1,050
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research

Effects of a maximal cycling all-out anaerobic test on visual performance

, , , , &
Pages 777-782 | Received 14 Jun 2022, Accepted 22 Nov 2022, Published online: 11 Dec 2022

ABSTRACT

Clinical relevance

All-out exercise may not impair all central nervous system processes, such as those related to visual-motor abilities, and may actually prove stimulatory to such tasks allowing athletes and sports conditioning specialists to develop strategies to take advantage of/mitigate the effects of such exercise on athletic performance.

Background

Despite research indicating that visual-motor abilities play a critical role in athletic performance, research has primarily focused on the effect of all-out exercise on processes along the motor pathway, such as resultant force production or simple cognitive tasks. Such research has neglected to investigate the effect of all-out exercise on visual tasks. When investigations on visual tasks are forthcoming, they focus on prolonged aerobic exercise, which is not the primary metabolic pathway for all, or even the majority of sports.

Methods

Sixty untrained males (experimental group; N = 30, control group; N = 30) completed a standardised six-item baseline sports vision test battery and one week later, the experimental participants returned to undertake a 30-second Wingate anaerobic test (30-WAnT) immediately followed by the same test battery.

Results

Significant (P < 0.05) improvements were found in accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, speed of recognition, peripheral awareness and hand-eye coordination (P < 0.001 for all), but not visual memory (P = 0.242) following the 30-WAnT.

Conclusions

Although the mechanisms underlying these improvements in visual task performance have not yet been studied, this study suggests that simple anaerobic all-out exercise does not cause central- or brain-based fatigue impairing the oculomotor system but may rather provide “excitability” of the underlying motor cortex, motoneurons and/or corticofugal connections utilised in visual task response. It appears that the sweeping improvements in visual task performance elucidate the need for an intense anaerobic warm-up when training visual skills and when visual skills form an integral part of athletic performance.

Introduction

While most research has demonstrated the impact of physical activities on simple cognitive tasks, such research has neglected the effect of fatigue on other important tasks, such as visual tasks. Recent studies have shown that visual-motor abilities play a critical role in athletic performance.Citation1,Citation2 It is for this reason that sport learning theory suggests that athletes need to assess and/or develop not only physical and motor abilities, but also visual and perceptual-cognitive skills.Citation3,Citation4 In this way, the assessment and training of visual skills has aroused the interest of athletes, coaches, sport scientists and vision specialists.Citation5 Further, pre-season baseline testing to highlight any postinjury deficits (i.e. following concussion) has become increasingly prevalent in sports as baseline testing allows clinicians to gauge pre-injury performance.Citation6 Athletes are often administered these baseline tests while at rest, despite injuries routinely occurring and being immediately assessed during physical activity.Citation6 Therefore, adjustments are required to baseline testing to account for any modifying factors that may jeopardise not only performance outcomes, but also injury assessments.Citation7

While fatigue is commonly associated with deleterious physical and cognitive performance, previously, it has been demonstrated that different types of metabolic fatigue, induced by anaerobic alactacid, anaerobic lactacid, sub-maximal aerobic, and maximal aerobic efforts have improved performance of a sensory task (peripheral threshold detection), a sensory-motor task (coincidence-anticipation), and a cognitive task (recall in central vision).Citation8 These differences in the effect of fatigue on physical, cognitive or visual performance may be related to central or peripheral origins of fatigue.Citation9

