore, the final sample was comprised of 442 respondents (184 and 258 in the psilocybin and LSD groups, respectively).

Measures

Demographic data included: age, gender, race, ethnicity, Hispanic identification, sexual orientation, country of residence, employment status, education level and relationship status. Psychedelic use information included: form of drug, number of times consumed before the reference experience, relative strength of the dose.

The Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ-30) is a 30-item self-report measure that assesses subjective mystical experiences, yielding a total score and four subscale scores: mystical, positive mood, transcendence of time and space, and ineffability (Barrett, Johnson, and Griffiths Citation2015). Questions on this scale are recorded on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from “none; not at all” to “extreme.”

The Challenging Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ) uses 26-items to assess psychologically and physically challenging experiences related to the psychedelic experience. It comprises 7 subscales: grief, fear, insanity, death, isolation, physical distress, or paranoia (Barrett et al. Citation2016). Questions on this scale are recorded on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from “none; not at all” to “extreme.”

The Psychological Insight Questionnaire (PIQ) assesses psychological insights that have occurred during the psychedelic experience related to emotions, beliefs, memories, and relationships (Davis et al. Citation2021). The final validated scale in English comprises 23-items with two subscales (goals and adaptive patterns insights, and avoidance and maladaptive patterns insights). Questions of this scale are recorded on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from “none; not at all” to “extreme.”

We included fifteen items from the Persisting Effects Questionnaire (PEQ). Four items asked respondents to rate the extent to which their psychedelic experience was personally meaningful, spiritually significant, psychologically insightful, and psychological challenging. These are rated on an 8-point scale ranging from “No more than routine, everyday experiences” to “The single most meaningful experience of my life.” The remaining items probed the degree to which each respondent believed their psychedelic experience had led to persisting changes in their personal well-being or life satisfaction, life’s purpose, life’s meaning, social relationships, attitudes about life, attitudes about self and relationship to nature. These are rated on a 7-point scale ranging from “Strong positive change that I consider desirable” to “Strong negative change that I consider undesirable.”

The 8-item Ego Dissolution Inventory (EDI) was included to assess the degree to which the respondent lost the capacity for self-referential processing during the psychedelic experience, thus giving way to feelings of unity and connectedness. Questions of this scale are recorded on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from “none; not at all” to “extreme.”

The Nature Relatedness Scale (NR-6) is a 6-item scale that was included to capture respondent’s perceived connectedness with the natural world (Nisbet & Zelenski, Citation2013). Two iterations of this measure were completed by respondents: one with respect to their answers before their psychedelic experience, and one capturing their answers after their psychedelic experience. Questions of this scale are recorded on a 5-point scale ranging from “Disagree strongly” to “Agree strongly.”

The Cognitive Fusion Questionnaire (CFQ) is a 7-item questionnaire that was included to measure the construct of cognitive fusion as operationalized within the framework of Acceptance and Commitment Therapy; a form of cognitive behavioral therapy that emphasizes distancing (defusing) from thoughts as a key ingredient in therapy (Harris Citation2006). Two iterations of this measure were completed by respondents: one with respect to their answers before their psychedelic experience, and one capturing their answers after their psychedelic experience. Questions of this scale are recorded on a 7-point scale ranging from “never true” to “always true.”

The Prosocial Behavioral Intentions Scale (PBIS) is a 4-item scale that was included to measure pro-social behavioral intentions (Baumsteiger and Siegel Citation2019). Two iterations of this measure were completed by respondents: one with respect to their answers before their psychedelic experience, and one capturing their answers after their psychedelic experience. Questions of this scale are recorded on a 7-point scale ranging from “Definitely would not do this” to “Definitely would do this.”

