1,169
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Impact of family characteristics on the gender publication gap: evidence for physicists in France

, &

ABSTRACT

The publication gender gap in science has been extensively studied. Although women have been found to be less productive than men, little is known about the reasons behind gender differences. Unique longitudinal data collected by surveying a large sample of French physicists gave us the opportunity to investigate the role of family characteristics over time. Using panel data econometrics, we confirm the existence of an average gender gap of about two-thirds of a journal article per year, and about one-third when taking into account several important control variables such as age and career characteristics. We find that female scientists suffer an average productivity loss of about one article when they have a young child, while male scientists suffer an insignificant loss. We also find that female scientists benefit from having large families, with a productivity gain of 0.63 articles per year per child.

SUBJECT CLASSIFICATION CODES:

Introduction

Since 1901, 904 individuals have been awarded a Nobel Prize, and among those Nobel laureates, only 51 are women. Female scientists not only are underrepresented as recipients of this most prestigious award in science, but have been found to have lower salaries than men (Ginther and Hayes Citation2003), to encounter greater difficulties in obtaining career promotions (Long, Allison, and McGinnis Citation1993) and in accessing resources (Witteman et al. Citation2019). While women are increasingly participating in scienceFootnote1, they still have difficulties in succeeding in science and the scientific community is debating about the reasons behind the phenomenon (for a recent overview on the debate, see the recent Special issue that appeared in the Lancet journal dedicated to the gender gap in science, Shannon et al. Citation2019).

One of the reasons that might explain gender inequality in scientific careers and access to resources is the women’s lower publication productivity (Zuckerman Citation1987; Levin and Stephan Citation1998; Bentley and Adamson Citation2003; Duch et al. Citation2012). Scientists are mainly evaluated on their scientific outcome as measured by publications productivity: a strong publication record guarantees scientists’ reward in terms of career progress and resource access (Stephan Citation2012). If women have lower publication productivity than men, especially in the early stages of their career, they will also have more difficulties in obtaining career promotions and access to resources. Our paper contributes to the debate about the gender gap in science by looking at the factors that explain differences in publication productivity between women and men.

Within the large body of literature about gender inequalities in science, several studies focus on and quantify the gender gap in publication productivity. For instance, West et al. (Citation2013) map differences in gender ratios across research disciplines finding that even if female authors are increasingly publishing, they are still far to reach equality with significant differences across fields. According to their study women progressed from 15.1% of authorship in 1665–1989 to 27.2% of authorship in 1990–2012. Fields like mathematics remain male dominated (10.6% of women’ authorships) while others like education are almost balanced (46.3% of women’ authorship). Larivière et al. (Citation2013) analyse gender disparity across geographical regions showing that gender disparity remains widespread worldwide. More recently Holman, Stuart-Fox, and Hauser (Citation2018) moved a step further the gender gap mapping, adding a formal modelling to predict when the gap will close. They estimate that ‘many research specialties (e.g. surgery, computer science, physics, and math) will not reach gender parity this century’ and claim that their results ‘support a need for further reforms to close the gender gap.’ Those reforms would need an understanding of the mechanisms behind gender disparities, however little is known about them. Our study aims to add new insights in disclosing these mechanisms by investigating the impact of family characteristics on publication productivity by gender.

Cole and Zuckerman (Citation1984) coined the term ‘productivity puzzle’ to identify the unsuccessful attempts to explain the documented gender gap in publication productivity and, after almost forty years, the puzzle remains unsolved. Extant studies have considered a broad set of explanatory factors such as age, family characteristics, tenure, and academic ranking. Among the explanatory factors, family characteristics have been pointed out as the most relevant factors. However, existing studies that look at the childrearing responsibilities found divergent findings (Fox Citation1981; Fox and Faver Citation1985; Zuckerman, Cole, and Bruer Citation1991; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Citation1993). The heterogeneity of those findings can be attributed to the lack of appropriate controls in the econometric analysis. For instance, information on individual ability, commitment to work or family life, and social and cultural background are often neglected since they are difficult to measure. Most of the existing empirical analyses are based on samples of scientists observed at a given point in time, cross-section data (with some exceptions such as Long Citation1990; Mairesse and Pezzoni Citation2015). Cross-sectional data allow comparing the productivity of individuals with different measurable characteristics. Differently from cross-sectional studies, longitudinal studies allow to observe the same individual over time and to control for measurable and unmeasurable time-invariant characteristics (Wooldridge Citation2006). New in our work is the use of fine-grained information on the scientists’ family status to exploit the complexity of childbearing responsibilities. Additionally, we use a longitudinal dataset to overcome properly the issue of controlling for unmeasurable time-invariant characteristics.

