Publication Cover
Experimental Aging Research
An International Journal Devoted to the Scientific Study of the Aging Process
Volume 49, 2023 - Issue 5
1,085
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Association Between Neuropsychological Functions and Activities of Daily Living in People with Mild Cognitive Impairment

ORCID Icon, & ORCID Icon
Pages 457-471 | Received 14 Jul 2021, Accepted 03 Oct 2022, Published online: 14 Oct 2022

ABSTRACT

Objective

The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between performance in neurocognitive variables and daily functioning (basic or b- and instrumental or i-ADL) in people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

Methods

A sample of 157 participants with MCI (73.65 ± 7.58 years) completed a battery of tests for assessing ADL and cognitive functions. t-test, Pearson’s correlation and multiple linear regression (backward stepwise selection) were used for data analyses.

Results

Significant correlations were found between b- and i-ADL, and several neuropsychological tests (p < .01). Multivariate analysis showed that difficulties in Blessed Rating Scales (BLS) explained 33.2% of the variation in b-ADL and that this variation rises to 42.9% when BLS is associated with Frontal Assessment Battery Flexibility, Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) and BLS Personality. For i-ADL, BLS and Dementia Rating Scale Total (DRS-T) explained 47.7% of the variation and the inclusion in the model of BLS, DRS-IP (Initiation/Perseveration), TMT-A and BLS Personality explained 53.5% of this variation. Executive functions explained 24.8% of the variation in i-ADL.

Conclusions

Cognitive functions are related to i- and b-ADL in people with MCI. The general indicators and those that assess executive functions and verbal- or visual-spatial memory should be considered to predict i-ADL.

Introduction

Basic activities of daily living (b-ADL) are focused on personal care and self-maintenance skills, while instrumental activities of daily living (i-ADL) are more complex, by allowing the integration of a person in the community, managing their home and their life (Cornelis, Gorus, Beyer, Bautmans, & De Vriendt, Citation2017). The literature points out that i-ADL are more sensitive to the effects of mental decline (Bangen et al., Citation2010; Luck et al., Citation2011). Cognitive limitations are able to predict functional decline (Cornelis, Gorus, Van Schelvergem, & De Vriendt, Citation2019; Jekel et al., Citation2015; Pérès et al., Citation2008), even in the presence of subjective complaints of decline (Atkins et al., Citation2018; Roehr, Riedel-Heller, et al, Citation2019). Both b- and i-ADL can contribute to the diagnostic differentiation between cognitively healthy aging and neurocognitive disorders in old age (Cornelis et al., Citation2017).

Cognitive decline is not the inevitable result of aging; rather it is an impairment (Bangen et al., Citation2019; Lindeboom & Weinstein, Citation2004). The symptoms often appear in a continuum in which the impairment is functional, but cognitive too (Amanzio et al., Citation2018). Neurocognitive disorders can range from a mild to a major degree of limitations (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is made after evidence of a small degree of cognitive decline, from a previous level of performance, in one or more cognitive domains, such as complex attention, executive function, learning and memory, language, perceptual-motor function or social cognition (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). The diagnostic criteria also consider that the i-ADL are reasonably preserved, and that decline cannot be better explained by other mental disorders.

An association has been found between deficits in i-ADL and MCI, namely in those functions with higher cognitive demands (Perneczky et al., Citation2006; Reppermund et al., Citation2013, Citation2011; Schmitter-Edgecombe, McAlister, & Weakley, Citation2012). The i-ADL requiring more demanding neuropsychological functioning, such as financial capacity, appears to be more severely affected in patients with MCI. In particular, the reduction of i-ADL seems to be related with deficits in executive functions, namely in the ability to inhibit responses, self-monitoring and flexibility (Amanzio et al., Citation2018). It has been reported that executive functions are less related to b-ADL than to i-ADL (Cornelis et al., Citation2019). Recent studies have suggested that MCI patients made less advantageous decisions when the information was more complex and required the availability of executive functions (Pertl, Benke, Zamarian, & Delazer, Citation2017).

The question as to which mental functions are most often compromised in MCI is controversial. MCI and the initial stage of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) are marked by the progressive deterioration of episodic memory (Gainotti, Quaranta, Vita, & Marra, Citation2014; Loewenstein et al., Citation2006) and working memory (Aretouli & Brandt, Citation2010; Farias et al., Citation2006). Although memory complaints are one of the first clinical symptoms in patients with MCI, impaired planning (Labyt et al., Citation2004), inhibitory control (Brosseau, Potvin, & Rouleau, Citation2007; Seidler et al., Citation2010), memory and learning, visuomotor coordination and visuospatial processing has also been identified in such patients (Cornoldi, Bassani, Berto, & Mammarella, Citation2007; Drag et al., Citation2016; Tippett, Krajewski, & Sergio, Citation2007; Verheij et al., Citation2012). Additionally, emotional symptoms have been related with MCI (Burton, O’Connell, & Morgan, Citation2018; Ginsberg et al., Citation2019).