Analysis

Demographic data were compared between the psilocybin and LSD groups using Welch’s t-test and chi-square independence tests. Analyses focused on examining the psychometric structure of three domains characterizing the psychedelic experience: MEQ, PIQ, and CEQ. We aimed to assess the Spanish version of these tests to validate whether their factor structure paralleled the previously published English version. After psychometric assessment, we also investigated the relationship of these three domains to a range of demographic characteristics and outcome variables including sex, race, age, prosocial behavior, nature relatedness, and cognitive fusion. Change scores were calculated for the measures of prosocial behavior, nature relatedness, and cognitive fusion, subtracting the after the psychedelic experience ratings from the before the psychedelic experience ratings. These change scores were used in all analyses.

We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to assess the psychometric structure of each of the tests in Spanish using factor structures hypothesized from the English version of these scales. We used the established factor structure for the English version tests to setup each factor structure as follows. Based on their current psychometric properties in the English versions, the CEQ was separated into seven factors (isolation, grief, fear, physical discomfort, insanity, paranoia, and death); the PIQ was separated into two factors (avoidance and maladaptive patterns, goals and adaptive patterns); the MEQ was separated into four factors (transcendence, positive mood, ineffability, mystical). We assessed the overall quality of the factor structure for these domains. Given our large sample in our study, we used comparative fit index (CFI) as our primary metric of CFA fit because it is not biased by the size of the sample used in the factor analysis (Bentler Citation1990). Our main results describe the psychometric structure of these tests from the sample, but we also repeated these analyses for subsamples of our data where participants affirmed having an experience with either psilocybin or LSD (available in Supplemental Materials, S. Tables 9–12).

Results

Respondent characteristics

Sample demographics are provided in . The total sample was comprised of 442 respondents, of which 184 reported using psilocybin and 258 reported using LSD. The majority of respondents were heterosexual (80.3%) and male (71.5%). The two groups differed in age (p < .001), educational attainment (p=.034), employment status (p=.017), relationship status (p=.003), and ethnicity (p=.005). The two groups did not statistically differ in gender, sexual orientation, or race. The results also did not reveal differences in subjective dose strength between groups; however, the psilocybin group reported fewer experiences (M = 5.9, SD = 6.9) since their reported experience compared to the LSD group (M = 4.6, SD = 5.7), t(429.26) = 2.174, p = .03. Country of residence is reported in Supplemental Table 13. As Table S-13 shows, most participants lived in Mexico (34.2%), Columbia (12.4%), the United States (12.0%), Argentina (10.9%), and Spain (7.0%).

Table 1. Sample demographics and comparison between drug groups. Age and number of psychedelic uses are reported as means (standard deviations) and using Welch’s t tests to compare groups. Gender, sexual orientation, education, employment, relationship status, race, ethnicity, and subjective dose strength are reported as percentages, using χ2 tests to compare groups.

Factor analysis

Running CFA on the PIQ, MEQ, and CEQ showed an acceptable to-good fit (All CFI 0.84–0.90; ), and individual factor reliabilities were adequate-to-excellent for each subscale of these measures (see ). To test the robustness of our factor structure on each assessment of psychedelic experiences across multiple substances we repeated our analysis in independent subsamples that either had psilocybin or LSD. We found that the factor analysis patterns for each subsample replicated those that we saw in the full sample, demonstrating consistency in factor structure of each assessment across these two psychedelic substances (). Notably, we found moderate to good fit for all factors in both the LSD and psilocybin subsamples, with all domains showing a good fit (All CFI 0.83–0.90). This suggests that the Spanish translations of the questionnaires can be applied across multiple psychedelic substances robustly.

Table 2. CFA Fit summaries for across the full sample and LSD and psilocybin subsamples. CFI: Comparative Fit Index; TLI: Tucker Lewis Index; AIC: Akaike Information Criterion; BIC: Bayesian Information Criterion; RMSEA: Root mean squared error of approximation. Original MEQ with poor model fit is included as well as refit MEQ for full sample as well as LSD and Psilocybin subsamples.

Table 3. Each of the individual factor reliabilities for the MEQ, PIQ, and CEQ are provided. Coefficient alpha, as well as three types of coefficient omega, each modeling uncorrelated measurement errors, correlated measurement errors, and observed covariance matrix for omega, omega2, and omega3 respectively. Average variance extracted for each factor is also provided.