We survey the entire population of French physicists active at the Institute of Physics (INP) in June 2017. Our survey has been addressed to 1,085 individuals and obtained positive replies from the 621 (57.23%) of those individuals. For these respondents, we were able to reconstruct their publication record, family characteristics, and career advancement history over the period 2001–2016. Physics, being one of the disciplines so-called ‘male-dominated,’ appears a field where the understanding of differences in gender dynamics deserve exploration and the high-reputation of French physicists in the international scientific community makes France the ideal country to conduct our investigation. While the vast majority of existing studies consider the US context (Aguinis, Ji, and Joo Citation2018), our focus on France allows us to add new insights about the productivity of European women in science.

We find that women scientists publish on average 0.68 articles less per year than men scientists. Controlling for the biographical, career, and family characteristics, this gap reduces to 0.32 papers per year. When we further investigate the role played by family characteristics, we find that women benefit from having larger families but suffer from having children less than three-year-old. Differently, men scientists are not affected by the family size nor by the presence of a child. In the conclusions of our paper, we suggest to what extent the institutional and social policy context in France may explain these findings, and why they may differ in other countries.

Family characteristics and the gender gap: a review of the existing studies

For a long time, scholars have debated the existence of a differential in productivity between women and men in science and on the reasons behind it. In the ’80s Cole and Zuckerman (Citation1984) coined the term ‘productivity puzzle’ to point out the lack of definitive explanations for the observed lower productivity of women in science. Extant studies identify three main reasons for the puzzle: biological differences in abilities between women and men, gender discrimination, and differences in life choices. Up to date, empirical studies seem to exclude biological differences in abilities and are not convergent in detecting gender discrimination by journal reviewers, granting agencies or recruitment committees (Aguinis, Ji, and Joo Citation2018; Ceci and Williams Citation2010, Citation2011). The most credible explanation is that women and men seem oriented to make different life choices. According to this explanation, women are restricted by societal and cultural constraints that impose them to be the primary childcare giver, to move to follow their partners’ career needs, and to take care of old parents.

Childrearing responsibilities are an essential part of work-home balance choices, and several studies have attempted to quantify the impact of having children both for women and men (Ceci and Williams Citation2010, Citation2011). Those studies have not yet reached convergent findings. For instance, Fox and Faver (Citation1985) find a positive effect of having children on women scientists’ productivity, while no effect exists for men scientists. Stack (Citation2004) and Mary Frank Fox (Citation2005) find a limited positive effect of having children for both women and men scientists. Other studies find negative effects of children on scientists’ productivity (Long Citation1990; Kyvik Citation1990; Zuckerman, Cole, and Bruer Citation1991; Kyvik and Teigen Citation1996). Finally, no effect results in the studies of Toren (Citation1991) and Sax et al. (Citation2002).

The heterogeneity of results can be attributed to three main limitations: an unaccounted complexity of childbearing responsibilities, lack of appropriate controls in the econometric analysis, and scarcity of longitudinal data. Our study contributes to the gender literature by addressing those three limitations.

First, childbearing responsibilities have often been considered in a simplified way. Several studies do not account for children age, or the interplay between children age and number of children within the family. Importantly, children do not impact equally over time on scientists’ productivity, i.e. children in their pre-schooling age are more time demanding than older children and are associated with the loss in productivity (Mary Frank Fox and Faver Citation1985; Stack Citation2004). When considering the number of children, Leslie (Citation2007) shows that when the number of children increases men and women adapt the amount of time devoted to their academic work. The hours worked per week decrease for women, while worked hours increase for men. Mary Frank Fox (Citation2005) finds that a greater number of children has a positive but not significant effect on women scientists’ productivity. Our study takes into account the children age distinguishing between children in the pre-maternity school period, i.e. the highest time demanding period, from older children. Moreover, it reconstructs the complexity of the family status both in term of children age and number of children.

Second, the studies listed above on the relationship between childbearing responsibilities and scientists’ productivity consider different sets of control variables. While Mary Frank Fox and Faver (Citation1985) and Stack (Citation2004) add controls for scientists’ characteristics, academic rank and co-authorships, other studies assess the publication productivity average without considering controls. Several factors might be correlated both with having children and with publication productivity and not controlling for these factors might bias the estimated effects of having children on productivity. For instance, having a child might interfere with the scientist’s publication outcomes making less likely a career advancement and the access to additional resources granted by a higher academic rank. Therefore, women might experience a disadvantage with respect to male colleagues since the early phases of their career overlap with the age in which women are more likely to have a child (Ceci, Williams, and Barnett Citation2009). To overcome these econometric concerns, in our study we exploit a fine-grained set of controls including academic rank and individual characteristics. Additional controls on the work context are unnecessary since we consider a homogeneous sample of scientists belonging to the same institute, the INP.

