1,440
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Editorial introduction

Editorial Introduction to the Special Issue “Advances in the Measurement and Evaluation of Creativity”

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

Creativity measurement: where we were and where we are going

Twenty-four years ago, Plucker and Runco (Citation1998) wrote that the death of creativity measurement had been largely exaggerated. Drawing from the past and interpreting the trends in the creativity research at that time, the two authors were able to forecast the current flourishing of research in creativity, which is strongly driven by the need to develop increasingly reliable methods to assess creative behavior. Indeed, there has been a substantial advance in methodological approaches to the study of creativity in the 24 years since they made this statement. The perceived weakness and low quality of creativity measures (Plucker & Renzulli, Citation1999) have been progressively substituted by the use of robust psychometric approaches and the application of diverse sets of instruments and techniques to creativity assessment (e.g., Dul, Karwowski, & Kaufman, Citation2020; Forthmann & Dumas, Citation2022; Qian, Plucker, & Yang, Citation2019). The illusion, inherited from the intelligence research, of finding a “g” factor of creativity and an accompanying measure to assess it (Piffer, Citation2012) has been forsaken in favor of a more precise theoretical and empirical understanding of the creativity construct, of the related phenomenology, and of the relationship with other closely related constructs (see, for example, the study of serendipity, wonder, inspiration, possibility, etc.; Glăveanu, Citation2018, Citation2019; Mastria, Agnoli, Corazza, Grassi, & Franchin, Citation2022; Ross & Vallee-Tourangeau, Citation2021). The awareness that intelligence and creativity represent two separate constructs has definitively taken root because of the common effort of the creativity research community to understand the relationship between the two (Corazza & Lubart, Citation2020; Karwowski et al., Citation2016; Kaufman, Citation2015; Weiss, Steger, Schroeders, & Wilhelm, Citation2020). In addition, creative cognition begun to find its own place within the domain of human cognition (Benedek & Fink, Citation2019), supported by a more precise understanding of the functional brain mechanisms underlying specific creative behaviors (in particular idea generation, Beaty, Seli, & Schacter, Citation2019; Benedek, Citation2018), giving rise to the creativity neuroscience approach (Saggar, Volle, Uddin, Chrysikou, & Green, Citation2021).

Current trends in the creativity measurement

Instead, therefore, of searching for the most parsimonious way to identify creativity, researchers accepted the inner complexity of the creativity concept embracing the dynamic (Corazza, Agnoli, & Mastria, Citation2022), multidimensional (Lubart, Citation2001), and the socio-cultural nature (Glăveanu et al., Citation2020) of the phenomenon. In the act of dissecting the inherent complexity of such phenomenon, creativity researchers started to learn more about the assessment and measurement of different creative thinking modalities (i.e., divergent thinking or convergent thinking), the stages of the creative process (e.g., problem finding, information processing, idea generation, idea evaluation, etc.), the interactive dynamics between key elements defining a creative performance (e.g., context, personality trait, memory, attention, etc.), and the brain dynamics associated with diverse creative performances and achievements. This flourishing of research is not, however, free from difficulties related to the emerging of new methodological issues. Although it has not been long since the last recent special issue dedicated to creativity measurement and, in particular, to the pitfalls that researchers should avoid in the measurement of this multi-dimensional construct (Barbot & Reiter-Palmon, Citation2019), new trends in the quantification of creativity are constantly emerging; the need to collect them together to develop new ideas, discussions, and research directions for the future is a fundamental contemporary request. In 2021, the Marconi Institute for Creativity (MIC) Conference, whose mission is to provide a global and multidisciplinary forum for presenting and discussing new visions in creativity studies, was specifically devoted to gather the new trends in the creativity evaluation, culminating in the idea of this Special Issue. The creativity research community responded enthusiastically to the call for papers, testifying to the contemporary focus on the relevance of creativity measurement. The Special Issue is divided into two Volumes, the current one published in 2022 and the next one in 2023, both showcasing a series of works dedicated to the current trends in creativity measurement.

Reviewing the works submitted and published in the two volumes three evident trends specifically emerged, which are already well represented by the current volume. The first marked trend is related to the need to reduce the subjectivity in the measurement of idea generation by automatizing the scoring of generated ideas. Hocevar in 1981 highlighted that the first critique of the measurement of creativity is related to the judgments about products, ideas, or other people. He specifically said that “this technique presents a particular problem since the researcher must decide who the judges should be and what the judges should be looking for” (Hocevar, Citation1981, p. 455). In recent year, especially in the domain of divergent thinking, several researchers proposed various approaches to automatize the scoring of generated ideas, strongly reducing the issues related to the subjectivity of judges scoring (e.g., Beaty, Johnson, Zeitlen, & Forthmann, Citation2022; Dumas, Organisciak, & Doherty, Citation2021; Forthmann, Oyebade, Ojo, Günther, & Holling, Citation2019). Interestingly, within this trend, Weinstein, Ceh, Meinel, and Benedek (Citation2022) proposed a new computational approach to extract the criteria defining creativity in a sentence generation tasks. Along the same line, Xie et al. (Citation2022) demonstrated that, compared to the use of human ratings, the objective measurement of creativity based on natural language processing combined with neuroimaging methods provides an opportunity to produce highly replicable findings to quantify creativity, at the cognitive and at the neural level.