Factor associations with outcomes

We found evidence for individual differences in outcome measures to be associated with our psychedelic experience domains. Moderate significant positive correlations were observed between the PIQ and MEQ scales and the pre-post changes observed in Cognitive Fusion (positive values in this change score indicate a decrease in the negative thought patterns measured by Cognitive Fusion; ), suggesting that individuals who experienced stronger psychological insight and mystical experiences after ingesting psychedelic substances experienced improvements in this domain (). Changes in prosocial behaviors were significantly negatively associated with PIQ and MEQ factors, suggesting that having greater mystical and psychological insight experiences were related to increases in prosocial behavior after the psychedelic experience (negative values in this change score is an increase in behaviors described in the Prosocial Behaviors Scale; ). Moderate significant positive correlations were also observed between the PIQ and MEQ and the pre-post changes observed in Nature Relatedness (positive values in this change score indicate an increase in connection with nature as measured by the Nature Relatedness Scale; ). Persisting effects were also more strongly related to MEQ and PIQ scales, with every PEQ item demonstrating significant positive correlations with MEQ and PIQ scales. Notably, questions 3 (“How psychologically challenging was your experience and your contemplation of that experience?”) and 4 (“How psychologically insightful was your experience and your contemplation of that experience?”) were positively associated with most CEQ factors as well. Overall, these results show that MEQ and PIQ are associated with the PEQ, Cognitive Fusion, and Nature Relatedness scales, and that the CEQ was less strongly associated with these measures (). We repeated these analyses for the LSD and psilocybin subsamples and found high consistency in the factor score associations with our outcome variables (Supplemental Tables 11–12).

Table 4. All significant associations (p < .05) between the CEQ, MEQ, and PIQ domains are shown here CEQ_1: Isolation; CEQ_2: Grief; CEQ_3: Fear; CEQ_4: Physical discomfort; CEQ_5: Insanity; CEQ_6: Paranoia; CEQ_7: Death; PIQ_1: Avoidance and Maladaptive Patterns Insights (AMP); PIQ_2: Goals and Adaptive Patterns Insights (GAP); MEQ_1: Transcendence; MEQ_2: Positive; MEQ_3: Ineffability; MEQ_4: Mystical. All correlations − 0.2 ≤ r ≤ 0.2 are removed. All colored panels represent significant correlations, ranging from red to green (Pearson’s r = −1,1).

Discussion

In the present study we performed a survey investigation to test the psychometric properties of the Spanish versions of widely used English assessments of acute psychedelic experiences and their relationship to several outcomes. Overall, the findings revealed that measures of mystical-type experiences (i.e., MEQ), psychological insight (i.e., PIQ), and challenging experiences (i.e., CEQ) showed reliable psychometric structure after being translated into Spanish. These findings indicate that these assessments of acute psychedelic experiences can be applied to Spanish-speaking individuals in laboratory and survey-based research settings, suggesting their applicability to a wider variety of cultural backgrounds.

From a historical perspective, the use of psychedelics has been strongly linked to Indigenous and mixed-race populations in Latin America (Samorini Citation2019), in which the Spanish language is commonly used. This is contrasted with contemporary research involving the use of psychedelics which has mostly focused on English-speaking individuals. Although there are reasons for the focus on English-speaking populations (e.g., privileged position of English-speaking institutions both funding and conducting psychedelic research), the lack of validated research instruments in other languages is a major barrier to expanding the scope of research in this field. The present survey findings provide a step forward in advancing research in more culturally diverse populations, including those linked to the traditional use of psychedelics, involving substances such as psilocybin mushrooms, Ayahuasca, and the mescaline-containing cacti Peyote and San Pedro. Advancing in this direction may represent an important step toward assessing 1) the suitability of using psychometric questionnaires for individuals with a wider range of cultural provenance and 2) the tools which may be used to establish commonalities and differences pertaining the subjective effects of substances that the evidence suggests are highly influenced by contextual and cultural influences (Carhart-Harris et al. Citation2018).