The third limitation of the existing studies, i.e. the scarcity of longitudinal data, relates to the data gathering. The vast majority of existing studies are based on cross-sectional data that gives a snapshot of family status and scientists’ profile on a precise point in time (with some exceptions, e.g. Long Citation1992; Mairesse and Pezzoni Citation2015). In assessing the causal relationship between productivity and family status, those studies make a comparison across individuals at the same point in time. For instance, an individual A with three children is compared with an individual B with one child. However, A and B might also differ in other characteristics. Stack (Citation2004) claims that ‘[female and male scientists] who have children [might have] higher organizational skills, energy, stamina, health, or other qualities which are also related to research productivity.’ Similarly, personal factors like family-orientation attitude might affect the decision of having a child as well as productivity. The advantage of having longitudinal data is that we can assess the effect of changes in family status over time for the same individual. We compare the individual A at different points in time, when family characteristics changes while other individual characteristics remain unchanged. With the addition of scientists’ fixed effects, we control for all the unobservable time-invariant characteristics obtaining an unbiased estimation of the effect of family characteristics on scientists’ productivity (Wooldridge Citation2006).

Empirical setting: The Institute of Physics in France

The empirical context for investigating the role of family characteristics in explaining the gender gap in science is a large group of physicists affiliated to the Institute of Physics (INP) in France.

France has a reputation for producing cutting-edge research in the field. Looking back at the history of physics, important discoveries are attributed to French scientists. For instance, the international system of measurement units of electric current, i.e. the ampere, was introduced by André-Marie Ampère who was one of the founders of the science of electromagnetism. Several discoveries were awarded the highest worldwide reward, the Nobel Prize. For example, in 1903, Marie Curie was awarded for her pioneering research on radioactivity. French scientists experiment successfully in the fields of traditional as well as modern physics like quantum electronics. In 2012, Serge Haroche was awarded the Nobel Prize for his studies on the measurement and manipulation of quantum systems. In 2018, Gérard Albert Mourou was awarded for his discoveries on very high-intensity laser pulses.

The INP is the physics French national institute. It is part of one large interdisciplinary public research organization under the responsibility of the French Ministry of Education and Research, the National Centre for Science Research (CNRS). CNRS is one of the most prestigious French research institute, created by the state in the 40ies’ with the mission of ‘advancing knowledge for the benefit of society,’ while ‘respecting ethical rules and showing commitment to professional equality.Footnote2’ It is with the objective of promoting gender equality that CNRS launched the Mission for Women’s integration (2014). In recent years, progress towards gender equality has been made: in 2017, 43% of the CNRS employees were women.Footnote3 However, women’s presence has not increased equaly across disciplines. Looking at the Ph.D. students’ composition, the female share in hard science was only 30% in 2017.Footnote4 In the same year, at higher career levels, institutes like INP count only 19% of female physicists. This lower percentage of female INP researchers reflects a significant gender bias in disciplines such as physics, mathematics, and chemistry.

INP aggregates scientists with high academic profile and productivity. Publication data retrieved from the Web of Science bibliometric dataset (Clarivate Analytics) shows that during the period 2001–2016, INP produced approximately 52,000 publications in Physics. Researchers affiliated to INP are French civil servants and follow a well-defined career progression that counts five career levels. Specifically, a researcher enters INP as (1) Junior Researcher Second Class and, later in his/her career can be upgraded to (2) Junior Researcher First Class, (3) Senior Researcher Second Class, (4) Senior Researcher First Class and, if he/she is recognized for outstanding academic achievements, can reach the level of (5) Senior Researcher Exceptional Class. Researchers are appointed according to their expertise, and, in each career level, they have different responsibilities and salaries. A Junior Research conducts independent research under the supervision of a Senior Researcher who runs his/her lab. Salaries vary from 2,000 euros for a Junior Researcher Second Class to 6,000 euros for a Senior Researcher Exceptional Class.Footnote5

Being part of the French system, INP researchers benefit from the national social security system. In this regard, France, as the majority of the EU countries, address a set of instruments to support parents when children are in their first years of life. Specifically, the French government considers as a critical turning point for childbearing the child’s third birthday. For instance, parents can opt for reducing their working hours until their children are 3-year old.Footnote6 Moreover, starting from this age the vast majority of children attend the Nursery School (‘Ecole Maternelle’) and, to consolidate this habit of sending children to a pre-school, the French government intends to reduce the compulsory school age from 6 to 3 years starting from 2019.

