Publication Cover
Policing and Society
An International Journal of Research and Policy
Latest Articles
289
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Exploring the relationship between shared identity and interoperability: a mixed methods analysis of discussion-based multi-agency emergency response exercises

, , , &
Received 17 Aug 2023, Accepted 26 Jun 2024, Published online: 09 Jul 2024

ABSTRACT

Previous research has shown ongoing difficulties between Police, Fire and Rescue, and Ambulance Service responders during multi-agency emergencies. Recently, researchers have used the Social Identity Approach to gain insight into these challenges, offering a psychological framework for understanding relations within and between response organisations. This study builds upon previous work by engaging responders from the emergency services in six discussion-based exercises. By analysing participants’ identity levels and their perceptions of joint working performance, we found a positive association between shared identity and interoperability. Analysis of the discussion transcripts highlighted areas where joint working faced obstacles, such as the use of organisation-specific terminology. Furthermore focus group discussions after the exercise revealed key factors linking shared identity to effective multi-agency response, including increased motivation to collaborate and increased trust and respect. This research deepens our understanding of multi-agency working from a social identity perspective, highlighting the importance of shared identity in enhancing joint efforts. Practical implications are addressed.

Introduction

The UK emergency services’ multi-agency response faces recurring challenges across incidents, as shown in inquiries into events including the 7/7 bombings (Hallett Citation2011), Hillsborough Stadium Disaster (Taylor Citation1990), and the Manchester Arena attack (Saunders Citation2022a). For example, the Manchester Arena inquiry noted ‘significant failures’ in communication and decision-making among Police, Fire and Rescue (FRS), and Ambulance Service responders resulting in conflicting information and no clear understanding of the incident (Saunders Citation2022a, Citation2022b). Similar issues have been reported in other countries too, such as the 2001 World Trade Centre Attacks (Kean and Hamilton Citation2004) and the Haiti earthquake (Patrick Citation2011). These persistent challenges highlight the need to better understand and address multi-agency response issues.

Recent research uses the Social Identity Approach to understand interoperability challenges (Davidson et al. Citation2022, Citation2023). This study applies the same approach to explore how responders’ shared identity affects their teamwork in a discussion-based exercise. To explain the rationale for why we chose this approach in the current study, we will first introduce interoperability and then explain how social identity processes relate to these challenges.

Interoperability

Interoperability, the ability of responders from different organisations to work together coherently (Joint Emergency Services Interoperability Programme [JESIP] Citation2013), has been a key focus in UK emergency management for over a decade (Pollock Citation2013). JESIP, introduced in 2012, aimed to address these challenges by introducing standardised joint working principles (e.g. clear communication, see ). However, interoperability issues persist (Pollock Citation2021). The 2017 Manchester Arena attack inquiry criticised emergency services for not embedding JESIP principles into their practices (Saunders Citation2022a). This is in line with findings from Pollock (Citation2013) who suggested that for interoperability to be effective, it needs to be embedded within the organisational culture of the emergency services.

Table 1. The five JESIP principles for joint working, from JESIP (Citation2021).

Organisational culture comprises the roles, norms, and values that create shared meaning for its members (Ellemers Citation2003, Haslam Citation2004). A recent review emphasised that embedding interoperability into organisational culture is crucial (Power et al. Citation2024). Davidson et al. (Citation2023) argue that a shared identity, where members feel, and feel to be seen as, part of the group is essential for fostering an interoperable culture. This sense of belonging enhances commitment to the organisational culture (Haslam Citation2004). The role of shared identity in interoperability is further discussed below.

Shared identity processes and interoperability

The Social Identity Approach (Tajfel and Turner Citation1979, Turner et al. Citation1987) provides insight into how people from different groups work together. Accordingly, a personal identity describes a person in contrast to others (e.g. ‘I’, ‘me’), whereas a social identity psychologically connects people through a sense of ‘we’ and ‘us’ (i.e. people who view each other as members of a common group; Neville et al. Citation2022). A sense of shared identity (i.e. a shared sense of ‘us-ness’) within a group can provide group members with shared definitions of situations and common norms for behaving in those situations (Reicher et al. Citation2010), foster trust and respect among group members (Turner et al. Citation1987, Haslam et al. Citation2012), and facilitate coordination and cooperation between group members (Haslam et al. Citation2009, Citation2022).

A key complexity present in the multi-agency response that can make coordination and cooperation between responders difficult is that they require usually independent teams of Police, FRS, and Ambulance responders to work together collaboratively to achieve a shared goal (Bharosa et al. Citation2010). In other words, it involves the formation of a multiteam system (MTS; Marks et al. Citation2001, Mathieu et al. Citation2001). Importantly, not only are these teams within the MTS working towards a shared goal, they often also have differing and potentially competing sub-goals that they must achieve in the pursuit of this shared goal (Power and Alison Citation2017). Crucially, the sub-teams within the MTS must work together to manage these sub-goals, as well as differing command structures, language, and procedures to provide an effective joint response (Brown et al. Citation2021).

An important consideration for MTS’s is that people can have multiple social identities which can become salient in different contexts (Connaughton et al. Citation2011, Millward and Haslam Citation2013, Luciano et al. Citation2018). For example, in a multi-agency response team, responders can identify both with their sub-team (Police, FRS, Ambulance) and their superordinate team (emergency responders). Research on military teams in the Netherlands showed that salient sub-team identities were associated with less trust and more conflict between sub-teams (Wijnmaalen et al. Citation2019). Additionally, a study using students in a simulated firefighting scenario showed that identification with the emergency response MTS improved MTS performance, emphasising the role of shared identity in MTS performance (Cuijpers et al. Citation2016).