The importance of validating tools such as the MEQ, PIQ, and CEQ are relevant to both characterize the phenomenology of psychedelic experiences and also to better determine psychological mechanisms which may underlie the therapeutic efficacy for psychedelics in mood, anxiety and substance use disorders, among others (Davis, Barrett, and Griffiths Citation2020; Yaden and Griffiths Citation2021). For example, the degree to which participants experience mystical-type experiences has been found to predict reductions of scores in measures of depression (Roseman, Nutt, and Carhart-Harris Citation2018), end-of-life anxiety (Griffiths et al. Citation2016), and addictive behaviors (Garcia-Romeu, Griffiths, and Johnson Citation2015), as well as increases in scores of well-being (Haijen et al. Citation2018) weeks after the acute psychedelic effects have subsided. The relevance of validating the MEQ for Spanish-speaking populations is thus relevant to properly determine the target states by psychedelic therapy in Spanish-speaking individuals.

Indeed, our results found that scores on the MEQ and the PIQ correlated significantly with self-report outcomes such as improvement in cognitive fusion and increases in prosocial behaviors. Cognitive fusion (the inability to detach from certain thoughts; Hayes Citation2019) has been proposed to be a key component of psychological inflexibility, which in turn may be central for a range of mental health conditions of transdiagnostic relevance (Levin et al. Citation2014). Findings of correlations between the Spanish versions of the MEQ/PIQ and changes in cognitive fusion, are thus consistent with the notion that psychedelics may act on core psychological mechanisms associated to a wide variety of mental health conditions (Kočárová, Horáček, and Carhart-Harris Citation2021), thereby providing evidence of the potential applicability of psychedelic therapy to more diverse populations.

Similarly, the association between Spanish versions of the MEQ/PIQ scores and changes in prosocial behaviors highlight previous evidence of acute increased empathy, sociality, and feelings of shared humanity induced by psychedelics, which have been found in both controlled and naturalistic environments (Dolder et al. Citation2016; Kettner et al. Citation2021). The relationship between psychedelics and intersubjectivity, as well as relational dimensions of wellbeing are resonant with the ritual use of psychedelics by Indigenous and mestizo populations. In these ritual contexts, it has been proposed that psychedelics serve a fundamental function of social cohesion (Kettner et al. Citation2021). We found that increased prosocial behaviors were linked to the extent to which individuals experienced acute mystical-type and psychological insight experiences in Spanish-speaking individuals, which may bear relevance for determining resonances between contemporary and traditional uses of psychedelics by populations living within the same region (i.e., Latin America). Furthermore, the replication of previous findings concerning increases in nature connectedness (determined here using the nature relatedness questionnaire) and persisting effects of meaningfulness of the psychedelic session also replicate previous findings in English-speaking participants (Griffiths et al. Citation2008; Kettner et al. Citation2019).

Taken together, the findings from this study point toward commonalities of psychedelic experiences between previously published findings among English-speaking participants and the sample of Spanish-speaking respondents we present here. Importantly, all measures showed good factor structure, and it is possible that there are cultural similarities that may have a significant influence in both the verbalization and actual lived qualities of psychedelic experiences which have commonly been deemed as mystical, insightful, and challenging. Nevertheless, it is important to note some limitations to the present findings. Consistent with the design of this survey study, the sample was self-selected and all measures relied on self-report. Furthermore, most of the sample consisted of male respondents and further studies may be needed to establish if the validity of these measures may hold in a gender-balanced sample. Also, despite the use of a retrospective pre-post survey design to better understand the self-reported changes in functioning before and after a memorable psychedelic experience, this methodology limits the ability to make any causal inferences regarding the impact of acute psychedelic experiences and changes in nature relatedness, cognitive flexibility, and prosocial behaviors. Further research using prospectively collected data are needed to provide such claims and are a worthy next step in this line of inquiry. Additionally, there was a large sample in this study, and so CFI was used as a primary CFA fit metric because it is not biased by sample size (Bentler Citation1990). Our results showed we were at or just below the cutoffs for acceptable fit based on CFI. However, we believe that retaining the factor structure of the MEQ, CEQ, and PIQ, has a number of theoretical and practical advantages (e.g., comparing scores to English versions of these questionnaires, allowing for future research to explore the factor structure of these newly devised questionnaires in laboratory and clinical settings, aligning the theoretical underpinnings of these constructs across linguistic boundaries and populations), that outweigh the desire for seeking a “best” fitting model through removing items from the scale or searching for a new model structure. However, this is a limitation that should be explored further as the Spanish-version of these scales are used in other studies.