Data

Data collection

We surveyed all French physicists affiliated to INP and active in June 2017. We contacted the 1085 scientists working in the five main study areas in which the institute specializes: theoretical physics, condensed matter and optics, atoms, molecules, and quantum physics. The survey was developed and conducted in French avoiding challenges of translation. We surveyed scientists on their family characteristics. Specifically, we inquired if a scientist had children, and – if this was the case – we asked to detail the birth year of each child. We reconstructed from their Curricula Vitae their career advancements, identifying the year of promotion (if any) from Junior Researcher Second Level to Senior Research Exceptional Level. To complete the tracking of their career advancements in the survey, we asked to specify the beginning and end year of promotion as head of a research unit, if any occurred. The survey was launched online, with INP administration providing us with the complete list of their scientists with their email contacts. Each scientist on the list received a link to access and fill in a form with our set of questions.Footnote7 In total, we received 621 usable answers. The overall response rate is 57.24%. We obtained 364 responses after the first round (June 2017), and 257 additional responses with the first and second reminders (177 and 80 additional responses, respectively in July 2017 and January 2018). We performed a series of statistical tests showing that respondents are similar to non-respondents (see Appendix A). Female responded in the 68.27% of the cases, while males responded in 54.62% of the cases.

We complemented the survey data with demographic data obtained from the French Ministry of Education, and we reconstructed the full scientists’ publication record by collecting their publications on the Web of Science dataset (Clarivate Analytics). In collecting the publications, we included only publications above a certain threshold of quality. Specifically, publications that appeared in journals with a 5-year impact factor greater than 0.5, meaning that the articles published in these journals received on average more than half a citation in the last five years. Journals with a 5-year impact factor greater than 0.5 correspond to the 99% of the entire poll of Physics journals covered by Web of Science. We end up with a sample of 273 journals.

Data description

The way in which we framed the questions allowed us to build a longitudinal dataset having important advantages compared to similar studies. While extant studies used surveys to capture a snapshot of the situation in a given period, having asked the exact birth year of each child and the exact period when they had been the head of a research unit, we reconstructed the yearly scientist’ family and academic rank. Observing each of our 621 scientists along the observation period 2001–2016, we end up with 9,021 observations. Not all our scientists are present in the observation period 2001–2016, due to their different entry and exit year at INP. In our sample, 67.3% of scientists are observed for the full period, whereas the minimum number of observations per scientist is five years (0.48% of the cases).

and show a picture of our scientists’ family status. According to , the most frequent case is having two children (36.88% of the total sample: 35.7% of the men have two children, 40.85% of women have the same family size). It is rare to have more than four children (1.45% of the total sample, 1.67% of men, 0.7% of women). The 22.55% of males have no children, 19.01% of females are in the same situation (21.74% of the total sample).

Figure 1. Children year of birth.

Figure 1. Children year of birth.

Table 1. Family size: Distribution of the number of children by scientists’ gender.

shows the distribution of the children birth years. Two hundred forty-eight scientists had the first child during our observation period, i.e. from 2001 to 2016.

19.42% of men (93/479) have had the responsibility for a research unit (head of a research unit), and 14.08% of women (20/142) had covered the same position.

Looking at the demographic and academic profile, scientists are middle aged (the average age is about 41 for both men and women) with on average 2.66 publications per year (2.82 for men, 2.14 for women).

Scientists in our sample are almost equally distributed across the INP sub-disciplines: 18.68% belong to Physical theories, 24.48% to Condensed Matter Physics with a focus on structures and electronic properties, 28.66% to Atoms and Molecules, Optics and Lasers and the remaining 28.18% to Condensed Matter Physics with a focus on organizations and dynamics.

Variable construction

Based on the survey data, the data released by the French Ministry of Education, and the bibliometric data collected on the Web of Science database, we calculate our variables of interest and controls. We measure scientist’s publication productivity as the Number of publications authored by the scientist in a given year t. Among the variables related to the scientist’s demographic characteristics, we consider the dummy variable Female that equals one if the scientist is a woman, zero otherwise, and Age that corresponds to the age of the scientist in year t. Footnote8 To measure family characteristics, we look at the Total number of children that equals to the number of children born until year t. Considering that children have different needs at the various stages of their life, we distinguish the cases where in the family there is a child in their first years of life. The dummy variable One child born in the last 3 years equals one if the scientist has at least one child born in the last three years, i.e. in t, t-1, and t-2. The French government considers 3-year old as the maximum age until when parents can opt for reducing their working-time (see the section Empirical setting). We assume that 3-years old is the threshold when parents devote more time to their child. Then, to take into account the effect of the career advancement on scientists’ productivity, we construct a set of dummy variables reflecting the scientists’ career level. The dummy variables Junior researcher (Second class), Junior researcher (First class), Senior researcher (Second class), Senior researcher (First class), and Senior researcher (Exceptional), assume the value of one if the scientist is at the corresponding career level, zero otherwise. We also consider the period of the scientist’s career before being appointed at INP (Pre-entry INP). Moreover, we include the dummy variable Head of a research unit in the last 3 years that equals one if the scientist had the full responsibility of the research unit in t, t-1, and t-2. To account for the past publication productivity, the variable Initial number of papers counts the number of papers published by the scientist in the year before entering the panel. Finally, we construct a dummy variable for each physics sub-discipline (Physical theories, Condensed matter physics -structures and electronic properties-, Atoms and Molecules, Optics and Lasers, Hot Plasma Physics, and Condensed matter physics -organizations and dynamics-) and each calendar year from 2001 to 2016.

shows the corresponding descriptive statistics at scientist-year level.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics.