Recent research has sought to expand on this by applying the Social Identity Approach to Police, FRS, and Ambulance responders joint working. Davidson et al. (Citation2022, Citation2023) conducted two interview studies with responders involved in the multi-agency COVID-19 response. They found evidence of shared identity among responders which was enhanced by a shared purpose, shared difficult experiences, and leaders strategically reinforcing this sense of identity.

Davidson et al. (Citation2022, Citation2023) provide useful insight into the presence of a shared identity between responders in these multi-agency teams. However, this research does not explore an association between shared identity and interoperability within these multi-agency teams. Previous research shows that shared identity improves MTS performance (Cuijpers et al. Citation2016, Wijnmaalen et al. Citation2019, Mell et al. Citation2020). While Cuijpers et al. (Citation2016) and Wijnmaalen et al. (Citation2019) suggest a link between shared identity and MTS performance, this (to our knowledge) has not been studied with real emergency responders during multi-agency emergency response. Our study aims to fill this gap by using discussion-based exercises with Police, FRS, and Ambulance Service responders to explore any associations between shared identity and interoperability.

The role of exercises in emergency response research

Exercises are essential for assessing emergency response capabilities (Borell and Eriksson Citation2013) and include live exercises (e.g. Skryabina et al. Citation2017), immersive simulations (e.g. Brown et al. Citation2020), and discussion-based exercises (e.g. Borell and Eriksson Citation2013). Whilst live exercises and simulations offer higher fidelity than discussion-based exercises (Alison et al. Citation2013), we chose discussion-based exercises for this study due to epistemological and logistical reasons. These exercises build on previous interview-based research and involve discussing a plan or scenario without needing real-time properties (Borell and Eriksson Citation2013, Skryabina et al. Citation2017). When developing discussion-based exercises, Borell and Eriksson (Citation2013) recommend incorporating variation into the exercise to provide different response experiences, using scenarios to provide this variation within and/or between scenarios, and providing interaction opportunities between participants to generate insights into different perspectives and opportunities for collaboration. We incorporated these three recommendations in the current study, as discussed below.

The current study

Given the role of shared identity in MTS’s and evidence of its presence in multi-agency response teams, this study used discussion-based exercises to explore the link between responders’ shared identity and their ability to work together. Specifically, this research aimed to answer the following research questions:

RQ1. Did participants experience a shared sense of identity with each other during the exercises?

RQ2. Did participants think they worked effectively together during the exercises?

RQ3. Was participants’ shared identity linked to their perceptions of how effectively they worked together during the exercises?

RQ4. How did participants work together during the exercises?

RQ5. How was shared identity linked to improved joint working?

Method

Design

Participants took part in two discussion-based exercises in groups of three to six (six groups in total). Using a within-subjects mixed-methods design, both quantitative and qualitative data were collected. The design had two factors: time (with three levels: baseline, post-exercise 1, and post-exercise 2) and exercise (with two levels: Exercise 1 and Exercise 2). Quantitative data came from questionnaires at each time-point, while qualitative data came from exercise transcripts and post-exercise focus groups.

Participants

Twenty-four current or former operational commanders from Police (N = 7), FRS (N = 15), and Ambulance (N = 2) Services from six regions across the UK participated. They were recruited through emergency service contacts and word of mouth. Participants were grouped regionally to minimise any between-region differences that may interfere with responders’ ability to work together. Due to availability and operational commitments, group sizes and service representation varied, but each group included representatives from at least two emergency services. Participants averaged 20 years of service (range: 8–30 years). Except for Groups 2, 4, and 6, where one participant lacked a pre-existing relationship, all participants knew at least one other group member before the exercise. Most reported participating in multi-agency training every 6–12 months and were ‘very’ or ‘extremely’ familiar with JESIP. See for participant group details by organisation and gender.

Table 2. Participant information.

Materials

Exercises

Following Borell and Eriksson’s (Citation2013) advice, the exercises featured two distinct and evolving scenarios: a flood at a primary school where several children and staff were trapped inside the school building and needed rescuing (Exercise 1); and a shooting at a restaurant where several members of the public were injured and there was conflicting information surrounding the location of the shooter (Exercise 2). Participants received updates on scenario developments during the exercise. Scenarios were developed in consultation with subject matter experts from the emergency services to ensure the situations and challenges presented were representative of what might occur in a real incident. All groups completed both exercises, with the order counterbalanced across groups (see Supplementary Materials 1 for full scenarios).

Pre-exercise questionnaire

The pre-exercise questionnaire included two items on participants’ identity: ‘I feel a bond with the other responders taking part in this exercise’ and ‘I think that the other responders taking part in this exercise feel a bond with each other’. Adapted from Leach et al. (Citation2008) items for solidarity and in-group identification, these items avoided the term ‘identity’ to prevent priming participants. Items showed good internal reliability (rsb>0.90). See Supplementary Materials 2 for the full pre-exercise questionnaire.

Post-exercise questionnaire

The post-exercise questionnaire contained the same scale as the pre-exercise questionnaire and showed good internal reliability at both time-points (rsb’s>0.90). It also included a scale on perceived joint working performance (e.g. ‘I communicated effectively with other responders taking part in this exercise’, 5 items). Items were adapted from the JESIP Assurance Framework (Citation2017), the standard training for interoperability in the UK at the time this research was conducted. These items showed good internal reliability (α >0.90). See Supplementary Materials 3 for the full post-exercise questionnaires.

Focus group topic guide

For consistency across measures, the focus group guide covered themes similar to those in the post-exercise questionnaires. Themes related to participants’ shared identity and their perceived joint working performance. See Supplementary Materials 4 for the full focus group topic guide.

Procedure

Ethical approval was obtained fromUK Health Security Agency Research and Governance Group, approval number: R&D 477. Because of constraints on in-person meetings due to the COVID-19 pandemic, exercises were conducted virtually via Microsoft Teams. The first author ran all exercises. All groups, except Group 3, logged in separately. Group 3 shared a computer in the same room.