The objective of the present study was to validate Spanish translations in commonly used measures of psychedelic experiences, as well as determine their relationship with outcome variables in order to address some of the limitations of diversity of participants in psychedelic studies. Our findings showed that these measures can be reliably employed in future studies of psychedelic substances in Spanish-speaking populations and suggests that some of the central psychological mechanisms previously associated with the potentially favorable outcomes linked to psychedelic use may also apply in these individuals.

Supplemental material

SpanishSurvey_SupplementaryMaterials 4.10.23.docx

Download MS Word (91.8 KB)

Disclosure statement

AKD and RL are board members of Source Research Foundation. AKD is a Lead Trainer at Fluence. RRG is a member of the Board of Directors of the Heffter Research Institute.

Supplementary material

Supplemental data for this article can be accessed online at https://doi.org/10.1080/02791072.2023.2232379

Additional information

Funding

AKD, SN, NS, and RRG are supported by the Center for Psychedelic and Consciousness Research, supported by private philanthropic funding from Tim Ferriss, Matt Mullenweg, Craig Nerenberg, Blake Mycoskie, and the Steven and Alexandra Cohen Foundation. AKD, NS, and RL are also supported by the Center for Psychedelic Drug Research and Education, funded by anonymous private donors. AMOB receives graduate school funding support from Usona Institute.