The association between functional impairment and neuropsychological variables is relevant for the diagnosis and prognosis of MCI (Amanzio et al., Citation2018). It is therefore very useful to compare the performance in different neuropsychological variables with the everyday functioning, reported by the patients themselves and by their caregivers (Jekel et al., Citation2015). The clinical stage of MCI worsens as ADL and neuropsychological functions become more compromised (Petersen et al, Citation2018). By knowing better the neuropsychological profile and the ADL impaired in the MCI, it is possible to anticipate proper interventions (Tulliani et al., Citation2019), including social community programs or training programs targeting specific cognitive functions. Furthermore, there have been few studies that attempted to predict ADL from other neuropsychological functions such as attention, visuospatial reproduction, abstraction, verbal and visuospatial memory and humor (Stephan et al., Citation2013).

Therefore, the aim of this study was to explore the relationship between performance in neurocognitive variables and daily functioning in b-ADL and i-ADL in people with MCI. Specifically, this investigation examined how these neurocognitive variables contribute to the variation of b-ADL and i-ADL.

Methods

Study Design and Participants

Participants in this cross-sectional, observational and quantitative study were recruited from five private clinics in the district of Santarém, in central Portugal. All participants requested the evaluation by the neuropsychology service throughout the years 2017, 2018 and 2019. Subsequently, between September and December 2019, all participants were contacted, having agreed to be included in this study and provided signed informed consent.

A total of 157 participants were selected based on the following inclusion criteria: a) adults over 60 years old; b) average values between 0.5 and 1.0 (very mild to mild dementia) on the Clinical Dementia Rating (Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, Citation1982); and c) being diagnosed with MCI according to the characteristics presented in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013).

Procedures

The collection of cognitive and functional data was carried out over two sessions, with a duration of about one hour each. The questionnaires related to functionality were completed in the presence of the patient and the caregiver at the beginning of the first session. For the collection of neurocognitive data, the interviewer and the patient were present in a clinical office, free of excessive stimuli. Both the collection and the quotation of the data were performed by one of the authors of this article, who is a psychologist and has the advanced specialty of neuropsychology by the Portuguese Psychologists Order. The evaluation followed the 14 guidelines for the evaluation as defined by the American Psychological Association (American Psychological Association, Citation2012).

Instruments

Activities of Daily Living

- Katz index (b-ADL). The Daily Living Activities Index was developed to measure the physical functioning of patients with chronic illness (Katz, Ford, Moskowitz, Jackson, & Jaffe, Citation1963). Six activities considered basic in everyday life were quoted: washing, dressing, using the toilet, mobility, being continent and eating (Chakraborty, Citation2018). Each activity receives a score from 1 (most independent level) to 3 (most dependent level). The total score of 6 indicates independence, 7–10 partial dependence, 11–17 dependence and above 17 total dependence.- Lawton-Brody scale (i-ADL). This instrument assesses the level of independence of the elderly person regarding instrumental activities (Lawton & Brody, Citation1969) and is a valid instrument for differentiating between decline and dementia (Roehr et al, Citation2019). It comprises eight tasks: using the phone, shopping, preparing meals, taking care of the house, washing clothes, using transportation, responsibility for own medication and the ability to handle money. The index varies between 8 and 30 points according to the following cutoff points: 8 points, independent; 9–20 points, moderately dependent and more than 20 points, severely dependent.