Method and results

This study aims to investigate the impact of the family characteristics on the gender publication gap. To this purpose, we proceed in two steps. First, we conduct an exploratory analysis based on the descriptive statistics of our data and, second, we estimate the parameters (β) of Equation (1) with a formal multiple regression model applying Ordinary Least Squares (OLS). (1) Publication productivityit=β0+β1demographic characteristicsit+β2family characteristicsit+β3Career advancementit+εit(1) Where i and t refer to the scientist i observed at time t. Our 621 individuals are observed over the period 2001–2016, for a total of 9021 observations. Individuals are observed on average for 14.52 years.

and show how publication productivity varies by family size (for the total sample) and by children age (for the 248 physicists sub-sample). Comparing the productivity of scientists across number of children classes (), we observe that men are more productive when they have one child or four children or more, while they have a lower productivity when they have no children or a medium size family (two or three children). Women have a lower productivity when they have one child and a higher productivity when they have three children. Men and women scientists show the closest productivity values in the case of 3-children families.

Figure 2. Publication productivity by family size (standard errors are reported on the top of the bars).

Figure 2. Publication productivity by family size (standard errors are reported on the top of the bars).

Figure 3. Publication productivity by children age in the sub-sample of 248 scientists for which we can observe the full family history.

Figure 3. Publication productivity by children age in the sub-sample of 248 scientists for which we can observe the full family history.

When adding the temporal dimension into the analysis, shows that when children grow up, gender disparities attenuate. In our study sample, 38.33% of scientists (238 scientists) enter the observation period already having children while 21.74% (135 scientists) remain for the entire observation period without children. To quantify the impact of having children over time, we consider the subsample of 248 scientists (39.94%) for which we can observe the full family history, i.e. those scientists who had the first child during our observation period. shows that men and women scientists with children older than four years have no significant differences in publication productivity. When the youngest child is younger than 3-year old, we observe lower publication productivity both for women scientists (about one publication less) and for men scientists (about half a publication less).

In , we present the results of the OLS estimation of Equation 1. In columns from 1 to 5, we progressively add to scientists’ demographic characteristics, controls for family characteristics and career advancement. Column 1 presents the baseline model including demographics’ characteristics (being a Female scientist, and Age) and scientist’s productivity at the beginning of the observation period (Initial number of papers). Column 2 adds controls for career advancement (from the entry level – Junior researcher (Second class) – to the highest one – Senior researcher (Exceptional) - and being the Head of a research unit). Columns 3 and 4 separately include controls for family characteristics: the total number of children (Total number of children), and a dummy if the youngest child is 3-year old or younger (One child born in the last 3 years) respectively. Column 5 includes both controls for family characteristics at the same time.

Table 3. Publication productivity gender gap, OLS estimations.

According to Column 1, women scientists publish 0.29 papers less than men scientists per year. The gender gap in favour of men increases up to 0.32 papers when we add career advancement and family characteristics. The impact of controls on publications productivity is in line with conventional expectations: young scientists are more productive, having a high-level managerial task as head of a research unit hurts publication productivity, and high initial observed productivity tend to persist.Footnote9 Interestingly, having a little child has a detrimental effect on scientists’ productivity −0.30 papers per year (Column 5).

The latter results on family characteristics might be biased by a series of omitted unobservable variables such as the commitment to work, the willingness to create a family, or other cultural and social background characteristics. To further investigate the family characteristics impact correcting for these omitted unobservable factors, we include scientists’ fixed effects, and we conduct separate analyses on the sub-samples of men (479 scientists observed yearly for a total of 6947 observations) and women scientists (142 scientists observed yearly for a total of 2074 observations).

reports the results. In columns from 1 to 4, we progressively add controls for family characteristics and career advancements. The model in column 1 includes controls for scientist’s age (Age), academic rank (Junior researcher and Senior researcher dummies), being the Head of a research unit and Scientists’ fixed effects. Columns 2 and 3 include the controls separately for family characteristics, i.e. the total number of children (Total number of children) and a dummy that equals one if the youngest child is 3-year old or younger (One child born in the last 3 years). The full model reported in Column 4 includes both the family characteristics at the same time.