After logging into Microsoft Teams participants received a link to the online survey. Participants read the information sheet and were able to ask the researcher any questions before signing an electronic consent form. Participants then completed the pre-exercise questionnaire.

After finishing the pre-exercise questionnaire, participants completed Exercise 1. The researcher provided the initial exercise information via Microsoft Teams chat. Participants had 15 min to discuss their response, with subsequent updates every (approximately) 12 min. Discussions took place verbally and each exercise lasted 1 h. Following Exercise 1, participants completed the first post-exercise questionnaire. Participants then completed Exercise 2 following the same timelines as Exercise 1, before completing the second post-exercise questionnaire. Finally, participants took part in a 30-min semi-structured focus group led by the researcher which lasted approximately 30 min (range 25–36 min). Participants received a certificate of participation by email. Exercises were recorded and transcribed by the first author.

Analysis

Questionnaire data

Questionnaire data were analysed using SPSS 28.0.1. Non-parametric tests were used due to violated parametric assumptions and were conducted as two-tailed tests. One-sample Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests compared identity and joint working performance scores to the midpoint value of 4 on the Likert scale to assess shared identity (RQ1) and joint working performance (RQ2). A Friedman’s test explored changes in identity scores and a Repeated-Sample Wilcoxon Signed Rank test compared joint working performance between Exercise 1 and 2. Spearman’s correlations examined relationships between identity scores and joint working performance (RQ3). We conducted effect-size sensitivity analyses using G*Power (Faul et al. Citation2007) for each statistical test to ensure adequate power to detect effects (Giner-Sorolla et al. Citation2022).

Discussion transcript data

Data was analysed using the framework approach (Pope et al. Citation2000). A thematic framework was identified based on the joint working principles laid out in the Joint Doctrine (JESIP Citation2021): communicate; coordinate; co-locate; joint understanding of risk; and shared situational awareness (see for coding framework). Relevant passages within the data were coded into one or more of the themes outlined in the coding framework. The first author coded the dataset and met with the research team to discuss the analysis.

Table 3. Coding framework, with illustrative quotes from data, for the discussion transcript data analysis, adapted from JESIP (Citation2017) Exercise Assurance Framework.

Focus group data

Data was analysed using semi-deductive thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke Citation2006). This analysis approach was chosen because, whilst there were no predetermined themes, the Social Identity Approach provided a general reference point to guide the analysis. The first author reviewed the transcripts, noting initial codes related to RQ5 and discussed potential themes with the research team. Transcripts were re-reviewed and relevant extracts were coded to these themes. The research team then finalised and defined the themes, which included shared goals, shared frame of reference, motivation to work with each other, confidence, and increased trust and respect (see ).

Table 4. Thematic framework and illustrative quotes.

Results

This section is organised by the research questions. First, questionnaire data addresses RQs 1–3. Second, discussion transcripts address RQ4. Finally, focus group data address RQ5.

Questionnaire data

This section is separated by RQs 1–3. Sensitivity analysis indicated sufficient power for RQs 1–2, but partial power for RQ3 (see Supplementary Materials 5).

RQ1. Did participants experience a sense of shared identity with each other during the exercises?

Participants’ identity scores were significantly higher than the midpoint (4) on the Likert scale at each time-point: baseline (Mdn = 5.75), z = 3.78, p <.01, r = 0.77; Time-Point 1 (Mdn = 6.00), z = 4.16, p <.01, r = 0.85; Time-Point 2 (Mdn = 6.00), z = 4.11, p <.01, r = 0.84. This suggests participants experienced a sense of shared identity both before and during the exercise.

Participants identity scores changed significantly over time, χ2 (2) = 13.20, p <.01. Post hoc analysis with Bonferroni-corrected (p <.017) Wilcoxon signed-ranks showed significant differences between baseline and Time-Point 1, T = 3.08, p <.01, and baseline and Time-Point 2, T = 2.89, p <.01. However, scores did not differ significantly between Time-Point 1 and Time-Point 2, T = .14, p = .89. This suggests that participants’ sense of shared identity increased after beginning the exercise, although no further increases occurred between Exercises 1 and 2.

RQ2. Did participants work effectively together?

Participants’ joint working scores were significantly higher than the midpoint (4) on the Likert scale at each time-point: Time-Point 1 (Mdn = 6.00), z = 4.23, p <.01, r = 0.86; Time-Point 2 (Mdn = 6.00), z = 4.24, p <.01, r = 0.87. This suggests that participants perceived that they worked well together during the exercise.

Participants’ joint working scores did not significantly differ between Time-Point 1 and Time-Point 2, z = 1.79, p = .07, r = 0.37. This suggests no improvement in perceived joint working in Exercise 2 compared to Exercise 1.

RQ3. Was participants’ sense of shared identity linked to improved joint working?

Participants familiarity levels with JESIP before taking part in the study was significantly related to their perceived joint working performance both during Exercise 1, rs = 0.75, 95% BCa CI [0.599, 0.846], p <.01, and Exercise 2, rs = 0.55, 95% BCa CI [0.195, 0.777], p <.01.

Participants’ identity levels after Exercise 1 was significantly related to their perceived joint working performance during Exercise 1, rs = 0.47, 95% BCa CI [0.084, 0.745], p <.05, and Exercise 2, rs = 0.75, 95% BCa CI [0.599, 0.846], p <.01, but not during Exercise 2, rs = 0.27, 95% BCa CI [−0.168, 0.673], p = .19.

Participants’ identity levels after Exercise 2 were significantly related to their perceived joint working performance during both Exercise 1, rs = 0.46, 95% BCa CI [0.059, 0.770], p <.05, and Exercise 2, rs = 0.52, 95% BCa CI [0.142, 0.796], p <.01.

