Publication Cover
Experimental Aging Research
An International Journal Devoted to the Scientific Study of the Aging Process
Volume 43, 2017 - Issue 2
532
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Corrigendum

Corrigendum

This article refers to:
The Emotional Stroop as an Emotion Regulation Task

Article title: The Emotional Stroop as an Emotion Regulation Task

Author: Cathleen Kappes and Christina Bermeitinger

Journal: Experimental Aging Research

Bibliometrics: Volume 42, Issue 2, Pages 161–194 (2016)

DOI: 10.1080/0361073X.2016.1132890

Due to reanalyses prompted by the careful reading of a colleague, the authors realized a mistake in the assignment of words presented to one group of participants (four words in the positive word list of both lists for young adults) as well as a few trials with a technical error in their experiment that was published as Study 2 in Kappes and Bermeitinger (Citation2016). In detail, first, for young participants, eight words of the positive list (four in word list A and four in word list B; between-subject) were erroneously assigned to the wrong arousal category (that is, four words that are actually medium arousing—i.e., Fahrt, Leistung, Zirkus, Ansporn—were assigned to the high arousing words and vice versa for four other words, i.e., Anreiz, Genie, Weltruhm, Abitur; see updated Appendix A and Appendix B). Second, we noticed that due to technical reasons in a few trials, for half of the older participants, no target was shown (M = 1.2% of trials, ranging from 0.9% to 1.6% between participants). We now rearranged the words in the positive lists for young participants and excluded the trials without any target for old participants. Of course, thus, the statistical results slightly changed. In the following, we present the corrected results, tables, and figures. Note that the general pattern and the main results (including statistical significance) were the same as presented in our original article. So, our interpretation of the data remains unaltered. According to the new results, the following two sentences of the original discussion of Experiment 2 had to be deleted: “Younger adults’ mean RTs were faster in the positive word blocks than in the negative word blocks for words presented in the background, while[whereas] there was no difference for the older age group and the other word positions. As this effect did not interact with arousal level (and therefore the effect also includes differences in response to neutral words), it is difficult to interpret this finding and [it] is in need of replication (for example[,] in a study with more power).” The authors very much regret any inconveniences the errors and corrections may have caused and/or cause.

RESULTS OF STUDY 2

Emotional Stroop Effects (ESE)

Based on the same outlier criteria as used in Experiment 1, 8.7% of all trials were discarded (2.3% of all trials were error responses).

A 3 (arousal) × 2 (valence) × 3 (word position) × 2 (level) × 2 (age group) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted (means and standard deviations are displayed in ). Moreover, order (block order: negative/positive vs. positive/negative) as a between-subject factor was included to control for order effects. As expected, we obtained a main effect of age group, F(1, 76) = 48.78, p < .001, ηp2 = .39. Again, older adults were significantly slower than young adults (Myoung = 918 ms, SDyoung = 130 ms vs. Mold = 1,190 ms, SDold = 208 ms).

Table 1. ESEs separately for young and old adults in Study 1 and Study 2

Table 3. Means and standard deviations of RT (in ms) separately for valence and arousal of words and word position for young and older adults in Study 2

There was no evidence for any ESEs—the main effect of arousal and the interaction between arousal and valence were nonsignificant (F(2, 75) = 0.10, p = .90 and F(2, 75) = 0.21, p = .81). Moreover (and in contrast to Study 1), we neither obtained a significant interaction effect between age and arousal, F(2, 75) = 0.59, p = .55, nor a three-way interaction between age, arousal, and valence, F(2, 75) = 0.12, p = .89. The Stroop effects (none of them were significant) are displayed in .Footnote1 Furthermore, we obtained an interaction effect between valence, age group, and order, F(1, 76) = 10.07, p = .002, ηp2 = .12. Older adults were faster on average in the second valence block irrespective of which one they started with. In contrast, young adults were slower in both negative valence blocks compared with the positive valence block.