Clinical Evaluation

- Blessed Rating Scales (BLS). BLS were used to assess functional and emotional changes in patients with dementia. These scales assess changes in the performance of daily activities, habits, personality, interests and motivations. BLS have been validated through their correlation with neuropathological markers of AD (Blessed, Tomlinson, & Roth, Citation1968). Scores greater than 9 indicate typical dementia changes. When the diagnosis is not fully determined, it is expected to find indices of 4 to 9 points. Scores less than 4 determine the absence of decline.- Phototest. This test allows the detection of cognitive impairment and dementia. The Phototest has three sub-tests: denomination, verbal fluency and free and facilitated recall. It is very brief and easy do administer, applicable to illiterates, not influenced by educational variables and useful for the identification of cognitive impairment and dementia (Carnero Pardo, Carrera Muñoz, Triguero Cueva, López Alcalde, & Vílchez Carrillo, Citation2018; Carnero Pardo et al., Citation2011, Citation2007). According to the authors, the discriminative validity of the Fototest is good, equal to or greater than that of the generalized employment tests in our environment, with the advantage that it is not influenced by the educational level of the subjects. Scores below 28 may indicate cognitive impairment. – Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). This test consists of five subscales: Attention (DRS-A), Initiation/Perseveration (DRS-IP), Construction (DRS-Const), Conceptualization (DRS-C) and Memory (DRS-M) (Pedraza et al., Citation2010). The DRS is able to differentiate between patients with MCI, patients with mild AD and the control group (Jurica, Leitten, & Mattis, Citation2001; Woolf et al., Citation2016). The second version of DRS was considered a general measure of cognitive ability for adults with low levels of cognitive functioning and neurological cortical or subcortical changes of a degenerative type (Jurica et al., Citation2001; Pedraza et al., Citation2010; Woolf et al., Citation2016). The gross results of each sub-scale are converted into adjusted scores and scalar scores, which in the Portuguese version (Cavaco & Teixeira-Pinto, Citation2011), are adjusted for age and education. Scalar scores have the following clinical interpretation: 2 to 3, significant deficit; 4 to 5, moderate deficit; 6 to 8, slight deficit; 9 to 10, low average; 11 to 13, on average and 14 to 18 points, above average. It was considered a general measure of cognitive ability for adults with low levels of cognitive functioning and degenerative-type cortical or subcortical neurological changes (Jurica et al., Citation2001).- Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF). The objective of this test is the differential diagnosis between constitutional mental deficit and the deficit acquired as a result of traumatic brain injury (Cruz, Da, Toni, Marco de, & Daiani Martinho de, Citation2011). The ROCF allows the assessment of perceptual activity and visual memory (Kasai et al., Citation2006). The application takes place firstly in the presence of the figure. Soon after, with an interval of time not exceeding 3 minutes, the reproduction is made from memory. The total score results from the sum of all the points achieved by the subject.- Trail Making Test (TMT). The TMT has two parts: part A (TMT-A) measures attention, visual perception, visuo-motor speed and information processing while part B (TMT-B) evaluates working memory and executive functions (Cavaco et al., Citation2013; Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, Citation2004). This test is highly sensitive to a variety of neurological conditions and their preclinical manifestations, including dementia. Both TMT-A and TMT-B figures are composed of 25 circles, TMT-A numbered from 1 to 25 and TMT-B including numbers and letters. The task is to connect the circles in ascending order; in TMT-B it is also required alternating between numbers and letters. The result is obtained from the number of seconds required to complete the task.-Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB). This battery is a brief administration instrument, originally intended for the assessment of executive functions in patients with dementia. Includes 6 sub-tests that assess different skills: conceptualization, mental flexibility, programming, sensitivity to interference, inhibitory control and autonomy (Dubois, Slachevsky, Litvan, & Pillon, Citation2000; Hisano, Citation2018). Each subtest is rated with a maximum of 3 points, and the total score can reach 18 points. The score from 16 to 18 remains in the middle range, whereas scores below 15 may indicate a dysfunctional pattern in the frontal regions of the patient’s brain.

- Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). BDI-II assesses the intensity of depression in clinical and non-clinical patients (Beck, Steer, & Brown, Citation1996). It is a measure of negative cognitions, that is, persistent, imprecise and intrusive negative thoughts about the self. Depression is understood by BDI-II as having two components, the affective and the physical or somatic. This inventory contains 21 questions scored from 0 to 3. Scores below 13 may indicate absence or minimal depression; from 14 to 19 mild; 20 to 28 moderate and 29 to 63 several depression.

Data Analysis

The assumption of normality regarding the distribution of the data of each variable was evaluated using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Initially, a descriptive analysis of demographic and clinical characteristics was carried out. For the comparison of variables between the groups of dependent and independent participants (according to b-ADL and i-ADL scores), we used the t-test for independent samples. Pearson’s correlation was used to analyze the intensity and direction of the relationship between the neuropsychological variables and the b-ADL and i-ADL scores. Correlation thresholds of 0.10 are small, 0.30 moderate and 0.50 high (Cohen, Citation1988).

For multivariate analysis, multiple linear regression (backward stepwise selection) was computed to examine which neuropsychological variables contribute significantly to the activities of daily living reflected in the b-ADL and i-ADL scores. Stepwise regression is used to select useful subsets of variables and to evaluate the order of importance of variables (Thompson, Citation1995). We developed seven linear regression models. For each model, b-ADL and i-ADL were considered separately as dependent variables. The independent variables considered were the general neurocognitive indicators composed by BLS, Phototest and DRS-T (model 1); all scales of DRS (model 2); indicators of executive functions, assessed by DRS-IP, FAB and TMT-B (model 3); performance in the verbal and visual-spatial memory subtests – DRS-M, ROCF Memory and Phototest (model 4); performance in visuo-graphical reproduction – DRS-Const, ROCF Reproduction and TMT-A (model 5); the assessment of mood and personality – BDI and BLS Personality (model 6) and a final model (model 7) in which all the significant variables detected in the previous models were included.

Statistical analyses were performed using the statistical package SPSS v.24 (IBM, New York, USA). For all tests, the significance level was set at p < .05.

Ethical Considerations

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Évora (ref: 12271), welcomed by the Research Unit of the Comprehensive Health Research Center and followed the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided signed informed consent.

Results

Of the 157 participants in this study, 107 were women and 50 men, with an average age of 73.6 years old (60 to 90 years) and a school level below the 6th year (5.8 ± 3.2). Seventy-two of the subjects reported a family history of neuro-degenerative disorders with impaired memory.

As illustrated in , overall, the i-ADL, verbal and visuospatial memory and executive functions were the variables with greater clinical deficit. Performance in the Phototest was above the cutoff score for this test. The most defective scales of the DRS were DRS-M, . DRS-Const and DRS-A were above the cutoff. The visual-graphic reproduction and the visual-spatial memory evaluated by the ROCF presented low values, manifesting clinical impairment. The time taken to perform TMT-A was above the cutoff, revealing a deficit. On average, participants had BDI scores that indicate mild depression.