References

  • American FactFinder. 2020. American FactFinder—Results. U.S. Census. http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?pid=ACS_10_1YR_B16001&prodType=table.
  • Barrett, F. S., M. P. Bradstreet, J.-M. S. Leoutsakos, M. W. Johnson, and R. R. Griffiths. 2016. The challenging experience questionnaire: Characterization of challenging experiences with psilocybin mushrooms. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 30 (12):1279–95. doi:10.1177/0269881116678781.
  • Barrett, F. S., M. W. Johnson, and R. R. Griffiths. 2015. Validation of the revised mystical experience questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. Journal of Psychopharmacology 29 (11):1182–90. doi:10.1177/0269881115609019.
  • Baumsteiger, R., and J. T. Siegel. 2019. Measuring prosociality: The development of a prosocial behavioral intentions scale. Journal of Personality Assessment 101 (3):305–14. doi:10.1080/00223891.2017.1411918.
  • Bentler, P. M. 1990. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin 107 (2):238–46. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.
  • Bogenschutz, M. P., A. A. Forcehimes, J. A. Pommy, C. E. Wilcox, P. Barbosa, and R. J. Strassman. 2015. Psilocybin-assisted treatment for alcohol dependence: A proof-of-concept study. Journal of Psychopharmacology 29 (3):289–99. doi:10.1177/0269881114565144.
  • Carbonaro, T. M., M. P. Bradstreet, F. S. Barrett, K. A. MacLean, R. Jesse, M. W. Johnson, and R. R. Griffiths. 2016. Survey study of challenging experiences after ingesting psilocybin mushrooms: Acute and enduring positive and negative consequences. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30 (12):1268–78. doi:10.1177/0269881116662634.
  • Carhart-Harris, R., B. Giribaldi, R. Watts, M. Baker-Jones, A. Murphy-Beiner, R. Murphy, J. Martell, A. Blemings, D. Erritzoe, and D. J. Nutt. 2021. Trial of psilocybin versus escitalopram for depression. New England Journal of Medicine 384 (15):1402–11. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa2032994.
  • Carhart-Harris, R., L. Roseman, E. Haijen, D. Erritzoe, R. Watts, I. Branchi, and M. Kaelen. 2018. Psychedelics and the essential importance of context. Journal of Psychopharmacology 32 (7):725–31. doi:10.1177/0269881118754710.
  • Davis, A. K., F. S. Barrett, and R. R. Griffiths. 2020. Psychological flexibility mediates the relations between acute psychedelic effects and subjective decreases in depression and anxiety. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 15:39–45. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.11.004.
  • Davis, A. K., F. S. Barrett, D. G. May, M. P. Cosimano, N. D. Sepeda, M. W. Johnson, P. H. Finan, and R. R. Griffiths. 2021. Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry 78 (5):481. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285.
  • Davis, A. K., F. S. Barrett, S. So, N. Gukasyan, T. Swift, and R. R. Griffiths. 2021. Development of the psychological insight questionnaire among a sample of people who have consumed psilocybin or LSD. Journal of Psychopharmacology 35 (4):437–46. doi:10.1177/0269881120967878.
  • Davis, A. K., J. M. Clifton, E. G. Weaver, E. S. Hurwitz, M. W. Johnson, and R. R. Griffiths. 2020. Survey of entity encounter experiences occasioned by inhaled N,N-dimethyltryptamine: Phenomenology, interpretation, and enduring effects. Journal of Psychopharmacology 34 (9):1008–20. doi:10.1177/0269881120916143.
  • Dolder, P. C., Y. Schmid, F. Müller, S. Borgwardt, and M. E. Liechti. 2016. LSD acutely impairs fear recognition and enhances emotional empathy and sociality. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology 41 (11):2638–46. doi:10.1038/npp.2016.82.
  • Dworatzyk, K., T. Jansen, T. T. Schmidt, and H. Covington. 2022. Phenomenological assessment of psychedelics induced experiences: Translation and validation of the German Challenging Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) and Ego-Dissolution Inventory (EDI). PLOS ONE 17 (3):e0264927. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0264927.
  • Eberhard, D. M., G. F. Simons, and C. D. Fenning. 2019. Ethnologue: Languages of theworld. 22nd ed. SIL International. https://web.archive.org/web/20190806163826/https:/www.ethnologue.com/statistics/size.
  • Ennis, S. R., M. Rios-Vargas, and N. G. Albert. 2011. The Hispanic population: 2010: 2010 census briefs. United States Census Bureau. Accessed July 3, 2023. https://www.census.gov/library/publications/2011/dec/c2010br-04.html