Studies have demonstrated that induced fatigue may result in deterioration in some visual skills.Citation10 This decrease in visual skill has purported to be as a result of prolonged exercise causing central- or brain-based fatigue in the corticospinal motor system, which in turn, may impair the oculomotor system.Citation10 Previously, a systematic narrative review showed that vision and cognitive disturbances were induced immediately following aerobic exercise bouts.Citation11 Additionally, a 20-minute interval fatigue protocol, utilising running, stepping and calisthenic exercises, has demonstrated that fatigue affects vestibular, and/or the ocular motor system.Citation12 Further, maximal aerobic capacity exercise has shown to decrease decision-making time while simultaneously increasing decision-making errors in athletes.Citation13 While strenuous prolonged aerobic exercise has been reported to affect various aspects of visual function, exercise modality appears to have varied effects. Specifically, while cycling, jogging and stair running have no effect on visual acuity, refractive error, dark focus, amplitude of accommodation or pupil size, contrast sensitivity was found to improve after cycling and jogging, but not after stair running.Citation14 While much research focuses on the effect of prolonged aerobic exercise, this mode is not the primary metabolic pathway for all, or even the majority of sporting codes. While fatigue have been explored following long-duration running or cycling,Citation15 the type of fatigue induced by a short cycling bout is understudied,Citation16 nor have its effects on sports-specific visual task performance. This is particularly important when evaluating the effects of exercise on visual function or evaluating the efficacy of vision training. Further, although these studies provide interesting information about the accumulative effect of several cycling bouts over the muscle fatigue mechanisms, it is still unknown how a single and maximal cycling all-out test affects fatigue.Citation17

Problematically, vision has a multitude of definitions in literature with none being as being as simple as only seeing.Citation5 To date, no research exists on how an all-out anaerobic effort affects visual task performance. Consequently, the objective of this study was to investigate the effects of a maximal cycling all-out test on the performance of several visual tasks. It was hypothesised that an acute maximal cycling all-out test would improve the performance of several visual tasks.

Methods

The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards of the University of Zululand (UZREC 171,110-030-PGD-2021/27) and complied with the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki as revised in 2013. All participants provided oral and written informed consent.

Study participants

Sixty males participated in the study (mean age: 23.11 ± 3.02 years). The inclusion criteria for the study was that the participant had to have a minimum of 20/20 vision (either unaided or corrected with soft contact lenses during the experiment), they should not have participated in any form of structured sport or exercise regime for the past six months, and none of the participants should have had previous experience with sport vision testing.Citation17 Participants were excluded from the study if they did not have 20/20 vision (either unaided or corrected with soft contact lenses during the experiment), had any form of visual disease or infection, physical disability and/or psychosocial distressCitation18 and if they presented with any relative or absolute contraindication to exercise or testing.Citation19 Participants received both written and oral information about the aims, data collection and data management of the project before providing written informed consent and were able to withdraw from the study at any time. Participants were randomly assigned using a random numbers table and divided into an experimental group (N = 30) or a control group (N = 30).

Procedures

Prior to participation in the study, participants underwent an optometric assessment to ensure 20/20 vision. Spectrum Eyecare software (Version 6.0.0, Digital Optometry, South Africa) was used to measure each participant’s depth perception and visual acuity.

The study utilised the Hart Near Far Rock Test to assess accommodation facility, which is the function whereby the refractive power of the optical system of an eye can change, enabling images of both distant and near objects to be viewed clearly.Citation20 The large Hart Chart was placed 3 metres away from the participants on a board, at head height.Citation21 Participants were instructed to hold another smaller chart at arm’s length away, after which they were tasked to read the first letter of the first line of the chart on the board three metres away and then proceed to read the first letter of the chart at arm’s length away. Participants then read the second letter of the first line of the far chart, then the second letter of the first line of the near chart, and so forth for 30 seconds, after which the errors were subtracted from the score to determine the final score.Citation21

To evaluate saccadic eye movement (rapid, ballistic movements of the eyes that abruptly change the point of fixation), the study utilised standardised saccadic eye movement charts.Citation22 Two charts were placed on a board, 1 metre apart, and 3 metres away from the participants and participants were instructed to read the first letter on the lateral side of the left chart, and then rapidly move their eyes and without moving their heads to the first letter on the lateral side of the right chart and read the first letter. The participant then altered focus to the second letter of the left chart once they had read the first letter of the left and right chart and this process continued for 30 seconds. At the conclusion of this test, errors were subtracted from the score to determine the final score. To ensure that participants could not memorise the letters, standardised, yet adjustable saccadic eye movement charts were utilised that had letters going down vertically on both sides of the page.Citation21

Speed of recognition was measured by the Batak Pro, using the Evasion program.Citation23,Citation24 This program lights the 12 LED lights randomly for 1 sec, and the participant was required to strike the target while still lit. If a target was lit but only flickered, participants were not to strike the target and if they did so, 5 points were deducted from the final score.Citation24 In turn, when all of the lights in the centre of the Batak Pro flickered, participants were required to evade the small central infrared beam and if caught by the beam, 5 points were deducted from the final score. All scores were automatically determined by the microcomputer with a maximum of 100 targets being illuminated.