Table 1. Scores for activities of daily living (ADL) and clinical assessment of participants with mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

In b-ADL, 41 subjects were considered dependent and 116 independent, whereas in i-ADL, 123 individuals were considered dependent and 34 independent. In , we can observe that the scores on the BLS, ROCF Memory and TMT-A were statistically different between the groups classified as dependent or independent, according to the b-ADL. Regarding the i-ADL, we found that the scores in BLS, Phototest, DRS-IP, DRS-C, DRS-M, DRS-T, visuo-graphic reproduction and visuospatial memory (ROCF), FAB and TMT-A were higher (p < .05) in the independent group in comparison with the dependent group.

Table 2. Clinical characteristics by level of dependence of participants with mild cognitive impairment (MCI).

shows the association between scores on b- and i-ADL and on the other neurospsychological variables. The results reveal that people with higher values on b-ADL had higher scores on BLS and TMT-A (p< .01) and worse performance on Phototest, reproduction of ROCF and FAB (p < .05). On the other hand, people with higher values on i-ADL had higher scores on BLS and TMT-A and worse results on Phototest, DRS-IP, DRS-M, DRS-T, ROCF Reproduction, ROCF Memory and FAB (p < .01). Also, people with higher values on i-ADL had worse results on DRS-C (p < .05).

Table 3. Pearson’s correlation between the neuropsychological variables and the basic activities of daily living (b-ADL) and instrumental activities of daily living (i-ADL) scores.

The results of the multivariate analysis (backward method) are shown in . In both cases, multiple linear regression was used to verify whether the independent (quantitative) variables described in each model can predict the dependent variable b-ADL or i-ADL. Five models were statistically significant for the b-ADL: Model 1 containing general neurocognitive indicators (F (1.155) = 77.117; p < .001; R2 = 0.332); model 3, performance in executive functions (F (2.113) = 4.243; p = .017; R2 = 0.070); model 5, visuo-graphic reproduction (F (1.130) = 10.993; p = .001; R2 = 0.078); model 6, mood and personality (F (1.142) = 9.103; p = .003; R2 = 0.060) and model 7, significant variables detected in the previous models (F (4.142) = 26.627; p < .001; R2 = 0.429).

Table 4. Multiple linear regression (backward stepwise) of neuropsychological function as a predictor for daily functioning basic activities of daily living (b-ADL).

Table 5. Multiple linear regression (backward stepwise) of neuropsychological function as a predictor for daily functioning instrumental activities of daily living (i-ADL).

For the i-ADL, seven models were statistically significant: Model 1 (F (2, 154) = 70.277; p < .001; R2 = 0.477); model 2 (F (2.154) = 9.406; p < .001; R2 = 0.109); model 3 (F (3.143) = 15.721; p < .001; R2 = 0.248); model 4 (F (2,137) = 14.099; p < .001; R2 = 0.171); model 5 (F (1,130) = 28.017; p < .001; R2 = 0.177); model 6 (F (1.142) = 22.200; p < .001; R2 = 0.135) and model 7 (F (4.99) = 28.482; p < .001; R2 = 0.535). The variables described above contributed to each model.

Discussion

This study relates different neuropsychological functions to everyday functioning assessed by the b-ADL Katz Index and the i-ADL Lawton & Brody Scale, in people with MCI. Furthermore, the present study also predicts how these mental functions contribute to the variation of b- and i-ADL performance.

Our results are aligned with the literature, which refers to i-ADL as more sensitive than the b-ADL to the effects of mental decline (Cornelis et al., Citation2019; Jekel et al., Citation2015; Pérès et al., Citation2008; Perneczky et al., Citation2006; Reppermund et al., Citation2013, Citation2011; Schmitter-Edgecombe et al., Citation2012). In fact, 73.9% of participants showed independence in b-ADL, while these results dropped to 21.7% in i-ADL. The correlations between everyday functioning and neuropsychological had less significant associations and less intensity for b-ADL compared to i-ADL. However, TMT-A (attention, visual perception, speed of visual-motor speed, and information processing) and BLS (functional and emotional changes) were positively associated with b-ADL. Also, Phototest (screening test), ROCF Reproduction (perceptual activity) and FAB (executive function), where higher scores indicate better performance, were negatively related to b-ADL. For the more complex i-ADL, the associations with neuropsychological measures were more intense. Significant and positive associations were found between i-ADL and functional and emotional changes and attention, where higher scores indicate more severe impairment. The performance in general neurocognitive indicators, in tests that assess verbal and visuo-spatial memory, executive functions and visuo-graphic reproduction, were negatively associated with i-ADL. Attention, construction, ability to switch between sequences or mood did not seem to have any relationship with i-ADL.