There was no significant relationship between participants’ identity levels before the study and their perceived joint working performance both during Exercise 1, rs = 0.18, 95% BCa CI [−0.234, 0.561], p = .40 and Exercise 2, rs = −0.12, 95% BCa CI [−0.503, 0.245], p = .58.

Discussion transcript data

This section is presented in relation to RQ4: how participants worked together during the exercise. Themes are presented in relation to the five principles for joint working principles, as per JESIP (Citation2021): co-locate, communicate, coordinate, jointly understand risk, and shared situational awareness.

Co-locate

Although physical co-location was not possible due to the study’s nature, all groups discussed co-locating during the exercises. However, the depth of this discussion varied across groups. Some groups briefly mentioned co-location at the response’s outset (e.g. Group 4), whilst other groups discussed in more detail the importance of co-location and why they would do it. For example, in the flooding scenario, Group 1 emphasised co-location to share information and assess rescue urgency. Additionally, Group 6 explicitly mentioned being in the same location during the flooding exercise, while FRS responders requested Police to secure a cordon for safe equipment delivery.

Yet, despite seemingly clear communication regarding a shared meeting point in the flooding exercise, confusion arose in some groups during the shooting exercise. For example, FRS participants in Groups 5 and 6 were uncertain if they were attending a shared meeting point with the Police and Ambulance or an FRS-designated one. They stressed the necessity of meeting with Police in person to discuss important response aspects and identify safe areas of work.

Communicate

Five groups discussed using common frameworks for information sharing (e.g. M/ETHANE, JESIP Citation2021), though sometimes this information had to be requested. For example, in the shooting scenario, Group 6 initially discussed using M/ETHANE, but the conversation shifted towards intelligence gathering instead of following the framework. Major incident declaration, the framework’s first stage, was not addressed until 15 min in when an FRS participant requested this from the Police. Once declared, the conversation focused on practical actions rather than speculations. Similarly, in Group 2 an FRS participant mentioned the framework early, but it was not discussed again until 17 min in when the participant discussed formulating a M/ETHANE response.

Furthermore, whilst participants from different services used similar terminology, it is uncertain if they shared a common understanding of its meaning. For example, during the shooting scenario in Group 1, both Police and FRS participants used the term ‘clear’ when discussing scene safety and how close to the scene FRS participants were able to be, leading to apparent confusion between the Police and FRS. The FRS participant intended to deploy crews once the scene was clear, but Police disagreed stating FRS would not be allowed near the scene. To address potential misunderstandings, the Ambulance participant sought clarification from Police on their use of the word ‘clear’. Similarly, in Group 6 there was confusion over ‘Silver meeting’, requiring clarification between Police and FRS participants with differing understandings.

Coordinate

All groups agreed that FRS would lead the flooding scenario and Police would lead the shooting scenario. Additionally, when declaring a major incident, most participants supported other service’s needs, even if it was not a major incident for their organisation.

Furthermore, participants in some groups actively shared their capabilities and requested assistance from other services to support a coordinated response. For example, in the flooding scenario, the Ambulance participant in Group 1 clarified their organisation’s limitations, stating they do not operate in water. Also, FRS participants in Group 2 asked Police for support, such as setting up a cordon for safe evacuation from the school. Similarly, in the shooting scenario, FRS, and Ambulance participants in Group 1 sought information from Police on safe work areas.

However, sometimes conversations seemed insular, reflecting siloed or hierarchical thinking. Participants expected others to perform a certain task without confirming this with them. For example, in Group 5, a Police participant mentioned setting up a joint meeting point, assuming Ambulance responders would not, but they did not mention discussing this with them, risking duplicated action. Additionally, some participants dismissed issues outside their priorities. For example, in Group 6’s flooding scenario, a Police participant shifted responsibility to FRS without discussing stepping down the multi-agency response. Similarly, in Group 2’s shooting scenario, an FRS participant considered media arrival a Police concern, neglecting collective responsibility.

Jointly understand risk

All groups agreed that the leading organisation in the response would also lead the joint risk assessment. Despite one service leading, participants from the other services still asked questions to facilitate a shared risk understanding. For example, in the shooting scenario, FRS and Ambulance participants questioned Police participants about safe work areas, protective equipment, and known or potential risks.

However, in the flooding scenario, risks appeared less clear, causing confusion among participants around safe response actions. For example, in Group 6 Police participants were confused about why three feet of water posed a risk and why they could not wade through the water to rescue the trapped people. They emphasised the importance of FRS’s communication of this risk to prevent Police from ignoring safety procedures and entering the water.

Shared situational awareness

In several discussions across all groups and scenarios, participants emphasised the need for information gathering before making operational decisions. This was a key focus at the beginning of the scenarios when many details about the incident were unknown. For example, in Group 6’s shooting scenario an FRS participant explained they would not commit crews to the scene until more information was available. Hence, their immediate focus was clarifying the situation by communicating with lead responders from other services. However, this early need for intelligence seemed to hinder explicit decisions being made, specifically in the shooting scenario where more uncertainties were present.

Focus groups

This section is presented in relation to RQ5, exploring how shared identity is linked to joint working. It is separated by two themes: The development of shared identity (shared goals, shared principles) and effect of shared identity on joint working (motivation to work with each other, confidence, and increased trust and respect). Themes are presented alongside relevant extracts from the focus group transcripts.

Development of a shared identity

This section is separated into two themes: shared goals and shared frame of reference.

Shared goals. In most groups, participants said they were trying to achieve the same goal, regardless of their organisation. Some specified what the goal was (e.g. ‘resolution of the incident’, ‘save life’), whilst others more broadly stated they were ‘all here for the same thing’ or their ‘goal is fundamentally the same’.

Participants noted shared goals provided a shared purpose in the response. One Ambulance participant in Group 1 highlighted how shared goals ensured participants were on the ‘same page’. Similarly, a Police participant in Group 2 emphasised shared goals facilitated a shared mindset creating a sense of being on the ‘same team’ despite organisational differences.