  • Fauvel, B., K. Samuli, S.-B. Lana, R. Bruno, and P. Pascale. 2022. Validation of a French version of the mystical experience questionnaire with retrospective reports of the most significant psychedelic experience among French users. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 55 (2):170–79. doi:10.1080/02791072.2022.2059796.
  • Garcia-Romeu, A., R. R. Griffiths, and M. W. Johnson. 2015. Psilocybin-occasioned mystical experiences in the treatment of tobacco addiction. Current Drug Abuse Reviews 7 (3):157–64. doi:10.2174/1874473708666150107121331.
  • Gasser, P., K. Kirchner, and T. Passie. 2015. LSD-assisted psychotherapy for anxiety associated with a life-threatening disease: A qualitative study of acute and sustained subjective effects. Journal of Psychopharmacology 29 (1):57–68. doi:10.1177/0269881114555249.
  • Glass-Coffin, B. 2010. Shamanism and san pedro through time: Some notes on the archaeology, history, and continued use of an entheogen in northern peru. Anthropology of Consciousness 21 (1):58–82. doi:10.1111/j.1556-3537.2010.01021.x.
  • Griffiths, R. R., E. S. Hurwitz, A. K. Davis, M. W. Johnson, R. Jesse, and R. Frey. 2016. Survey of subjective “God encounter experiences”: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PLOS ONE 14 (4):e0214377. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0214377.
  • Griffiths, R. R., M. W. Johnson, M. A. Carducci, A. Umbricht, W. A. Richards, B. D. Richards, M. P. Cosimano, and M. A. Klinedinst. 2016. Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized double-blind trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England) 30 (12):1181–97. doi:10.1177/0269881116675513.
  • Griffiths, R. R., M. W. Johnson, W. A. Richards, B. D. Richards, U. McCann, and R. Jesse. 2011. Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: Immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology 218 (4):649–65. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2358-5.
  • Griffiths, R., W. Richards, M. Johnson, U. McCann, and R. Jesse. 2008. Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. Journal of Psychopharmacology 22 (6):621–32. doi:10.1177/0269881108094300.
  • Griffiths, R. R., W. A. Richards, U. McCann, and R. Jesse. 2006. Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology 187 (3):268–83. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5.
  • Guerra-Doce, E. 2015. Psychoactive substances in prehistoric times: Examining the archaeological evidence. Time and Mind 8 (1):91–112. doi:10.1080/1751696X.2014.993244.
  • Gukasyan, N., and S. M. Nayak. 2020. Psychedelics, placebo effects, and set and setting: Insights from common factors theory of psychotherapy. Transcultural Psychiatry 59 (5):652–64. doi:10.1177/1363461520983684.
  • Haijen, E. C. H. M., M. Kaelen, L. Roseman, C. Timmermann, H. Kettner, S. Russ, D. Nutt, R. E. Daws, A. D. G. Hampshire, R. Lorenz, et al. 2018. Predicting responses to psychedelics: A prospective study. Frontiers in Pharmacology 9. doi:10.3389/fphar.2018.00897.
  • Harris, R. 2006. Embracing your demons: An overview of acceptance and commitment therapy. Psychotherapy in Australia 12 (4). doi: 10.3316/informit.545561433272993.
  • Hartogsohn, I. 2017. Constructing drug effects: A history of set and setting. Drug Science, Policy and Law 3:205032451668332–205032451668332. doi:10.1177/2050324516683325.
  • Hayes, S. C. 2019. A liberated mind: How to pivot toward what matters. London, UK: Avery.
  • Johnson, M. W., A. Garcia-Romeu, M. P. Cosimano, and R. R. Griffiths. 2014. Pilot study of the 5-HT 2A R agonist psilocybin in the treatment of tobacco addiction. Journal of Psychopharmacology 28 (11):983–92. doi:10.1177/0269881114548296.
  • Kangaslampi, S., A. Hausen, and T. Rauteenmaa. 2020. Mystical experiences in retrospective reports of first times using a psychedelic in Finland. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs 52 (4):309–18. doi:10.1080/02791072.2020.1767321.
  • Kettner, H., S. Gandy, E. C. H. M. Haijen, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2019. From egoism to ecoism: psychedelics increase nature relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent manner. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16 (24):E5147. doi:10.3390/ijerph16245147.
  • Kettner, H., F. E. Rosas, C. Timmermann, L. Kärtner, R. L. Carhart-Harris, and L. Roseman. 2021. Psychedelic communitas: Intersubjective experience during psychedelic group sessions predicts enduring changes in psychological wellbeing and social connectedness. Frontiers in Pharmacology 12:234. doi:10.3389/fphar.2021.623985.