Hand-eye coordination was evaluated using the Ball Wall Toss Test.Citation25 A mark was measured 2 metres away from a wall at which participants were required to throw a standard tennis ball, and catch it, while alternating hands for 30 seconds.Citation21 The amount of successful catches was recorded.

Peripheral awareness was measured using the Accumulator Programme on the Batak Pro.Citation23,Citation26 In this programme, random LED targets illuminated, and remained illuminated until it was struck by the participant. After being struck, another LED target would immediately light up for a period of 60 seconds.Citation24 The Batak Pro microcomputer automatically calculated a final score at the end of the test.

Visual memory was assessed using the Flash Memory Programme on the Batak Pro.Citation24,Citation27 In this program, 6 targets were lit for ½ second. Following this, participants were required to remember the 6 targets that illuminated, as well as the order in which they lit up.Citation24,Citation28 The Batak Pro microcomputer calculated the maximum score at the end of the test.

After a week had passed following baseline visual task testing, participants underwent a 30-WAnT, immediately followed by the same test battery described above. The 30-WAnT was performed on a cycle ergometer (Model 834E, Monark Exercise AB, Vansbro, Sweden) as described by Inbar et al.Citation29 The 30-WAnT was preceded by a five-minute (min) warm-up at the inertial resistance of the equipment, including two bouts of four seconds performed in the final seconds of the second and fourth minutes. After a 10-min rest, the participants were instructed to pedal “all-out” for 30 sec against a resistance of 0.09 kg.kg−1 body mass.Citation29 Verbal encouragement was provided throughout the test. The external power output was calculated every 1 sec. The highest external power output in the first 5 sec of the test was used to represent the peak power (PP), whereas the average power generated over 30 sec was recorded as the mean power (MP).Citation29 In addition, the fatigue index (FI) is reported as the percentage decline from PP to the lowest power produced at the end of the test [(PP-lowest power)/PP × 100)].Citation29 Each participant was then required to begin the sports vision test battery immediately after completing the 30-WAnT. Control group participants returned to undertake the same test battery (without a treatment immediately beforehand) to determine if a learning effect occurred.

Statistical analysis

The Shapiro-Wilk Test was utilised to determine if the data was normally distributed. Dependent and independent t-tests were utilised to determine if any changes occurred at post-test both within- and between-groups, respectively. Hedges’ correction was utilised to determine effect size between measures for baseline and after anaerobic treatment. The substantial effects for φ were divided into more fine-graded magnitudes as follows: 0.20 ≤ φ < 0.50 corresponded to a small effect size, 0.50 ≤ φ < 0.80 corresponded to a medium effect size, and φ ≥ 0.80 corresponded to a large effect size. For all statistical analyses, the results were assumed to be significant at an alpha level of 0.05. The statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics software, version 25 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Results are presented as means and standard deviations.

Results

At baseline, the experimental and control groups were heterogeneous for accommodation facility (P < 0.001) and saccadic eye movement (P < 0.001), but homogenous for speed of recognition (P = 0.836), hand/eye coordination (P = 0.562), peripheral awareness (P = 0.446), and visual memory (P = 0.729). Paired t-test results indicate that for the experimental group for all measures, except visual memory (P = 0.242), statistically significant (P < 0.05) differences existed between the baseline and after-anaerobic treatment (P < 0.001 for all other measures). It therefore appears that the anaerobic treatment significantly influenced visual measures ().

Table 1. Effects of a maximal cycling all-out test on visual task performance.