An FRS participant in Group 6 noted that despite different organisations’ approaches to achieving a shared goal, just having one can build a bond between them. Additionally, a Police participant in Group 5 acknowledged overarching goals, though priorities may differ. However, working together and recognising shared goals, as well as different priorities, improved working relationships. This participant also highlighted awareness of other organisations’ need beyond a singular (e.g. ‘police way’) approach.

Shared frame of reference. All groups acknowledged that JESIP provided a shared frame of reference, aiding cohesion during the exercise because they had a common guide for how to approach the scenarios and what to discuss. A Police participant in Group 3 said that this shared frame of reference helped to create a bond within their group. An Ambulance participant in Group 1 found reassurance in using JESIP principles to help guide their discussions and decision-making because it highlighted that they were ‘all on the same page’, emphasising shared understanding over organisation-specific procedures. This recognition of shared procedures fostered unity despite organisational differences:

I think [recognising similarities] helps massively […] ‘cause everyone can see everyone’s doing the best they can […] we realise we’ve all got the same kind of stresses and strains in our jobs and that kind of unifies people almost into that blitz spirit where everyone is just doing the best they can and we find we can do better with each other than we can in isolation […] out there or in here on this exercise. (Police, Group 2)

In addition to shared principles, participants highlighted psychological similarities, such as reasons for joining their organisations, and intrinsic motivations for being an emergency responder, as contributing to a shared frame of reference. Participants said this perception aided in viewing each other as part of the same group. For example, a participant in Group 4 said despite varying expertise, being part of the emergency services fostered ‘mutual recognition and respect’ due to shared values. Another participant emphasised similar reasons for joining their respective services, promoting a shared frame of reference, helping them see each other as part of the same team:

We are pretty much all of a same mindset. Everyone joins their respective services to to to be that hero and to make that difference, we just do it in slightly different ways. But essentially erm if you stripped us of our […] colours, you probably won’t be able to tell the difference in the service […] so yeah, straight away you’ve got that […] shared sort of frame of reference to each other. (Police, Group 2)

Effect of shared identity on joint working

This section is separated into three themes: motivation to work with each other, confidence, and increased trust and respect.

Motivation to work with each other. Over half of the groups discussed that forming bonds during the exercise motivated collaboration, for example through listening to each other’s input. Two groups mentioned organisational differences that made working together challenging, for example, different appetites to risk (e.g. Group 2). However, despite these differences, participants acknowledged that established relationships prevented these differences from hindering group working:

There will always be a pull from each other’s own services […] it is the relationships that build around that will stop those conversations from happening. (FRS, Group 2)

Some groups emphasised the importance of clear communication to ensure mutual understanding between participants. For example, in Group 5 one participant said they were actively considering how to communicate with participants from other organisations:

That’s your point of view, well how can I put across my point of view and my understanding to you in your terms or something’s that’s gonna make a lot more sense for you. (Police)

In addition, some groups also discussed wanting to seek information from other organisations before making decisions, especially on topics which were outside of their expertise. For example, Group 4 discussed ‘referring and deferring’ to each other’s expertise rather than making decisions independently and this was driven by the relationships established early in the exercise.

Confidence. Several groups highlighted that building bonds fostered participants’ confidence in both others’ ability and their own. In Group 2 a participant said that forming relationships enabled them to communicate more technically because they had confidence the other participants would understand.

Additionally, an established bond between participants fostered confidence in challenging each other when needed. Participants in Group 1 discussed how during the shooting scenario, the Ambulance participant challenged a decision by the Police to not allow them to enter the scene. When asked why they challenged this, the Ambulance participant said that they felt comfortable doing so due to the established working relationship.

An established bond between participants not only gave participants confidence to challenge each other but also confidence when challenged. In Group 6 an FRS participant said they felt comfortable responding to a challenge from a Police participant regarding the risk posed by three feet of water. They described a supportive atmosphere enabling open exchanges due to the relationships between them.

Increased trust and respect. Half the groups discussed the importance of trusting each other during the response. In Group 2, trust was established early on via introductions where participants stated their identities and roles.

Establishing trust through understanding each other’s roles was particularly important, especially when scenarios fell outside of participants’ expertise. For example, in Group 2, one FRS participant said they were ‘heavily relying’ on the Police participant to lead due to the scenario not being within their area of expertise. Yet, participants said communicating ‘often and clearly’ with the other participants in the group enabled trust in other participants to develop through recognising ‘everyone brings something to the table’.

Participants said that trust also stemmed from the bond formed between participants through shared principles, like JESIP. In Group 2, JESIP was described as a ‘golden thread’ that guided discussions. Similarly, in Group 4 JESIP served as a common element, fostering ‘mutual recognition and respect’ among participants. This group said this facilitated group working through making them want to get along with each other, unlike past incidents where they might have seen themselves as individual organisations.

Discussion

The current study explored the relationship between shared identity and interoperability in six multi-agency emergency response groups through discussion-based exercises. It aimed to explore if participants felt a shared identity, how they worked together, and if shared identity was associated with interoperability.

This study found a positive relationship between participants’ identity and their perceived joint working performance, indicating an important role of shared identity in interoperability. Interestingly, although participants felt a sense of shared identity before the exercise discussions began, it was not directly linked to their perceived joint working performance. This suggests two important findings. First, shared identity can develop as a result of spending time together, for example through working together in the exercises, or through being part of the same study. Second, increased shared identity is associated with improved self-perceived joint working performance. This expands on previous research, showing that not only can the time spent together facilitate a sense of shared identity (e.g. Davidson et al. Citation2022), but also that this developed identity is related to improved joint working, thus providing an important advance in our understanding of the role of shared identity in interoperability within emergency response multiteam systems (MTSs).