The second trend is related to the need of establishing the validity of the creativity measurements. As a first contribution to this trend, Ivcevic (Citation2022), focusing on the relationship between creativity and emotion, through the illustration of several methodological and conceptual examples, demonstrated the need for specificity in creativity research to make reliable and comparable conclusions. Moreover, several creativity measurement methods now have a sufficiently long tradition to allow meta-analytic inferences as well as to be tested with robust statistical approaches. For instance, based on a large sample of participants (N > 26,000), Zielińska, Lebuda, and Karwowski (Citation2022) used an item response theory approach to test the structure and the reliability of the Short Scale of Creative Self (Karwowski, Lebuda, & Wiśniewska, Citation2018), one of the most frequently used scale for the measurement of creative self-concepts.

The third trend emerges from the need to quantify creativity within a broader system of measures. This trend answers to the call by Plucker and Runco (Citation1998) to rely on batteries of assessment to capture the multi-dimensional nature of creativity. Del Missier et al. (Citation2022) adopted a multi-measure approach showing how creative thinking can be explored in a special population (i.e., schizophrenic patients) using a wide array of measures joining the divergent and convergent thinking tests with the measurement of a variety of cognitive, affective, and response inhibition dimensions. Moreover, considering the varied phenomenological expression of creativity, Smith, Pickering, and Bhattacharya (Citation2022) explored the role of daily creative activities on personality and subjective well-being, including daily diary methods into a wider assessment of individual personality and well-being. In the same vein, Lloyd-Cox, Pickering, and Bhattacharya (Citation2022) showed how approaching the study of idea evaluation with a wide assessment of the contextual and interpersonal factors influencing idea estimation can shed new light on the understanding of the concepts of novelty and usefulness.