As in Study 1, we found a main effect of word position, F(2, 75) = 26.76, p < .001, ηp2 = .42, which indicated longer RTs with words in the relevant object (M = 1069 ms) than with words in the irrelevant object (1053 ms) or in the background (1038 ms). This main effect was qualified by an interaction with level and order of word blocks, F(2, 75) = 4.66, p = .012, ηp2 = .11. Differences between levels were larger for words in the background and irrelevant object in the negative/positive order than in the positive/negative order, but the levels did not differ depending on order when the word appeared in the relevant object.

We conducted the same MANOVA for error rates. There was a significant main effect of age group, F(1, 76) = 22.31, p < .001, ηp2 = .23, indicating that older adults overall made fewer mistakes than young adults (Myoung = 3.1% vs. Mold = 1.5%). Moreover, besides significant effects of valence, level, Level × Valence, Level × Age × Order, Position × Order × Arousal, and Arousal × Valence × Order, we obtained a four-way interaction between age, valence, arousal, and level, F(2, 75) = 4.55, p = .014, ηp2 = .11. As in Study 1, the error rate is very low, which is why we refrain from an interpretation of this four-way interaction.

Relationship Between ESEs, Positive and Negative Affect, and FGA

To test group differences, we conducted three univariate analyses of variance (ANOVA) with age (young vs. old) and order of blocks (negative/positive vs. positive/negative) as between-subject factors and negative affect, positive affect, and flexible goal adjustment (FGA), respectively, as dependent variables. Only age was significant for negative affect and FGA. As in Study 1, older adults reported significantly less negative affect in the last 12 months than young adults, F(1, 76) = 7.10, p < .01 (see Table 2 in Kappes & Bermeitinger, Citation2016). Moreover, older adults reported significantly higher flexible goal adjustment values than young adults, F(1, 76) = 8.50, p < .01.

As in Study 1, we were interested in the relationship between ESE, positive and negative affect, and FGA. In a first step, we tested correlations between the four ESEs, positive and negative affect, and FGA. As we also wanted to test whether the results of Study 1 with negative (compared with neutral) stimuli could be replicated, we also conducted analyses separately for age groups and order of valence blocks (see ).

Table 4. Summary of intercorrelations of NA, PA, and FGA with the different ESEs overall and separately for age and order groups in Study 2

Negative Affect

Overall, there was no significant correlation between ESEs and negative affect obtained. The correlation effect between the ESEmed/neg and negative affect for the old age group in the negative/positive block order was in the same direction as in Study 1 but was not significant (r(17) = −.24, p = .33).

Positive Affect

In contrast to Study 1, we obtained a negative correlation between the ESEhigh/neg and positive affect in the young age group with negative/positive order of blocks (r(19) = −.48, p = .029), meaning that a larger ESE with these words was related to less positive affect in the last 12 months.

Flexible Goal Adjustment

Regarding FGA, we only found a significant negative relationship between the ESE for high arousing negative words and FGA, r(78) = −.26, p = .019, that is, the more pronounced flexible goal adjustment was, the smaller was the ESE for negative high arousing words. Replicating the findings of Study 1, the old age group in the block order group confronted with negative word blocks first followed by positive word blocks exhibited a marginally significant positive correlation between ESEmed/neg and FGA (r(17) = .41, p = .081). However, this effect was not significantly different from the old age group in the opposite block order group (r(18) = −.13; Z = 1.61, p = .11). For correlations between ESEhigh/pos and FGA, they showed the same pattern. Older adults in the negative/positive order group had a positive correlation (r(17) = .22, p = .37), whereas the other old age group had a negative correlation (r(18) = −.49, p = .029), and the correlations differed significantly from each other, Z = 2.18, p = .03. Thus, higher values in flexible goal adjustment were related to a smaller ESE for high arousing positive words in the older age group, when they saw positive word blocks first.