An earlier study demonstrated that identifying with the overarching MTS improved team performance (Cuijpers et al. Citation2016). However, they focused on undergraduate students, limiting its applicability to real-world responders. Thus, our study marks the first crucial investigation into shared identity’s role in interoperability with this specific population of interest.

In our study, shared identity was associated with improved joint working, evident in the focus group discussions where it fostered motivation, confidence, trust, and respect among responders. This aligns with existing research demonstrating how shared identity improves group dynamics, including increased trust, respect, and cooperation between group members (Reicher et al. Citation2010, Neville and Reicher Citation2011). In addition, shared identity has been shown to improve coaction between group members (Drury and Reicher Citation2020). Further research has shown that MTSs coordinate more effectively when their sub-groups share an identity (Cuijpers et al. Citation2016, Mell et al. Citation2020). Thus, our study highlights the important impact of shared identity on group behaviour, particularly within emergency response MTSs.

Shared identity impacts not only multi-agency response teams but also various emergency services that collaborate with other organisations. In policing, the concept of ‘plural policing’ is used to demonstrate the frequent need for collaborative work with other organisations (Crawford et al. Citation2005). For example, child safeguarding entails inter-organisational collaboration among Police, healthcare, education, social care, and youth services (Crawford and L’Hoiry Citation2017). Effective intergroup working in such contexts relies on factors like shared commitment, purpose, and trust, as noted by Crawford and L’Hoiry (Citation2017) and supported by other studies on plural policing (Whelan Citation2017, Nøkleberg Citation2020).

As expected, in our study participants with more prior knowledge of JESIP reported better joint working during the exercise, aligning with the standardised guidance it offers for interoperability. However, despite its established principles challenges persist in implementing JESIP effectively in real-world responses, as documented in emergency response literature (e.g. Pollock Citation2013) and incident inquiries (e.g. Saunders Citation2022a).

Upon further exploration, whilst the questionnaire data suggested participants perceived effective joint working, analysis of discussion transcripts revealed challenges. Instances of ‘silo thinking’ emerged, where participants disregarded certain problems as others’ concerns and assumed tasks would be handled by other organisations. This indicates a focus on individual sub-goals over the overarching shared goal, potentially neglecting the needs and priorities of other participants. This aligns with Cuijpers et al.’s (Citation2016) findings on inter-team conflicts impacting MTS performance. The qualitative findings in our study support this, illustrating how potential conflicts between sub-groups could affect joint working (e.g. through differing expectations of roles).

Previous research has highlighted the role of pre-existing relationships in fostering interoperability among emergency responders (Davidson et al. Citation2023). In this study, most participants reported having such relationships. Davidson et al. (Citation2023) found that these relationships, coupled with recent shared experiences, facilitated group working during crises like the COVID-19 pandemic. In our study, despite pre-existing relationships, participants’ shared identity increased during the exercise which was associated with improved perceived joint working. This suggests that while pre-existing relationships aid present group dynamics, joint working can further strengthen these bonds.

Though our study focused on UK emergency response, its implications extend internationally. Research from various countries, such as the Netherlands (Bharosa et al. Citation2010), Australia (Cattermole-Terzic and Horberry Citation2020), and Norway (Eide et al. Citation2014), along with inquiries into incidents like the 2010 Haiti earthquake (Arnaouti et al. Citation2022) and Hurricane Katrine in California, USA (Comfort and Haase Citation2006) reveal similar challenges. For example, Eide et al. (Citation2014) found communication gaps among emergency responders in Norway, including issues with shared language and situational awareness. While further research is needed to validate the direct applicability of our findings to other countries’ emergency response teams, the shared challenges suggest relevance, emphasising the role of shared identity in addressing such issues.

Strengths, limitations, and recommendations for future research

Our study builds on previous research by exploring the link between shared identity and interoperability. Its mixed-methods approach offers a comprehensive understanding of the role of shared identity in interoperability. But there are also limitations that need to be addressed. First, there were unequal participant numbers and representation from emergency services across groups, potentially affecting group dynamics. Whilst findings were consistent across groups, inclusion of all services would enhance generalisability. Future research should therefore consider representation across services.

Second, the study faced challenges with low sample size due to difficulty in recruiting emergency responders because of busy work schedules and operational commitments. It is a well-documented problem that low samples are a common challenge in MTS research (e.g. Alison et al. Citation2015, Bell et al. Citation2018, Brown et al. Citation2021, Wilkinson et al. Citation2019). However, conducting research specifically with the population of interest is essential within this research area to ensure findings are relevant to the population to whom the research is aimed at (Boulton and Cole Citation2016). Despite sensitivity analyses addressing power, caution is needed in interpreting resulting, particularly in correlational analysis. In addition, group-level analysis may be more appropriate due a potential concern of non-independent samples as participants took part in the study in groups, however, small group sizes limit this approach. Whilst further research is warranted, this study provides valuable insights into shared identity and interoperability.

Third, the study lacked experimental manipulation and therefore whilst relationships between variables can be explored, causal effects cannot be determined. Discussion-based exercises, whilst informative, lack the complexities and pressures of a real incident due to the artificial environment. In the current study this was exacerbated by the virtual nature of the teams. Future research should consider experimental design to allow any causal relationships to be explored, as well as considering high-fidelity environments such as large-scale exercises (e.g. Waring et al. Citation2020) or immersive simulations (e.g. Wilkinson et al. Citation2022) to better understand shared identity’s role in interoperability.

Whilst this paper focused on the relationship between shared identity and interoperability, other factors like governance (e.g. Lakoma Citation2023) and technology (e.g. Bharosa et al. Citation2010, Abdeen et al. Citation2021) can also influence multi-agency response effectiveness. Therefore, recommendations from this research should be considered alongside these other factors.

