1,498
Views
46
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Greeting: Displaying Stance Through Prosodic Recipient Design

Pages 375-398 | Published online: 07 Nov 2012
 

Abstract

This article examines the social action of greeting in naturally occurring face-to-face interaction, paying special attention to how people prosodically produce their very first vocalized utterances. Close analysis of a corpus of 337 video recorded openings shows that participants recipient design greetings on the level of prosody, tailoring them to each addressee and thus hearably displaying a stance toward the current state and character of their social relationship. Documenting the discovery of a prosodic continuum along which parties fine-tune their greetings, this article elucidates two distinct clusters of prosodic features with which participants recurrently design their greetings. Analysis demonstrates that parties use each prosodic cluster to display a different stance toward encountering the addressed recipient, with prosodically “large” greetings displaying a positive stance of approval and prosodically “small” greetings displaying (no more than) a neutral stance.

Acknowledgments

I am indebted to Emily Elfner for hours of helpful discussion about the technical aspects of this research, and I thank John McCarthy, Mary Clark, Manny Schegloff, and Elizabeth Couper-Kuhlen for generously sharing their wisdom. I also thank the ROLSI reviewers for their thoughtful, detailed comments. Portions of this article were presented at the 2010 International Conference on Conversation Analysis, Mannheim, Germany. I thank the University of New Hampshire Center for Humanities for support.

Notes

1To indicate the type of intonation movement at the end of each TCU, I use the following notation: rise-to-high [?], rise-to-mid [,], level pitch [-], fall-to-mid [;], and fall-to-low [.]. Data in this article also use the British pound sign [£] to indicate smile voice, and an asterisk [*] to indicate onset of visible conduct described inside double parentheses [(())].

2This finding contradicts CitationSacks's (1975, pp. 68–69) claim that “greetings are not repeatably used,” unless Sacks is suggesting only that greetings should not occur directly upon completion of a prior sequence of them.

3Owen's initial and subsequent greetings of Paula are also lexically identical (Hi in Excerpt 3, line 3, and Excerpt 4, line 3) but prosodically distinctive from one another in all of the ways described for Paula. Paula and Owen's greetings thus exhibit striking prosodic similarities and may be analyzable as instances of “prosodic matching,” even though they are virtually simultaneous and not apparently matched as the result of one person “copying” the previous speaker's prosodic pattern (CitationSzczepek Reed, 2006, p. 35).

4The descriptions small and large are designed to specifically avoid imposing unwarranted labels on the data. While other terms exist in the literature for this kind of prosodic clustering (e.g., “phonetically downgraded” and “upgraded,” CitationOgden, 2006), these terms occasion deep analytic and methodological problems when used to analyze the design of first actions—the analytic target of this article.

5More recently, CitationDunbar (2004) suggests that we tend to greet distant acquaintances more effusively than we do those whom we see more often, thereby further bolstering the relationship due to longer time-since-last-contact.

6Within the “small” cluster of prosodic features, a phrase-final fall-to-low helps prosodically project the relevance of stopping to explicitly deal with identification as a discrete interactional activity. This contrasts with the fall-to-mid characteristic of the “large” cluster, which projects immediate movement onto next nonidentification sequences.

7For additional exemplars and analysis of locally subsequent greetings, see CitationPillet-Shore (2008, pp. 150–161).

8This analysis sheds additional light on Excerpt 2: Paula and Derik display that they are having trouble recognizing each other through the way they each prosodically produce their greetings at lines 2–3. Through Paula's use of high onset pitch and overall high pitch setting (see ), however, she does “displaying a positive stance” toward encountering the addressed-recipient—part of doing “being a welcoming host” even to an arriver with whom she is less familiar. In this excerpt, Paula is also the first to display remembering having previously met Derik (lines 10–11), with Derik lagging in his claimed recall of meeting her, which helps explain why he produces his greeting with level pitch.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 387.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.