In the control group, significant changes were found from baseline to re-test for accommodation facility (35.8 ± 4.6 to 38.0 ± 4.2; P = 0.001), saccadic eye movement (38.2 ± 6.9 to 40.6 ± 7.5; P = 0.023), peripheral awareness (65.4 ± 11.7 to 68.8 ± 7.8; P = 0.045), and visual memory (41.6 ± 5.1 to 43.4 ± 6.1; P = 0.021). This indicated that a learning effect did take place from baseline to re-test. No significant changes were observed for speed of recognition(28.8 ± 18.6 to 30.4 ± 20.1; P = 0.200), and hand/eye coordination (23.0 ± 5.3 to 23.6 ± 5.8; P = 0.398).

Independent t-tests found significant differences between the experimental and control groups at post-test for accommodation facility (P < 0.001), saccadic eye movement (P < 0.001), speed of recognition (P = 0.036), peripheral awareness (P = 0.044) and hand-eye coordination (P = 0.042), but not visual memory (P = 0.729). Further post hoc analysis using Hedges’ correction effect size measure indicated a large effect size for accommodation facility (g = −2.692), saccadic eye movement (g = −2.043), speed of recognition (g = −1.365), hand/eye coordination (g = −1.295), peripheral awareness (g = −2.457), and confirmed only a small effect size between visual memory measures for baseline and after the anaerobic treatment (g = −0.212). For the control group, a medium effect size was found between measures for baseline and re-test with re-test providing a slightly higher mean score for accommodation facility (g = −0.659), saccadic eye movement (g = −0.432), peripheral awareness (g = −0.377), and visual memory (g = −0.438).

Discussion

This study used a new experimental approach to assess the effects of a maximal cycling all-out test on the performance of several visual tasks. Results of this study indicate visual task performance increased in five of the six measured parameters, barring visual memory, following a 30-WAnT. While the control group too demonstrated improvements in visual task performance, such improvements were in four of the six measured visual task parameters, namely accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, peripheral awareness, and visual memory, but not speed of recognition and hand/eye coordination. This may represent a rapid learning effect in these visual task tests. Despite the two groups being heterogenous for accommodation facility and saccadic eye movement, with the control group being significantly higher at baseline when compared to the experimental group, the control group still experienced a learning effect in these two parameters. As participants were randomised and not matched for any variables to avoid bias, this statistical heterogeneity for accommodation facility and saccadic eye movement became apparent only after the analysis of the results. Further, this heterogeneity represents clinical differences in the participants and does not represent a sampling error as all participants had no history of such visual task performance testing. While previous investigations on visual testing reproducibility have demonstrated that repeated processing might lead to training effects,Citation30–32 specific investigations regarding reproducibility are not forthcoming on the precise tests utilised in this study. To differentiate the learning effects from the exercise effects, this study demonstrated differences between the experimental and control groups at pre-test for five of the six visual task parameters, namely, accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, speed of recognition, peripheral awareness and hand-eye coordination, but not visual memory. This indicates that the exercise effect was significantly more than the learning effect on these parameters. Further, this study revealed only a medium effect size for learning effect on the measured parameter’s compared to the large effect size for the exercise effect. As such, while the experimental group may have indeed also experienced some learning effect, the additional improvements in visual task performance could be attributed directly to the exercise effect.

Due to the novelty of this study, only the study of Fleury and BardCitation8 supports the findings of this study when they found that anaerobic alactacid and anaerobic lactacid exercise improved sensory task (peripheral threshold detection), a sensory-motor task (coincidence-anticipation), and a cognitive task (recall in central vision) performance. What is particularly noteworthy is that both the present study and that of Fleury and BardCitation8 differ from those studies that have examined the effect of prolonged aerobic exercise. In this regard, research has demonstrated artificial deleterious alterations in visual task performance following prolonged aerobic exercise. Specifically, maximal aerobic exercise has previously produced a limiting effect on cognitive function, impairing immediate memory composite scores, delayed memory composite scores, and verbal memory composite scores.Citation33 Connell et al.Citation34 too have demonstrated an impaired control of eye movement by examining ocular motor system function through saccadic eye movements following a strenuous cycling bout of prolonged exercise. In addition, Fery et al.Citation35 reported that cerebral hypoxia was seen to hamper central aspects of the visual system following prolonged cycling to exhaustion.