In the present study, the subdomains of i-ADL with the most deficits were responsibility for own medication, shopping and preparing meals, which represent activities that appeal to cognitive abilities such as executive functions, mental flexibility, sensitivity to interference, inhibitory control and information processing, as well as verbal and visuospatial memory. Overall, these findings are in accordance with previous studies (Amanzio et al., Citation2018; Perneczky et al., Citation2006; Reppermund et al., Citation2013, Citation2011; Schmitter-Edgecombe et al., Citation2012). We also found two studies (Mariani et al., Citation2008; Pedrosa et al., Citation2010) showing that there is a significant impairment in handling economy in people with MCI, which was not confirmed in the present study. According to the literature, subjects with higher executive dysfunctions tend to have more limitations in i-ADL (Cornelis et al., Citation2017, Citation2019). Verbal and visuospatial memory difficulty has been also associated with increase in i-ADL difficulty (Cordier, Chen, Clemson, Byles, & Mahoney, Citation2019; Cornoldi et al., Citation2007). On the other hand, it was found that having more difficulties in i-ADL, especially in those tasks with higher cognitive demand, was associated with overall cognitive functioning (Reppermund et al., Citation2011). These findings have implications for the diagnostic determination of MCI, suggesting that the difficulties in everyday tasks are more likely observed in activities with higher cognitive demand, especially those involving executive functions and memory.

The multivariate analysis carried out in this study showed that difficulties in functional and emotional changes explain 33.2% of the variation in b-ADL. This variation rises to 42.9% when BLS is associated with lexical fluency test, TMT-A and BLS Personality. All other models computed by linear regression analysis proved little ability to predict b-ADL. On the other hand, models 2 and 4, which included the DRS scales and the memory assessment tests, did not present any predictive value. Regarding the i-ADL, we found that models 1 and 7 were the most predictive. A general neurocognitive tests explained 47.7% of the variation in i-ADL, and a group of significant variables detected in the previous models as BLS, DRS-IP, TMT-A and BLS Personality explained 53.5% of this variation. The model that includes the variables measuring executive functions explained 24.8% of the variation in i-ADL. All other models proved to be more limited to predict dependence in i-ADL. In the present study, was found a modest result of 10.9% of the different scales of DRS to predict i-ADL. Our results did not confirm previous investigations that the DRS scores predicted functional decline, showing good clinical validity to detect and classify the severity of cognitive and functional impairment (Katsarou et al., Citation2010; Woolf et al., Citation2016).

When we consider the final statistical models, the BLS and TMT-A were the most significant predictors of everyday functioning, assessed by b- and i-ADL. However, when we focus on i-ADL, other indicators seemed to be relevant, such as DRS-IP, DRS-T, DRS-M, FAB, TMT-B, ROCF Memory and Phototest. These results agree with previous findings suggesting the importance of considering the executive functions to predict i-ADL (Cornelis et al., Citation2019). We also found that memory is important to predict daily activities, which is not in line with previous studies (Amanzio et al., Citation2018). Recognition memory tasks, assessed by DRS, show interference from other intermediate tasks (Pedraza et al., Citation2010). Visuo-spatial memory, evaluated by the ROCF, is a highly complex task (Cruz et al., Citation2011). These results confirm that activities with higher cognitive demands have a higher predictive value for i-ADL. These are domains that may support the selection of techniques for preventing decline and intervention in MCI.

It is important to note that this study includes a sample size which is larger than other similar studies, and the administration of a wide neuropsychological assessment battery. Nevertheless, the present study also included limitations that require further discussion. First, we should mention that the addition of other groups of the population (e.g., AD, or without cognitive impairment) could strengthen the analysis of the association between the clinical neuropsychological assessment and functional decline. Second, to enrich the analysis and verify the predictive power of neurocognitive variables in the face of more complex tasks, it would be interesting to use in the future a scale of advanced ADL. Finally, a future study should better control factors that may influence cognitive functioning, such as comorbidities, vitamin deficiencies, and drugs.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that there is an association between performance on several neurocognitive variables and daily functioning in people with MCI, especially in i-ADL. However, it is recommended to use BLS, DRS-IP, DRS-T, DRS-M, FAB, TMT-A, TMT-B, ROCF Memory, and Phototest to predict activities that require more complex cognitive processes such as i-ADL. The global indicators of clinical evaluation and those that assess executive functions and verbal and visuospatial memory seem to contribute strongly to predict the instrumental functioning of everyday life. These results point to the need for interventions focused on executive functions and memory decline to prevent decrements in the performance of tasks in everyday functioning.

Ethical Principles

The authors affirm having followed professional ethical guidelines in preparing this work. These guidelines include obtaining informed consent from human participants, maintaining ethical treatment and respect for the rights of human or animal participants, and ensuring the privacy of participants and their data, such as ensuring that individual participants cannot be identified in reported results or from publicly available original or archival data.

Role of the Funders/Sponsors

None of the funders or sponsors of this research had any role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Disclosure Statement

Each author signed a form for disclosure of potential conflicts of interest. No authors reported any financial or other conflicts of interest in relation to the work described.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, [R H], upon reasonable request. Please contact https://www.uevora.pr.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the Fundação Ciência e Tecnologia, IP national support through CHRC (UIDP/04923/2020; fundação ciência e tecnologia, ip.