Finally, in this study, we used the Joint Doctrine principles (JESIP Citation2021) as a framework to assess participants’ joint working effectiveness. Whilst this approach aligns with standardised emergency responder training, it lacks validation and may overlook key behavioural and psychological interoperability indicators. To advance interoperability research, future research should develop and validate a systematic framework for interoperability analysis.

Conclusions

This study is the first, to our knowledge, to provide evidence of a relationship between shared identity and improved group working during multi-agency emergency response. This highlights the important role of psychological factors in fostering interoperability. Our findings highlight how developing a shared frame of reference and emphasising shared goals fosters the development of a shared identity. Based on this, we argue that psychological factors, such as shared identity, should be integrated into multi-agency training and guidance for emergency responders to support preparedness for emergency requiring interoperability.

Practitioner points

  • Shared identity facilitates improved joint working through providing responders with motivation to work with each other, fostering mutual confidence, and increasing levels of trust and respect among team members.

  • The development of a shared identity among responders is facilitated when responders align on shared goals and possess a unified framework such as practical principles like JESIP, or shared intrinsic motivations, which foster teamworking.

  • Training and guidance for interoperability should prioritise the inclusion of psychological factors, particularly those that foster and highlight a sense of shared identity among responders.

Ethics statement: Ethical approval was obtained from the UK Health Security Agency Research and Governance Group, approval number: R&D 477.

Supplemental material

Supplementary_Materials_1.docx

Download MS Word (24.9 KB)

Supplemental material

Download MS Word (23.5 KB)

Supplemental material

Download MS Word (25.8 KB)

Supplemental material

Download MS Word (24 KB)

Supplemental material

Download MS Word (23.5 KB)

Acknowledgements

The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, the Department of Health and Social Care or UKHSA. For the purpose of open access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY 4.0) licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.

Additional information

Funding

This research was carried out as part of a PhD project by Louise Davidson at the University of Sussex in collaboration with the UK Health Security Agency (UKHSA). The PhD is jointly funded by the Fire Service Research and Training Trust and the University of Sussex. John Drury and Holly Carter were supported by a grant from ESRC reference ES/V005383/1. Holly Carter and Richard Amlôt are funded by the National Institute for Health Research Health Protection Unit (NIHR HPRU) in Emergency Preparedness and Response (EPR), a partnership between UKHSA, King’s College London and the University of East Anglia and the NIHR HPRU in Behavioural Science and Evaluation in partnership with the University of Bristol. Louise Davidson is also affiliated to the EPR HPRU.

