341
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Reduced impact of imagery processing on false autobiographical recollection: the effects of dynamic visual noise

&
Pages 163-173 | Received 05 Apr 2018, Accepted 25 Jun 2018, Published online: 05 Jul 2018
 

ABSTRACT

Research has demonstrated that merely imagining an autobiographical event can bring about false memories for that event. One explanation for this is that imagination leads to the creation and incorporation of visual-imagistic information into the event representation. This idea was tested in two experiments in which visual-imagery processing was disrupted by the use of Dynamic Visual Noise (DVN). In Experiment 1, autobiographical memories that were rated as “known” and lacking in event detail were subsequently rated as more “remembered” following imagination. In Experiment 2, imagination led to improbable autobiographical events being rated as more believable and vivid. In both experiments, interfering with imagery processing by DVN reduced these effects. It was concluded that visual-imagistic processing plays an important role in altering the mnemonic status of autobiographical representations.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Dr Steve Jachim for the construction of the Dynamic Visual Noise software used in this research. In addition, we would like to thank Kath Walmsley, Lara Shelley, Nicola Diggle, Rachel Russell and Lisha Dahyabhai for assistance with the collection of data for the experiments reported here.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes

1. In Hyman et al. (Citation1998) and Experiment 1 of the current paper, the distinction between remembering and knowing was treated initially in a dichotomous manner (generating memories that were either “remembered” or “known”) and later in a continuous manner (using the 7-point rating scale with 1 as known and 7 as remember). For readers familiar with dual-process models of memory this “cross-classification” and usage of such terms may seem unwarranted. This is because extant theorising about the basis of memory has treated these two forms of memory as qualitatively distinct (dual process) (Yonelinas, Citation20022002), or varying along a continuum of memory trace strength (single process) (Dunn, Citation2004).

However, the purpose of the current work (and Hyman et al., Citation1998) was not to demarcate between dual (vs. single) process accounts of memory. Rather, the initial memory generation and classificatory task, was to allow the production of different types of memory that varied in terms of the details that each contained. For a memory that is generated as “known” the rating of this will likely vary along a continuum depending upon the relative familiarity of the memory. Irrespective of this, the ratings will be more towards the “know” end of the continuum. This was the outcome in Hyman et al. (Citation1998) and Experiment 1 of the present research.

For memories classified as “remembered” it might be conjectured that all ratings should be designated a maximum of 7 on the rating scale (because if a memory is indeed “remembered” and thus rich in episodic detail, then it should not receive a rating score of less than 7). However, remembering (recollection) is not necessarily an all or none process (Yonelinas, Aly, Wang, & Koen, Citation2010). Thus, individuals can remember more or less of the precise episodic details of an event and this will translate into variations in the responses on the rating scale; albeit with all such memories being rated as high on the response sale continuum (Yonelinas et al., Citation2010). This was likely reflected in Hyman et al. (Citation1998) and Experiment 1 of the current work in which the mean of the “remembered” memories was less than 7 and rated as 5.79 (see the upper half of in the result section).

2. Sample sizes were determined based on previous DVN research. The stopping rule was when we reached a minimum of 20 per-between subject group or the end of an academic year, whichever produced the most. The maximum was 30.

3. These analyses were not planned but were suggested by a reviewer. We performed these as it was felt that such relationships might prove to be of interest in the context of this and prior work on autobiographical memory.

4. For Experiment 2, this was partly precluded by the fact that the participant was listening to the recorded description of the events. Of course, it could have been possible to play the event narrative via headphones and record any verbal elaborations. However, doing this may have made the task unduly complex.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 354.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.