From previous studies, it can be seen that visual task performance may be artificially altered either negatively as following prolonged aerobic exercise or positively as following anaerobic exercise. This may be due to differences in central fatigue conditions following either prolonged aerobic exercise or short duration anaerobic exercise. In this regard, fatigue of the muscular system and resultant impairment of excitation-contraction coupling is proportional to the duration of the activity.Citation36 Muscular system fatigue leads to peripheral nervous system fatigue and eventually central nervous system fatigue. It is this central fatigue has been shown to produce vascular changes, hypoxia, decreased cerebral oxygenation, and cerebral cortical activity changes in the brain, which changes overall sensory inflow and alters central processing of afferent input, affecting visual task performance. However, it may be that short-duration exercise, albeit maximal, is not of sufficient duration to fatigue the muscular system sufficiently to result in central nervous system fatigue. In this regard, a study by Decorte et al.,Citation37 found that cycling to exhaustion led to peripheral fatigue that develops early during constant-load intense cycling, while central fatigue appeared to be present towards the end of the exercise after locomotor running.Citation38 It may be that that prolonged running, rather than cycling results in central fatigue and is more likely to result in a deleterious decline in the cognitive process affecting vision. This supposition is plausible due to movement related cortical potentials which validates that movement is preceded and accompanied by brain activities related to the preparation and execution of that movement.Citation39 This supposition is supported by previous research that indicates that visual function is affected by simultaneous physical and mental effort, so that a short-duration task, such as all-out maximal cycling has only minimal mental workload demands and may enhance visual and motor processing depending on the visual parameter tested, and those changes could be related to the activation state of the nervous system.Citation40

In turn, these improvements following anaerobic training might be related to the fact that short-term maximal anaerobic exercise may result in positive excitatory and inhibitory muscle responses, not only at musculoskeletal level,Citation37 but also at central nervous system level. Although the mechanisms underlying these improvements in visual task performance have not yet been studied, it may be that short-term maximal anaerobic exercise provides “excitability” of the underlying motor cortex, “excitability” of the motoneurons utilised in visual task response and an enhanced “strength” of the mono- and oligosynaptic corticofugal connections.Citation37 Irrespective of the underlying mechanism, it appears that the sweeping improvements in visual task performance in this study following a maximal cycling all-out test provides appeal for the use of an intense, short-term warm-up when training visual skills or when visual skills form an integral part of athletic performance.

There were limitations to this research, including the lack of binocular testing, such as cover test, phorias, or vergences as part of the pre-study optometric assessment which could have led to some participants not being screened for conditions, such as strabismus or amblyopia, which could have a significant impact on accommodation, saccades, visual-motor integration, and visual perceptual skills. Further, due to the novelty of this research area, future studies should attempt to determine the duration or time-course effect of visual task improvement following different forms of exercise, such as high-intensity interval training (HIIT). This has obvious implications for sport where athletes presumably will benefit from even a minor improvement in visual skills during competition and where visual task assessments are used during sporting events (i.e. side-line assessments) to assess postinjury deficits.

Conclusion

These findings indicate that different physiological systems may determine visual performance under different exercise conditions. Also, different exercise conditions may affect some, but not all visual test measures. Hence, sport scientists need to consider all visual skills or tasks that an athlete must possess in their given sport and how that sport affect the said visual skills. This study may provide insight on the necessity of specific exercise modalities for use in warm-ups when training visual skills or when visual skills form an integral part of athletic performance. These findings will assist athletes, coaches, sport scientists and vision specialists to understand how exercise-induced fatigue or all-out exercise, and specifically the different types of metabolic fatigue, affects vision in their sporting discipline and enable the active development of strategies to take advantage of or mitigate the effect of all-out exercise on performance. The findings may have clinical relevance where side-line visual assessments are conducted at sporting events to assess postinjury deficits, such as to determine if an athlete has a concussion. These results highlight that if an athlete completes a vision test immediately following an injury/contact, there may be a need to understand the extent of any changes in visual function due to the acute exercise, potential injury, or a degree of both.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, Brandon S Shaw, upon reasonable request.