References

  • Amanzio, M., Palermo, S., Zucca, M., Rosato, R., Rubino, E., Leotta, D., … Rainero, I. (2018). Neuropsychological correlates of instrumental activities of daily living in neurocognitive disorders: A possible role for executive dysfunction and mood changes. International Psychogeriatrics, 30(12), 1871–1881. doi:10.1017/S1041610218000455
  • Aretouli, E., & Brandt, J. (2010). Everyday functioning in mild cognitive impairment and its relationship with executive cognition. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25(3), 224–233. doi:10.1002/gps.2325
  • Association, A. P. (2012). Guidelines for the evaluation of dementia and age-related cognitive change. The American Psychologist, 67(1), 1–9. doi:10.1037/a0024643
  • Association, A. P. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author.
  • Atkins, A. S., Khan, A., Ulshen, D., Vaughan, A., Balentin, D., Dickerson, H., … Keefe, R. (2018). Assessment of instrumental activities of daily living in older adults with subjective cognitive decline using the virtual reality functional capacity assessment tool (VRFCAT). The Journal of Prevention of Alzheimer’s Disease, 5(4), 216–234. doi:10.14283/jpad.2018.28
  • Bangen, K. J., Jak, A. J., Schiehser, D. M., Delano-Wood, L., Tuminello, E., Han, S. D., … Bondi, M. W. (2010). Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 16(4), 630–639. doi:10.1017/S1355617710000330
  • Bangen, K. J., Weigand, A. J., Thomas, K. R., Delano-Wood, L., Clark, L. R., Eppig, J., … Bondi, M. W. (2019). Cognitive dispersion is a sensitive marker for early neurodegenerative changes and functional decline in nondemented older adults. Neuropsychology, 33(5), 599–608. doi:10.1037/neu0000532
  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
  • Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., & Roth, M. (1968). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 114(512), 797–811. doi:10.1192/bjp.114.512.797
  • Brosseau, J., Potvin, M. J., & Rouleau, I. (2007). Aging affects motor skill learning when the task requires inhibitory control. Developmental Neuropsychology, 32(1), 597–613. doi:10.1080/87565640701361120
  • Burton, R. L., O’Connell, M. E., & Morgan, D. G. (2018). Cognitive and neuropsychiatric correlates of functional impairment across the continuum of no cognitive impairment to dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 33(7), 795–807. doi:10.1093/arclin/acx112
  • Carnero Pardo, C., Carrera Muñoz, I., Triguero Cueva, L., López Alcalde, S., & Vílchez Carrillo, R. (2018). Normative data for the Fototest from neurological patients with no cognitive impairment. Valores normativos del Fototest en pacientes neurológicos sin deterioro cognitivo (pp. S0213-4853(18) 30086–0). Neurologia (Barcelona, Spain): Advance online publication. doi:10.1016/j.nrl.2018.03.001
  • Carnero Pardo, C., Espejo-Martinez, B., Lopez-Alcalde, S., Espinosa-Garcia, M., Saez-Zea, C., Vilchez-Carrillo, R., … Navarro-Espigares, J. L. (2011). Effectiveness and costs of phototest in dementia and cognitive impairment screening. BMC Neurology, 11, 92. doi:10.1186/1471-2377-11-92
  • Carnero Pardo, C., Sáez-Zea, C., Montiel Navarro, L., Del Sazo, P., Feria Vilar, I., Pérez Navarro, M. J., … Montoro Ríos, M. T. (2007). Utilidad diagnóstica del Test de las Fotos (Fototest) en deterioro cognitivo y demencia [Diagnostic accuracy of the Phototest for cognitive impairment and dementia]. Neurologia (Barcelona Spain), 22(10), 860–869.
  • Cavaco, S., Gonçalves, A., Pinto, C., Almeida, E., Gomes, F., Moreira, I., … Teixeira-Pinto, A. (2013). Trail making test: Regression-based norms for the Portuguese population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 28(2), 189–198. doi:10.1093/arclin/acs115
  • Cavaco, S., & Teixeira-Pinto, A. (2011). Escala de Avaliação da Demência – 2: Manual Técnico. Edições Cegoc (Lisboa).
  • Chakraborty, P. (2018). Activities of daily living among the oldest-old people: A rural urban study. Vol. 5 (J Adv Res Humani Social Sci), 1–4. doi:10.24321/2349.2872.201801
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203771587
  • Cordier, R., Chen, Y. W., Clemson, L., Byles, J., & Mahoney, N. (2019). Subjective memory complaints and difficulty performing activities of daily living among older women in Australia. Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 66(2), 227–238. doi:10.1111/1440-1630.12548
  • Cornelis, E., Gorus, E., Beyer, I., Bautmans, I., & De Vriendt, P. (2017). Early diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and mild dementia through basic and instrumental activities of daily living: Development of a new evaluation tool. PLoS Medicine, 14(3), e1002250. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1002250
  • Cornelis, E., Gorus, E., Van Schelvergem, N., & De Vriendt, P. (2019). The relationship between basic, instrumental, and advanced activities of daily living and executive functioning in geriatric patients with neurocognitive disorders. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34(6), 889–899. doi:10.1002/gps.5087
  • Cornoldi, C., Bassani, C., Berto, R., & Mammarella, N. (2007). Aging and the intrusion superiority effect in visuo-spatial working memory. Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14(1), 1–21. doi:10.1080/138255890969311