Table 4. Family characteristics, scientists’ fixed effect estimations.

The values of the R-squared reported in Column 1, 2, 3 and 4 of indicate the proportion of the variation of the publication productivity that our models explain. In a range that goes from 0 to 1, all the R-squared values are above 0.54 indicating that our models are explaining more than half of the publication productivity variation. The increase of the R-squared values between the models reported in and those reported in including scientist fixed effects, shows the importance of controlling for the time-invariant unobserved characteristics of the scientists in explaining their publication productivity.

Interestingly, results of the full model reported in Column 4 confirm that when scientists are appointed Head of a research unit they decrease significantly their productivity (−0.52 papers per year for men, −0.78 papers per year for women). For men scientists, having a larger family or having at least one little child in the family does not affect publication productivity. Women scientists have productivity gain when the number of children increases (+0.63 papers per year for each child). However, for women having a young child has detrimental effects on publication productivity (−0.99 papers per year).

provides a graphical representation of the regression results reported in , Column 4.Footnote10 Specifically, it shows the predicted scientist’s publication productivity for a representative scientist when family characteristics vary. We assume that the representative scientist is a junior researcher (first class), not the head of a lab, and 40-year-old. This scientist’s profile is the most frequent in our dataset (see the average characteristics of the scientists in our sample reported in ). For family characteristics, in , we predict the publication productivity for women and men scientists, having or not a young child, and with different family sizes.

Figure 4. Publication productivity predictions for a representative scientist when family characteristics vary.

Figure 4. Publication productivity predictions for a representative scientist when family characteristics vary.

For the representative scientist, shows that a gender gap in publication productivity exists for women scientists with no children with respect to their male counterparts. Moreover, the figure shows that, for women scientists, there is a compensation effect between the number of children and children age: having a young child disadvantages women scientists, while women increase their publication productivity when the family enlarges. Interestingly, men scientists benefit less from having a larger family and, at the same time, they are less affected by the presence of a young child in the family. The turning point when women scientists catch up their male colleagues in terms of publication productivity is when they have a family of at least three grown-up children.

Discussion and conclusions

This paper contributes to the gender literature by analysing the role played by family characteristics on male and female scientists’ publication productivity. Unique to our study is the use of longitudinal data. We surveyed all the physicists affiliated to INP in June 2017 and reconstructed their full data record, including family status, academic rank, and publications over the period 2001–2016. We find that family characteristics play a different role for men and women.

The paper addresses a number of econometric challenges faced in the attempt to accurately quantify the relationship between family-related explanatory factors and publication productivity. Family characteristics are the observable characteristics most commonly suggested as an explanatory factor of the productivity gender gap (Ceci and Williams Citation2010, Citation2011). However, few studies have been able to reconstruct the entire scientists’ family history. Mary Frank Fox and Faver (Citation1985) and Kyvik (Citation1990) use cross-sectional data showing not convergent evidence on the effect of having children. By asking our surveyed scientists the exact date of birth of their children, we have been able to identify how women and men scientists’ productivity vary when scientists’ family characteristics change over the scientists’ career. We find that while family characteristics do not impact on men scientists’ productivity, while women scientists benefit in having a large family but are disadvantaged in the early years of their children life.

Concerning the number of children, we have two possible explanations for its positive impact on publication productivity. First, there might be an incentive to publish more and to progress in career to assure better-living conditions to a larger family. Second, there might be a selection effect: women scientists who have larger families are also those showing higher publication productivity. The selection effect might depend on cultural or social factors. Nonetheless, in our econometric exercise we control for scientists’ fixed effects and, as long as we consider cultural or social factors as time-invariant, we can exclude the presence of a selection effect explaining our results.

Finding that women are disadvantaged when having a young child might be surprising in the light of the characteristics of the empirical context we are considering. The French government is investing many efforts to support families. Parents are allowed to devote more time to family life when they have young children by reducing their working time. Later on, when parents are asked to go back to their regular work schedule, children enter the pre-school. Despite all these efforts, our results show that there is still space for additional policy interventions, and greater investments are needed. The French government seems to perceive this need. For instance, the government intends to reduce the compulsory school age from 6 to 3 years starting from 2019 to consolidate the habit of sending children to pre-school.

This study is not without limitations, which open venues for future research. For instance, we applied our analysis to one field, Physics, and one European country with a strong social security system and a centralized academic system, France. Physics is one of the so-called ‘male-dominated’ fields, and future research could assess the impact of family characteristics in fields with a greater representation of women like social science. The French system is close to other European countries like Italy (Pezzoni, Sterzi, and Lissoni Citation2012), it differs from others like Switzerland where the social security system is weaker, and the academic system is decentralized. It is possible that the gender differences we found are even more substantial for countries with a weaker social security system than France, and cross-country studies would add further insights into the gender gap debate.