References

  • Burris K, Vittetoe K, Ramger B et al. Sensorimotor abilities predict on-field performance in professional baseball. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 116. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-18565-7.
  • Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ et al. Visio-spatial skills in athletes: comparison of rugby players and non-athletes. Sport Sci Health 2021; 17: 137–143. doi:10.1007/s11332-020-00663-1.
  • Hadlow SM, Panchuk D, Mann DL et al. Modified perceptual training in sport: a new classification framework. J Sci Med Sport 2018; 21: 950–958. doi:10.1016/j.jsams.2018.01.011.
  • Williams AM, Ericsson KA. Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. Hum Mov Sci 2005; 24: 283–307. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2005.06.002.
  • Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ et al. Factors affecting vision and visio-spatial intelligence (VSI) in sport: a review of the literature. Asian J Sports Med 2020; 11: e101670. doi:10.5812/asjsm.101670.
  • Moran RN, Murray NG, Esco MR et al. Effects of exercise on symptoms, vestibular/ocular motor screening and postural stability in a college-aged sample. Concussion 2020 14; 5: CNC73. doi:10.2217/cnc-2020-0003.
  • Asken BM, Houck ZM, Schmidt JD et al. A normative reference vs. baseline testing compromise for ImPACT: the CARE Consortium Multiple Variable Prediction (CARE-MVP) Norms. Sports Med 2020; 50: 1533–1547. doi:10.1007/s40279-020-01263-2.
  • Fleury M, Bard C. Metabolic fatigue and the performance of visual tasks. Can J Sport Sci 1990; 15: 43–50.
  • O’Leary TJ, Morris MG, Collett J et al. Central and peripheral fatigue following non-exhaustive and exhaustive exercise of disparate metabolic demands. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2016; 26: 1287–1300. doi:10.1111/sms.125821287.
  • Connell CJW, Thompson B, Turuwhenua J et al. Fatigue-related impairments in oculomotor control are prevented by norepinephrine-dopamine reuptake inhibition. Sci Rep 2017; 7: 42726. doi:10.1038/srep42726.
  • Balasundaram AP, Sullivan JS, Schneiders AG et al. Symptom response following acute bouts of exercise in concussed and non-concussed individuals- a systematic narrative review. Phys Ther Sport 2013; 14: 253–258. doi:10.1016/j.ptsp.2013.06.002.
  • Ratka J, Cheever K, Mansell JL et al. The effect of an interval fatigue protocol on Vestibular/Ocular Motor Screening (VOMS) performance. Brain Inj 2020; 34: 110–114. doi:10.1080/02699052.2019.1682194.
  • Thomson K, Watt AP, Liukkonen J. Differences in ball sports athletes speed discrimination skills before and after exercise induced fatigue. J Sports Sci Med 2009; 8: 259–264.
  • Woods RL, Thomson WD. Effects of exercise on aspects of visual function. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 1995; 15: 5–12.
  • Millet GY, Martin V, Lattier G et al. Mechanisms contributing to knee extensor strength loss after prolonged running exercise. J Appl Physiol 2003; 94: 193–198. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00600.2002.
  • Fernandez-Del-Olmo M, Rodriguez FA, Marquez G et al. Isometric knee extensor fatigue following a Wingate test: peripheral and central mechanisms. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2011; 23. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.2011.01355.x.
  • Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ et al. Visual abilities distinguish level of play in rugby. Ann Appl Sport Sci 2020; 8: e906. doi:10.29252/aassjournal.906.
  • West SK, Rubin GS, Bronman AT. How does visual impairment affect performance on tasks of everyday life? Arch Ophthal 2002; 120: 774–780. doi:10.1001/archopht.120.6.774.
  • Riebe D, Ehrman JK, Liguori G et al. ACSM’s guidelines for exercise testing and prescription. 10th ed. Philadelphia (PA): Wolters Kluwer; 2016.