  • Cruz, V., Da, Toni, L. P., Marco de, P., & Daiani Martinho de, O. (2011). As funções executivas na Figura Complexa de Rey: Relação entre planejamento e memória nas fases do teste. Boletim de Psicologia, 61(134), 17–30. Recuperado em 19 de maio de 2021: http://pepsic.bvsalud.org/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0006-59432011000100003&lng=pt&tlng=pt
  • Drag, L. L., Light, S. N., Langenecker, S. A., Hazlett, K. E., Wilde, E. A., Welsh, R., … Bieliauskas, L. A. (2016). Patterns of frontoparietal activation as a marker for unsuccessful visuospatial processing in healthy aging. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(3), 686–696. doi:10.1007/s11682-015-9428-y
  • Dubois, B., Slachevsky, A., Litvan, I., & Pillon, B. (2000). The FAB: A frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621–1626. doi:10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621
  • Farias, S. T., Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Harvey, D., Cahn-Weiner, D., & Decarli, C. (2006). MCI is associated with deficits in everyday functioning. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 20(4), 217–223. doi:10.1097/01.wad.0000213849.51495.d9
  • Gainotti, G., Quaranta, D., Vita, M. G., & Marra, C. (2014). Neuropsychological predictors of conversion from mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease: JAD, 38(3), 481–495. doi:10.3233/JAD-130881
  • Ginsberg, T. B., Powell, L., Emrani, S., Wasserman, V., Higgins, S., Chopra, A., … Libon, D. J. (2019). Instrumental activities of daily living, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and neuropsychological impairment in mild cognitive impairment. The Journal of the American Osteopathic Association, 119(2), 96–101. doi:10.7556/jaoa.2019.015
  • Hisano, S. (2018). Relationship between frontal assessment battery scores and activities of daily living/instrumental activities of daily living ability in older adults. Journal of Physical Therapy Science, 30(10), 1237–1240. doi:10.1589/jpts.30.1237
  • Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 140, 566–572. doi:10.1192/bjp.140.6.566
  • Jekel, K., Damian, M., Wattmo, C., Hausner, L., Bullock, R., Connelly, P. J., … Frölich, L. (2015). Mild cognitive impairment and deficits in instrumental activities of daily living: A systematic review. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 7(1), 17. doi:10.1186/s13195-015-0099-0
  • Jurica, P. J., Leitten, C. L., & Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale-2: professional manual. Lutz: psychological assessment resources. (Portuguese version: Cavaco, S., Teixeira-Pinto, A. (2011)) (California).
  • Kasai, M., Meguro, K., Hashimoto, R., Ishizaki, J., Yamadori, A., & Mori, E. (2006). Non-verbal learning is impaired in very mild Alzheimer’s disease (CDR 0.5): Normative data from the learning version of the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 60(2), 139–146. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01478.x
  • Katsarou, Z., Bostantjopoulou, S., Zikouli, A., Kazazi, E., Kafantari, A., Tsipropoulou, V., … Peitsidou, E. (2010). Performance of Greek demented and nondemented subjects on the Greek version of the mattis dementia rating scale. A validation study. The International Journal of Neuroscience, 120(11), 724–730. doi:10.3109/00207454.2010.518779
  • Katz, S., Ford, A. B., Moskowitz, R. W., Jackson, B. A., & Jaffe, M. W. (1963). Studies of illness in the aged. The index of ADL: A standardized measure of biological and psychosocial function. JAMA, 185, 914–919. doi:10.1001/jama.1963.03060120024016
  • Labyt, E., Szurhaj, W., Bourriez, J. L., Cassim, F., Defebvre, L., Destée, A., & Derambure, P. (2004). Influence of aging on cortical activity associated with a visuo-motor task. Neurobiology of Aging, 25(6), 817–827. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.08.010
  • Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9(3), 179–186. doi:10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179
  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological Assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Lindeboom, J., & Weinstein, H. (2004). Neuropsychology of cognitive ageing, minimal cognitive impairment, Alzheimer’s disease, and vascular cognitive impairment. European Journal of Pharmacology, 490(1–3), 83–86. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.02.046
  • Loewenstein, D. A., Acevedo, A., Agron, J., Issacson, R., Strauman, S., Crocco, E., … Duara, R. (2006). Cognitive profiles in Alzheimer’s disease and in mild cognitive impairment of different etiologies. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 21(5–6), 309–315. doi:10.1159/000091522
  • Luck, T., Luppa, M., Angermeyer, M. C., Villringer, A., König, H. H., & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2011). Impact of impairment in instrumental activities of daily living and mild cognitive impairment on time to incident dementia: Results of the Leipzig longitudinal study of the aged. Psychological Medicine, 41(5), 1087–1097. doi:10.1017/S003329171000142X