Acknowledgments

We are especially thankful to Catherine de Montenay, Anne Pepin, Fabrice Planchon Elizabeth Politzer, Alain Schuhl, Anne Sigogneau, and to two anonymous referees.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Jacques Mairesse

Jacques Mairesse is honorary professor at Institut Polytechnique Paris, ENSAE and CREST, and professor emeritus at Maastricht University and UNU-MERIT. His research has been focused in the economics of production, the measurement of productivity and capital, and the econometrics of individual data panels. His current research mostly concerns the economics of science, with interest in gender issues and public funding performance.

Michele Pezzoni

Michele Pezzoni is an associate professor at Université Côte d’Azur. His main research interests are in the economics of science and innovation, and in particular the investigation of the determinants of researchers’ productivity and careers.

Fabiana Visentin

Fabiana Visentin is an assistant professor at the School of Business and Economics at Maastricht University and UNU-MERIT. Her research interests focus on the microeconomics of innovation and on the economics of science.

Notes

1 According to the US National Science Foundation and the Eurostat data, the share of women doctorate recipients is increasing and has almost reached the parity across fields (Sources: US National Science Foundation Survey of Earned Doctorates (SED) and Eurostat (Science and Technology Database, Statistics on Research and Development).

5 See the CNRS website for details on INP scientists’ work conditions (www.dgdr.cnrs.fr/drhchercheurs/Travail/concoursch2010/chercheur/carriere-en.htm).

6 For further details about the French Social Security System, see https://www.cleiss.fr/docs/regimes/regime_france/an_1.html

7 Our questionnaire is available upon request.

8 The variable age is centred on the age of 40, meaning that we subtract 40 to the actual age at time t

9 There is a positive correlation between the number of papers published by a scientist when she enters the observation period and the later yearly productivity. One paper more in the initial stock guarantee 0.72 paper more by year.

10 The predictions are based on the two models reported in Column 4 of excluding calendar year dummies. We opted for this exclusion since calendar dummies are not statistically significant (nor for male neither for female). The exclusion also allows us to interpret predictions as the average all over the observation period.