It is with great enthusiasm that we dedicated the two volumes of this Special Issue to the current trends in the measurement and evaluation of creativity. In the attempt to understand where we stand and to foresight the future of creativity research, we hope you will enjoy the first volume of this Special Issue.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to all the authors who submitted their original works to this special issue. Moreover, we are obligated with the outstanding court of reviewers who invested their time to improve the quality of the submitted papers with their suggestions and expertise, as well as with Wendy Ross for her valuable comments to an early version of this editorial note. We are finally grateful to the staff of the Creativity Research Journal editorial office for their support in the development of this first volume of the Special Issue.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  • Barbot, B., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2019). Creativity assessment: Pitfalls, solutions, and standards. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(2), 131. doi:10.1037/aca0000251
  • Beaty, R. E., Johnson, D. R., Zeitlen, D. C., & Forthmann, B. (2022). Semantic distance and the alternate uses task: Recommendations for reliable automated assessment of originality. Creativity Research Journal, 1–16. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2025720
  • Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2019). Network neuroscience of creative cognition: Mapping cognitive mechanisms and individual differences in the creative brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 22–30. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.08.013
  • Benedek, M. (2018). The neuroscience of creative idea generation. In Z. Kapoula, J. Renoult, E. Volle, & M. Andreatta (Eds.), Exploring Transdisciplinarity in Art and Sciences (pp. 31–48). Basel, CH: Springer.
  • Benedek, M., & Fink, A. (2019). Toward a neurocognitive framework of creative cognition: The role of memory, attention, and cognitive control. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 27, 116–122.
  • Corazza, G. E., Agnoli, S., & Mastria, S. (2022). The dynamic creativity framework. European Psychologist, 27(3), 191–206. doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000473
  • Corazza, G. E., & Lubart, T. (2020). Intelligence and creativity: Mapping constructs on the space-time continuum. Journal of Intelligence, 9(1), 1. doi:10.3390/jintelligence9010001
  • Del Missier, F., Stragà, M., Galfano, G., Venerus, E., Ferrara, D., & Penolazzi, B. (2022). Creativity in schizophrenia: Evidence beyond anecdotes. Creativity Research Journal, 1–15. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2134545
  • Dul, J., Karwowski, M., & Kaufman, J. C. (2020). Necessary condition analysis in creativity research. In V. Dörfler & M. Stierand (Eds.), Handbook of research methods on creativity (pp. 351–368). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing.
  • Dumas, D., Organisciak, P., & Doherty, M. (2021). Measuring divergent thinking originality with human raters and text-mining models: A psychometric comparison of methods. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 15(4), 645–663. doi:10.1037/aca0000319
  • Forthmann, B., & Dumas, D. (2022). Quantity and quality in scientific productivity: the tilted funnel goes Bayesian. Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), 95. doi:10.3390/jintelligence10040095
  • Forthmann, B., Oyebade, O., Ojo, A., Günther, F., & Holling, H. (2019). Application of latent semantic analysis to divergent thinking is biased by elaboration. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(4), 559–575. doi:10.1002/jocb.240
  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2018). The possible as a field of inquiry. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 14(3), 519. doi:10.5964/ejop.v14i3.1725
  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2019). Creativity and wonder. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 53(2), 171–177. doi:10.1002/jocb.225
  • Glăveanu, V. P., Hanchett Hanson, M., Baer, J., Barbot, B., Clapp, E. P., Corazza, G. E., … Sternberg, R. J. (2020). Advancing creativity theory and research: A socio‐cultural manifesto. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(3), 741–745. doi:10.1002/jocb.395
  • Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: Review and critique. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45(5), 450–464. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4505_1
  • Ivcevic, Z. (2022). Conceptual and measurement specificity are key: The case of creativity and emotions. Creativity Research Journal, 1–10. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2122373
  • Karwowski, M., Dul, J., Gralewski, J., Jauk, E., Jankowska, D. M., Gajda, A., … Benedek, M. (2016). Is creativity without intelligence possible? A necessary condition analysis. Intelligence, 57, 105–117. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2016.04.006
  • Karwowski, M., Lebuda, I., & Wiśniewska, E. (2018). Measuring creative self-efficacy and creative personal identity. The International Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving, 28(1), 45–57.
  • Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Why creativity isn’t in IQ tests, why it matters, and why it won’t change anytime soon probably. Journal of Intelligence, 3(3), 59–72. doi:10.3390/jintelligence3030059
  • Lloyd-Cox, J., Pickering, A., & Bhattacharya, J. (2022). Evaluating creativity: How idea context and rater personality affect considerations of novelty and usefulness. Creativity Research Journal, 1–18. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2125721
  • Lubart, T. I. (2001). Models of the creative process: Past, present and future. Creativity Research Journal, 13(3–4), 295–308. doi:10.1207/S15326934CRJ1334_07
  • Mastria, S., Agnoli, S., Corazza, G. E., Grassi, M., & Franchin, L. (2022). What inspires us? An experimental analysis of the semantic meaning of irrelevant information in creative ideation. Thinking & Reasoning, 1–28. doi:10.1080/13546783.2022.2132289
  • Piffer, D. (2012). Can creativity be measured? An attempt to clarify the notion of creativity and general directions for future research. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7(3), 258–264. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2012.04.009
  • Plucker, J. A., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of creativity. In R. J. Stemberg (Ed.), Handbook of human creativity (pp. 35-61). New York: Cambridge University Press .
  • Plucker, J. A., & Runco, M. A. (1998). The death of creativity measurement has been greatly exaggerated: Current issues, recent advances, and future directions in creativity assessment. Roeper Review, 21(1), 36–39. doi:10.1080/02783199809553924
  • Qian, M., Plucker, J. A., & Yang, X. (2019). Is creativity domain specific or domain general? Evidence from multilevel explanatory item response theory models. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 33, 100571.
  • Ross, W., & Vallee-Tourangeau, F. (2021). Catch that word: Interactivity, serendipity and verbal fluency in a word production task. Psychological Research, 85(2), 842–856. doi:10.1007/s00426-019-01279-y
  • Saggar, M., Volle, E., Uddin, L. Q., Chrysikou, E. G., & Green, A. E. (2021). Creativity and the brain: An editorial introduction to the special issue on the neuroscience of creativity. NeuroImage, 231, 117836. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117836
  • Smith, K., Pickering, A., & Bhattacharya, J. (2022). The creative life: A daily diary study of creativity, affect, and well-being in creative individuals. Creativity Research Journal, 1–20. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2122371
  • Weinstein, T. J., Ceh, S. M., Meinel, C., & Benedek, M. (2022). What’s creative about sentences? A computational approach to assessing creativity in a sentence generation task. Creativity Research Journal, 1–12. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2124777
  • Weiss, S., Steger, D., Schroeders, U., & Wilhelm, O. (2020). A reappraisal of the threshold hypothesis of creativity and intelligence. Journal of Intelligence, 8(4), 38. doi:10.3390/jintelligence8040038
  • Xie, C., Luchini, S., Beaty, R. E., Du, Y., Liu, C., & Li, Y. (2022). Automated creativity prediction using natural language processing and resting-state functional connectivity: An fNIRS study. Creativity Research Journal, 1–18. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2108265
  • Zielińska, A., Lebuda, I., & Karwowski, M. (2022). Scaling the creative self: An item response theory analysis of the short scale of creative self. Creativity Research Journal, 1–14. doi:10.1080/10400419.2022.2123139

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.