Relationship Between ESEs and Change in Momentary Affect

In a last step, we were interested in the relationship between the ESEs and (change in) momentary affect in response to the emotional Stroop task. First, we analyzed differences between baseline valence level and valence after the first two and second two blocks. We conducted a 2 (age group: young vs. old) × 2 (block order: negative/positive vs. positive/negative) × 3 (time: baseline valence vs. valence after first two blocks vs. after second two blocks; within-subject) repeated-measures ANOVA with valence as dependent variable. We obtained a significant interaction between time and age group, F(2, 152) = 7.45, p < .01, ηp2 = .09 (see Table 5 in Kappes & Bermeitinger, Citation2016). Although valence decreased in the young age group between baseline and the Stroop task (Mbaseline = 5.7 vs. M1st blocks = 5.2 vs. M2nd blocks = 5.2), it increased in the older age group (Mbaseline = 5.4 vs. M1st blocks = 5.7 vs. M2nd blocks = 5.7). Moreover, there was a significant interaction effect between age and order of blocks, F(1, 76) = 4.23, p < .05, ηp2 = .05. Young adults in the negative/positive words block order experienced the least positive affect overall (young: Mneg/pos = 4.8 vs. Mpos/neg = 5.6; old: Mneg/pos = 5.9 vs. Mpos/neg = 5.4).

To test the relationship between changes in momentary affect and the different ESEs, we calculated the difference between valence levels after the first two blocks and baseline and the difference between valence levels after the second two blocks and valence after the first two blocks. In order to examine changes in momentary affect after confrontation with either negative or positive words, we restructured the data set to obtain one variable signifying change after negative word blocks and one representing change after positive word blocks.Footnote2 We conducted a regression analysis and regressed the difference variable after negative word blocks on the ESEmed/neg (z-standardized), age (effect-coded: young = −1, old = 1), and their interaction. The age groups differed significantly in the change of momentary affect after completing negative word blocks (B = 0.29, β = 0.23, p = .043), with older adults experiencing less decrease in momentary affect than young adults (Myoung = −0.66 vs. Mold = −0.08). The influence of ESEmed/neg differed for the age groups, as indicated by a marginally significant interaction effect (B = 0.25, β = 0.20, p = .077). As displayed in , in the older age group, the ESEmed/neg was positively related to change in momentary affect (B = 0.27), which means the larger the ESE, the more positive is the change in momentary affect (i.e., the slower participants responded to negative medium arousing words in comparison with neutral words, the more increased their momentary affect). In contrast, younger adults showed the reversed pattern (B = −0.24). We repeated this analysis with ESEhigh/neg; only the age group effect was significant (see ).

Table 6. Predictors of change in momentary affect after negative word blocks in Study 2

Table 7. Predictors of change in momentary affect after positive word blocks in Study 2

Figure 3. Change in momentary affect in Study 2 after (a) negative word blocks as a function of ESE for negative medium arousing words separately for the age groups, and (b) after positive word blocks as a function of ESE for positive high arousing words separately for the age groups.

Figure 3. Change in momentary affect in Study 2 after (a) negative word blocks as a function of ESE for negative medium arousing words separately for the age groups, and (b) after positive word blocks as a function of ESE for positive high arousing words separately for the age groups.

We conducted the same analyses for change in momentary affect after completing positive word blocks. As can be seen in , only age group was marginally significant (B = 0.39, β = 0.22, p = .053). Young adults momentary affect decreased, whereas that of older adults increased (Myoung = −0.29 vs. Mold = 0.44). More importantly, the ESEhigh/pos showed differential effects depending on age (). In the young age group, the ESE was positively related to change in momentary affect (B = 0.82), that is, the more delayed response times to high arousing positive words were in comparison with neutral words, the more increased their momentary affect. In contrast, momentary affect decreased in the older age group as a function of the ESEhigh/pos (B = −0.49).

Notes

1 As in Study 1, a main effect of object level was qualified by an interaction with age, F(1, 76) = 10.36, p = .002, η2 = .12. Participants responded faster when the relevant object was in the front than in the rear. This difference was larger for older adults.

2 Prior analyses included order of blocks (effect-coded: negative/positive = −1, positive/negative = 1) and its interaction terms. Order had no significant influence.

REFERENCE

  • Kappes, C., & Bermeitinger, C. (2016). The emotional Stroop as an emotion regulation task. Experimental Aging Research, 42, 161–194. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2016.1132890

Appendix A. Stimuli used in Studies 1 and 2

Appendix B. Mean arousal ratings, valence rating, and word frequency by arousal and valence condition for age groups separately

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.