References

  • Aguinis, Herman, Young Hun Ji, and Harry Joo. 2018. “Gender Productivity Gap among Star Performers in STEM and Other Scientific Fields.” Journal of Applied Psychology 103 (12): 1283–1306. doi:10.1037/apl0000331.
  • Bentley, Jerome T., and Rebecca Adamson. 2003. “Gender Differences in the Careers of Academic Scientists and Engineers: A Literature Review. Special Report”.
  • Ceci, Stephen J., and Wendy M. Williams. 2010. “Sex Differences in Math-Intensive Fields.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 19 (5): 275–279. doi:10.1177/0963721410383241.
  • Ceci, Stephen J., and Wendy M. Williams. 2011. “Understanding Current Causes of Women’s Underrepresentation in Science.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 (8): 3157–3162. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014871108.
  • Ceci, Stephen J., Wendy M. Williams, and Susan M. Barnett. 2009. “Women’s Underrepresentation in Science: Sociocultural and Biological Considerations.” Psychological Bulletin 135 (2): 218–261. doi:10.1037/a0014412.
  • Cole, Jonathan R., and Harriet Zuckerman. 1984. The Productivity Puzzle: Persistence and Change in Patterns of Publication of Men and Women Scientists. Vol. 2.
  • Duch, Jordi, Xiao Han T. Zeng, Marta Sales-Pardo, Filippo Radicchi, Shayna Otis, Teresa K. Woodruff, and Luís A. Nunes Amaral. 2012. “The Possible Role of Resource Requirements and Academic Career-Choice Risk on Gender Differences in Publication Rate and Impact.” Plos One 7 (12): e51332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051332
  • Fox, Mary Frank. 1981. “Sex, Salary, and Achievement: Reward-Dualism in Academia.” Sociology of Education 54 (2): 71–84. doi: 10.2307/2112352
  • Fox, Mary Frank. 2005. “Gender, Family Characteristics, and Publication Productivity among Scientists.” Social Studies of Science 35 (1): 131–150. doi:10.1177/0306312705046630.
  • Fox, Mary Frank, and Catherine A. Faver. 1985. “Men, Women, and Publication Productivity: Patterns among Social Work Academics.” The Sociological Quarterly 26 (4): 537–549. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.1985.tb00243.x
  • Ginther, D. K., and K. J. Hayes. 2003. “Gender Differences in Salary and Promotion for Faculty in the Humanities 1977–95.” Journal of Human Resources 38 (1): 34–73. doi: 10.2307/1558755
  • Holman, Luke, Devi Stuart-Fox, and Cindy E. Hauser. 2018. “The Gender Gap in Science: How Long Until Women are Equally Represented?” Edited by Cassidy Sugimoto. PLOS Biology 16 (4): e2004956. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2004956.
  • Kyvik, Svein. 1990. “Motherhood and Scientific Productivity.” Social Studies of Science 20 (1): 149–160. doi: 10.1177/030631290020001005
  • Kyvik, Svein, and Mari Teigen. 1996. “Child Care, Research Collaboration, and Gender Differences in Scientific Productivity.” Science, Technology & Human Values 21 (1): 54–71. doi: 10.1177/016224399602100103
  • Larivière, Vincent, Chaoqun Ni, Yves Gingras, Blaise Cronin, and Cassidy R. Sugimoto. 2013. “Bibliometrics: Global Gender Disparities in Science.” Nature News 504 (7479): 211. doi: 10.1038/504211a
  • Leslie, David. 2007. “The Reshaping Of America’s Academic Work Force.” Research Dialogue 87: 1–23.
  • Levin, Sharon G., and Paula E. Stephan. 1998. “Gender Differences in the Rewards to Publishing in Academe: Science in the 1970s.” Sex Roles 38 (11–12): 1049–1064. doi: 10.1023/A:1018882711314
  • Long, J. Scott. 1990. “The Origins of Sex Differences in Science.” Social Forces 68 (4): 1297–1316. doi: 10.1093/sf/68.4.1297
  • Long, J. Scott. 1992. “Measures of Sex Differences in Scientific Productivity.” Social Forces 71 (1): 159–178. doi: 10.1093/sf/71.1.159
  • Long, J. Scott, Paul D. Allison, and Robert McGinnis. 1993. “Rank Advancement in Academic Careers: Sex Differences and the Effects of Productivity.” American Sociological Review 58 (5): 703–722. doi: 10.2307/2096282
  • Mairesse, Jacques, and Michele Pezzoni. 2015. “Does Gender Affect Scientific Productivity?” Revue economique 66 (1): 65–113. doi: 10.3917/reco.661.0065
  • Pezzoni, M., V. Sterzi, and L. Lissoni. 2012. “Career Progress in Centralized Academic Systems: Social Capital and Institutions in France and Italy.” Research Policy 41 (4): 704–719. doi: 10.1016/j.respol.2011.12.009
  • Sax, Linda J., Linda Serra Hagedorn, Marisol Arredondo, and Frank A. Dicrisi III. 2002. “Faculty Research Productivity: Exploring the Role of Gender and Family-Related Factors.” Research in Higher Education 43 (4): 423–446. doi: 10.1023/A:1015575616285
  • Shannon, Geordan, Melanie Jansen, Kate Williams, Carlos Cáceres, Angelica Motta, Aloyce Odhiambo, Alie Eleveld, and Jenevieve Mannell. 2019. “Gender Equality in Science, Medicine, and Global Health: Where are We at and Why Does It Matter?” The Lancet 393 (10171): 560–569. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)33135-0
  • Stack, Steven. 2004. “Gender, Children and Research Productivity.” Research in Higher Education 45 (8): 891–920. doi: 10.1007/s11162-004-5953-z
  • Stephan, Paula. 2012. How Economics Shapes Science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Toren, Nina. 1991. “The Nexus between Family and Work Roles of Academic Women in Israel: Reality and Representation.” Sex Roles 24 (11–12): 651–667. doi: 10.1007/BF00288204
  • West, Jevin D., Jennifer Jacquet, Molly M. King, Shelley J. Correll, and Carl T. Bergstrom. 2013. “The Role of Gender in Scholarly Authorship.” PloS One 8 (7): e66212. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0066212
  • Witteman, Holly O, Michael Hendricks, Sharon Straus, and Cara Tannenbaum. 2019. “Are Gender Gaps due to Evaluations of the Applicant or the Science? A Natural Experiment at a National Funding Agency.” The Lancet 393 (10171): 531–540. ISSN 0140-6736. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32611-4
  • Wooldridge, Jeffrey M. 2006. Introductory Econometrics: A Modern Approach. 3rd ed. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western College Publishing.
  • Zuckerman, Harriet. 1987. “Persistence and Change in the Careers of Men and Women Scientists and Engineers.” In Women: Their Underrepresentation and Career Differentials in Science and Engineering, edited by Linda S. Dix, 123–156. Washington, DC: The National Academy Press.
  • Zuckerman, Harriet, Jonathan R. Cole, and John T. Bruer. 1991. The Outer Circle. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Appendix A

. Compares respondents and non-respondents across their principal characteristics, i.e. birth year, gender, research sub-field, and academic rank. Respondents and non-respondents show minimal evidence of bias on birth year and gender.

Table A1. Respondents versus non-respondents’ characteristics (comparison on the means).