  • Mcbrien NA, Millodor M. The relationship between tonic accommodation and refractive error. Invest Ophthalmol 1987; 28: 997–1004.
  • Du Randt R, Raffan R, Millard L et al. Impact of a visual skills training program on the visual ability of elite rugby players [ dissertation]. Nelson Mandela University; 2016.
  • Purves D, Augustine JG, Fitzpatrick D et al. Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Sunderland: Sinauer Associates; 2001.
  • Lobier M, Dubois M, Valdois S. The role of visual processing speed in reading speed development. Plos One 2013; 8: 58097. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0058097.
  • Quotronics Limited. Batak Pro Manual. Surrey (UK); 2011.
  • Rizzo JR, Beheshti M, Shafieesabet A et al. Eye-hand re-coordination: a pilot investigation of gaze and reach biofeedback in chronic stroke. Prog Brain Res 2019; 243: 361–374. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2019.04.013.
  • Kruger PE, Campher J, Smit CE. The role of visual skills and its impact on skill performance of cricket players. Afri J Phys Health Educ Recr Dance 2009; 15: 605–623. doi:10.4314/ajpherd.v15i4.49548.
  • Shurgin MW. Visual memory, the long and the short of it: a review of visual working memory and long-term memory. Percept Psychophys 2018; 80: 1035–1056. doi:10.3758/s13414-018-1522-y.
  • Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ et al. Differences in visio-spatial expertise between 1st division rugby players and non-athletes. Heliyon 2021; 7: e06290. doi:10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e06290.
  • Inbar O, Bar-Or O, Skinner JS. The Wingate anaerobic test. Champaign IL: Human Kinetics; 1996.
  • Erickson GB, Citek K, Cove M et al. Reliability of a computer-based system for measuring visual performance skills. Optometry 2011; 82: 528–542. doi:10.1016/j.optm.2011.01.012.
  • Foreman K, Baas EA, Sanghera NK et al. Test-retest reliability on the Wayne Saccadic Fixator in professional soccer players. Optometry Visual Perform 2016; 4: 132–136. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029109.
  • Heijl A, Lindgren G, Olsson J. The effect of perimetric experience in normal subjects. Arch Ophthalmol 1989; 107: 81–86. doi:10.1001/archopht.1989.01070010083032.
  • Covassin T, Weiss L, Powell J et al. Effects of a maximal exercise test on neurocognitive function. Br J Sports Med 2007; 41: 370–374. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2006.032334.
  • Connell CJW, Thompson B, Kuhn G et al. Fatigue related impairments in oculomotor control are prevented by caffeine. Sci Rep 2016; 6: 1–9. doi:10.1038/srep26614.
  • Fery YA, Ferry A, Vom Hofe A et al. Effect of physical exhaustion on cognitive functioning. Percept Mot Skills 1997; 84: 291–298. doi:10.2466/pms.1997.84.1.291.
  • Nardone A, Tarantola J, Giordano A et al. Fatigue effects on body balance. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Electromyogr Mot Contr 1997; 105: 309–320. doi:10.1016/S0924980X(97)00040-4.
  • Decorte N, Lafaix PA, Millet GY et al. Central and peripheral fatigue kinetics during exhaustive constant-load cycling. Scand J Med Sci Sports 2012; 22: 381–391. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01167.x.
  • Millet GY, Lepers R. Alterations of neuromuscular function after prolonged running, cycling and skiing exercises. Sports Med 2004; 34: 105–116. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00600.2002.
  • Gandevia SC, Allen GM, Butler JE et al. Supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue: evidence for suboptimal output from the motor cortex. J Physiol 1996; 490: 529–536. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021164.
  • Vera J, Jiménez R, García JA et al. Simultaneous physical and mental effort alters visual function. Optom Vis Sci 2017; 94: 797–806. doi:10.1097/OPX.0000000000001105. PMID: 28708697.