  • Kočárová, R., J. Horáček, and R. Carhart-Harris. 2021. Does psychedelic therapy have a transdiagnostic action and prophylactic potential? Frontiers in Psychiatry 12:661233. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.661233.
  • Krebs, T. S., P.-Ø. Johansen, and L. Lu. 2013. Psychedelics and mental health: A population study. PLoS One 8 (8):e63972. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0063972.
  • Levin, M. E., C. MacLane, S. Daflos, J. Seeley, S. C. Hayes, A. Biglan, and J. Pistorello. 2014. Examining psychological inflexibility as a transdiagnostic process across psychological disorders. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 3 (3):155–63. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.06.003.
  • Michaels, T. I., J. Purdon, A. Collins, and M. T. Williams. 2018. Inclusion of people of color in psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy: A review of the literature. BMC Psychiatry 18 (1):245. doi:10.1186/s12888-018-1824-6.
  • Nayak, S. M., and R. R. Griffiths. 2022. A single belief-changing psychedelic experience is associated with increased attribution of consciousness to living and non-living entities. Frontiers in Psychology 13. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.852248.
  • Nayak, S. M., and M. W. Johnson. 2020. Psychedelics and psychotherapy. Pharmacopsychiatry 54 (4):167–75. doi:10.1055/a-1312-7297.
  • Nisbet, E. K., and J. M. Zelenski. 2013. The NR-6: A new brief measure of nature relatedness. Frontiers in Psychology 4:813. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00813.
  • Nour, M. M., L. Evans, D. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2016. Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: Validation of the Ego-Dissolution Inventory (EDI). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269.
  • Palhano-Fontes, F., D. Barreto, H. Onias, K. C. Andrade, M. M. Novaes, J. A. Pessoa, S. A. Mota-Rolim, F. L. Osório, R. Sanches, R. G. dos Santos, et al. 2019. Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: A randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychological Medicine 49 (4):655–63. doi:10.1017/S0033291718001356.
  • Roseman, L., D. J. Nutt, and R. L. Carhart-Harris. 2018. Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Frontiers in Pharmacology 8:974. doi:10.3389/fphar.2017.00974.
  • Ross, S., A. Bossis, J. Guss, G. Agin-Liebes, T. Malone, B. Cohen, S. E. Mennenga, A. Belser, K. Kalliontzi, J. Babb, et al. 2016. Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30 (12):1165–80. doi:10.1177/0269881116675512.
  • Samorini, G. 2019. The oldest archeological data evidencing the relationship of Homo sapiens with psychoactive plants: A worldwide overview. Journal of Psychedelic Studies 3 (2):63–80. doi:10.1556/2054.2019.008.
  • Schenberg, E., L. Tófoli, D. Rezinovsky, and D. Silveira. 2017. Translation and cultural adaptation of the States of Consciousness Questionnaire (SOCQ) and statistical validation of the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ30) in Brazilian Portuguese. Archives of Clinical Psychiatry (São Paulo) 44 (1):1–5. doi:10.1590/0101-60830000000105.
  • Sepeda, N. D., J. M. Clifton, L. Y. Doyle, R. Lancelotta, R. R. Griffiths, and A. K. Davis. 2019. Inhaled 5-methoxy-N,N-dimethyltryptamine (5-MeO-DMT): Supportive context associated with increased positive acute and enduring effects. Journal of Psychedelic Studies. doi:10.1556/2054.2019.033.
  • Slotkin, J. S. 1956. The peyote religion. 1st ed. Glengoe: The Free Press.
  • Timmermann, C., H. Kettner, C. Letheby, L. Roseman, F. Rosas, and R. Carhart-Harris. 2021. Psychedelics alter metaphysical beliefs. PsyArXiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/f6sjk.
  • Uthaug, M. V., N. L. Mason, S. W. Toennes, J. T. Reckweg, E. B. de Sousa Fernandes Perna, K. P. C. Kuypers, K. van Oorsouw, J. Riba, and J. G. Ramaekers. 2021. A placebo-controlled study of the effects of ayahuasca, set and setting on mental health of participants in ayahuasca group retreats. Psychopharmacology 238 (7):1899–910. doi:10.1007/s00213-021-05817-8.
  • Yaden, D. B., and R. R. Griffiths. 2021. The subjective effects of psychedelics are necessary for their enduring therapeutic effects. ACS Pharmacology and Translational Science 4 (2):568–72. doi:10.1021/acsptsci.0c00194.