  • Mariani, E., Monastero, R., Ercolani, S., Rinaldi, P., Mangialasche, F., Costanzi, E., … Mecocci, P.; ReGAl Study Group. (2008). Influence of comorbidity and cognitive status on instrumental activities of daily living in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Results from the ReGAl project. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23(5), 523–530.
  • Pedraza, O., Lucas, J. A., Smith, G. E., Petersen, R. C., Graff-Radford, N. R., & Ivnik, R. J. (2010). Robust and expanded norms for the dementia rating scale. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 25(5), 347–358. doi:10.1093/arclin/acq030
  • Pedrosa, H., De Sa, A., Guerreiro, M., Maroco, J., Simoes, M. R., Galasko, D., & de Mendonca, A. (2010). Functional evaluation distinguishes MCI patients from healthy elderly people–the ADCS/MCI/ADL scale. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 14(8), 703–709. doi:10.1007/s12603-010-0102-1
  • Pérès, K., Helmer, C., Amieva, H., Orgogozo, J. M., Rouch, I., Dartigues, J. F., & Barberger-Gateau, P. (2008). Natural history of decline in instrumental activities of daily living performance over the 10 years preceding the clinical diagnosis of dementia: A prospective population-based study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 56(1), 37–44. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01499.x
  • Perneczky, R., Pohl, C., Sorg, C., Hartmann, J., Komossa, K., Alexopoulos, P., … Kurz, A. (2006). Complex activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment: Conceptual and diagnostic issues. Age and Ageing, 35(3), 240–245. doi:10.1093/ageing/afj054
  • Pertl, M. T., Benke, T., Zamarian, L., & Delazer, M. (2017). Effects of healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment on a real-life decision-making task. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease: JAD, 58(4), 1077–1087. doi:10.3233/JAD-170119
  • Petersen R C et al . (2018). Practice guideline update summary: Mild cognitive impairment. Neurology, 90(3), 126–135. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004826
  • Reppermund, S., Brodaty, H., Crawford, J. D., Kochan, N. A., Draper, B., Slavin, M. J., … Sachdev, P. S. (2013). Impairment in instrumental activities of daily living with high cognitive demand is an early marker of mild cognitive impairment: The Sydney memory and ageing study. Psychological Medicine, 43(11), 2437–2445. doi:10.1017/S003329171200308X
  • Reppermund, S., Sachdev, P. S., Crawford, J., Kochan, N. A., Slavin, M. J., Kang, K., … Brodaty, H. (2011). The relationship of neuropsychological function to instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(8), 843–852. doi:10.1002/gps.2612
  • Roehr, S., Riedel‐Heller, S. G., Kaduszkiewicz, H., Wagner, M., Fuchs, A., van der Leeden, C., … Luck, T. (2019). Is function in instrumental activities of daily living a useful feature in predicting A lzheimer’s disease dementia in subjective cognitive decline? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34(1), 193–203. doi:10.1002/gps.5010
  • Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., McAlister, C., & Weakley, A. (2012). Naturalistic assessment of everyday functioning in individuals with mild cognitive impairment: The day-out task. Neuropsychology, 26(5), 631–641. doi:10.1037/a0029352
  • Seidler, R. D., Bernard, J. A., Burutolu, T. B., Fling, B. W., Gordon, M. T., Gwin, J. T., … Lipps, D. B. (2010). Motor control and aging: Links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 721–733. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005
  • Stephan, C. M., Minett, B., Pagett, T., Siervo, E., Brayne, C, M., & McKeith, I. G. (2013). Diagnosing Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) in clinical trials: A systematic review. BMJ Open, 3(2), e001909. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2012-001909
  • Thompson, B. (1995). Stepwise regression and stepwise discriminant analysis need not apply here: A guidelines editorial. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55(4), 525–534. doi:10.1177/0013164495055004001
  • Tippett, W. J., Krajewski, A., & Sergio, L. E. (2007). Visuomotor integration is compromised in Alzheimer’s disease patients reaching for remembered targets. European Neurology, 58(1), 1–11. doi:10.1159/000102160
  • Tulliani, N., Bissett, M., Bye, R., Chaudhary, K., Fahey, P., & Liu, K. (2019). The efficacy of cognitive interventions on the performance of instrumental activities of daily living in individuals with mild cognitive impairment or mild dementia: Protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. Systematic Reviews, 8(1), 222. doi:10.1186/s13643-019-1135-0
  • Verheij, S., Muilwijk, D., Pel, J. J., van der Cammen, T. J., Mattace-Raso, F. U., & van der Steen, J. (2012). Visuomotor impairment in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease: Changes in relative timing of eye and hand movements. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease: JAD, 30(1), 131–143. doi:10.3233/JAD-2012-111883
  • Woolf, C., Slavin, M. J., Draper, B., Thomassen, F., Kochan, N. A., Reppermund, S., … Sachdev, P. S. (2016). Can the clinical dementia rating scale identify mild cognitive impairment and predict cognitive and functional decline? Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 41(5–6), 292–302. doi:10.1159/000447057