References

  • Abdeen, F.N., et al., 2021. Challenges in multi-agency collaboration in disaster management: a Sri Lankan perspective. International journal of disaster risk reduction, 62, 102399. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102399.
  • Alison, L., et al., 2013. Immersive simulated learning environments for researching critical incidents. Journal of cognitive engineering and decision making, 7 (3), 255–272. doi:10.1177/1555343412468113.
  • Alison, L., et al., 2015. Decision inertia: deciding between least worst outcomes in emergency responses to disasters. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 88 (2), 295–321. doi:10.1111/joop.12108.
  • Arnaouti, M.K.C., et al., 2022. Medical disaster response: a critical analysis of the 2010 Haiti earthquake. Frontiers in public health, 10, 1–34. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2022.995595.
  • Bell, S. T., et al., 2018. An approach for conducting actionable research with extreme teams. Journal of Management, 42 (5), 1–26. doi:10.1177/0149206316653805.
  • Bharosa, N., Lee, J., and Janssen, M., 2010. Challenges and obstacles in sharing and coordinating information during multi-agency disaster response: propositions from field exercises. Information systems frontiers, 12 (1), 49–65. doi:10.1007/s10796-009-9174-z.
  • Borell, J. and Eriksson, K., 2013. Learning effectiveness of discussion-based crisis management exercises. International journal of disaster risk reduction, 5, 28–37. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2013.05.001.
  • Boulton, L. and Cole, J., 2016. Adaptive flexibility: examining the role of expertise in the decision making of authorized firearms officers during armed confrontation. Journal of cognitive engineering and decision making, 10 (3), 291–308. doi:10.1177/1555343416646684.
  • Braun, V. and Clarke, V., 2006. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative research in psychology, 3 (2), 77–101. doi:10.1191/1478088706qp063oa.
  • Brown, O., Power, N., and Conchie, S.M., 2020. Immersive simulations with extreme teams. Organizational psychology review, 10 (3-4), 115–135. doi:10.1177/2041386620926037.
  • Brown, O., Power, N., and Conchie, S.M., 2021. Communication and coordination across event phases: a multi-team system emergency response. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 94 (3), 591–615. doi:10.1111/joop.12349.
  • Cattermole-Terzic, V. and Horberry, T., 2020. Improving traffic incident management using team cognitive work analysis. Journal of cognitive engineering and decision making, 14 (2), 152–173. doi:10.1177/1555343419882595.
  • Comfort, L.K. and Haase, T.W., 2006. Communication, coherence, and collective action. Public works management & policy, 10 (4), 328–343. doi:10.1177/1087724X06289052.
  • Connaughton, S., Shuffler, M., and Goodwin, G.F., 2011. Leading distributed teams: the communicative constitution of leadership. Military psychology, 23 (5), 502–527. doi:10.1080/08995605.2011.600147.
  • Crawford, A., et al., 2005. Plural. Bristol: Policy Press.
  • Crawford, A. and L’Hoiry, X., 2017. Boundary crossing: networked policing and emergent ‘communities of practice’ in safeguarding children. Policing and society, 27 (6), 636–654. doi:10.1080/10439463.2017.1341508.
  • Cuijpers, M., Uitdewilligen, S., and Guenter, H., 2016. Effects of dual identification and interteam conflict on multiteam system performance. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 89 (1), 141–171. doi:10.1111/joop.12113.
  • Davidson, L., et al., 2022. Advancing a social identity perspective on interoperability in the emergency services: evidence from the pandemic multi-agency response teams during the UK COVID-19 response. International journal of disaster risk reduction, 77, 103101. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2022.103101.
  • Davidson, L., et al., 2023. A social identity perspective on interoperability in the emergency services: emergency responders’ experiences of multiagency working during the COVID-19 response in the UK. Journal of contingencies and crisis management, 31 (3), 353–371. doi:10.1111/1468-5973.12443.
  • Drury, J. and Reicher, S., 2020. Crowds and collective behaviour. Version 1. University of Sussex. Available from: https://hdl.handle.net/10779/uos.23478062.v1.
  • Eide, W.A., et al., 2014. Key challenges in multi-agency collaboration during large-scale emergency management. Proceedings on the workshop on ambient intelligence for crisis management. SINTEF ICT. Available from: https://ceur-ws.org/Vol-953/paper5.pdf.
  • Ellemers, N, 2003. Identity, Culture, and Change in Organizations: A Social Identity Analysis and Three Illustrative Cases. In: S. A. Haslam, et al., ed. Social Identity at Work: Developing Theory for Organizational Practice. Philadelphia: Psychology Press, 191–203.
  • Faul, F., et al., 2007. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior research methods, 39 (2), 175–191. doi:10.3758/BF03193146.
  • Giner-Sorolla, R., et al., 2022. Power to detect what? Considerations for planning and evaluating sample size. PsyArXiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/rv3kw.
  • Hallett, J., 2011. Coroner’s inquests into the London bombings of 7 July 2005. Available from: https://www.jesip.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2022/03/Coroners-Inquest-in-to-London-Bombings.pdf.
  • Haslam, S. A, 2004. Psychology in Organizations: The Social Identity Approach. London: Sage.
  • Haslam, S.A., et al., 2022. Social identity makes group-based social connection possible: implications for loneliness and mental health. Current opinion in psychology, 43, 161–165. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.07.013.
  • Haslam, S.A., Jetten, J., and Waghorn, C., 2009. Social identification, stress and citizenship in teams: a five-phase longitudinal study. Stress and health, 25 (1), 21–30. doi:10.1002/smi.1221.
  • Haslam, S.A., Reicher, S.D., and Levine, M., 2012. When other people are heaven, when other people are hell: how social identity determines the nature and impact of social support. In: J. Jetten, C. Haslam, and S.A. Haslam, eds. The social cure: identity, health and well-being. London: Psychology Press, 157–174.
  • JESIP, 2013. Joint doctrine: the interoperability framework. 1st ed. Available from: https://www.ukfrs.com/sites/default/files/2017-09/JESIP%20Joint%20Doctrine%20-%20The%20Interoperability%20Framework.pdf.
  • JESIP, 2021. Joint doctrine: the interoperability framework. 3rd ed. Available from: https://www.jesip.org.uk/downloads/joint-doctrine-guide/.
  • JESIP Assurance Programme, 2017. Report on findings. Available from: https://www.jesip.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2022/03/JESIP_Assurance_Programme_Report.pdf.
  • Kean, T.H. and Hamilton, L.H. 2004. The 9/11 commission report. Available from: https://govinfo.library.unt.edu/911/report/911Report.pdf.
  • Lakoma, K., 2023. A comparative study of governance changes on the perceptions of accountability in fire and rescue services in England. Public administration, 102 (1), 3–20. doi:10.1111/padm.12923.
  • Leach, C.W., et al., 2008. Group-level self-definition and self-investment: a hierarchical (multicomponent) model of in-group identification. Journal of personality and social psychology, 95 (1), 144–165. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.144.
  • Luciano, M.M., DeChurch, L.A., and Mathieu, J.E., 2018. Multiteam systems: a structural framework and meso-theory of system functioning. Journal of management, 44 (3), 1065–1096. doi:10.1177/0149206315601184.
  • Marks, M.A., Mathieu, J.E., and Zaccaro, S.J., 2001. A temporally based framework and taxonomy of team processes. The academy of management review, 26 (3), 356–376. doi:10.2307/259182.
  • Mathieu, J.E., Marks, M.A., and Zaccaro, S.J., 2001. Multiteam systems. In: N. Anderson, et al., eds. Handbook of industrial, work and organizational psychology. London: Sage, 289–313.
  • Mell, J.N., et al., 2020. Identity asymmetries: an experimental investigation of social identity and information exchange in multiteam systems. Academy of management journal, 63 (5), 1561–1590. doi:10.5465/amj.2018.0325.
  • Millward, L.J. and Haslam, S.A., 2013. Who are we made to think we are? Contextual variation in organizational, workgroup and career foci of identification. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 86 (1), 50–66. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8325.2012.02065.x.
  • Neville, F.G., et al., 2022. Shared social identity transforms social relations in imaginary crowds. Group processes & intergroup relations, 25 (1), 158–173. doi:10.1177/1368430220936759.
  • Neville, F. and Reicher, S., 2011. The experience of collective participation: shared identity, relatedness and emotionality. Contemporary social science, 6 (3), 377–396. doi:10.1080/21582041.2012.627277.
  • Nøkleberg, M., 2020. The public-private divide revisited: questioning the middle ground of hybridity in policing. Policing and society, 30 (6), 601–617. doi:10.1080/10439463.2019.1576673.
  • Patrick, J., 2011. Haiti earthquake response: emerging evaluation lessons. Available from: https://www.oecd.org/dac/evaluation/48432995.pdf.
  • Pollock, K., 2013. Review of persistent lessons identified relating to interoperability from emergencies and major incidents since 1986. Available from: https://regulation.org.uk/library/2013-Pollock_Review.pdf.
  • Pollock, K., 2021. “Lessons will be learned” A euphemism for failure? Available from: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/348235663_Lessons_Will_Be_Learned_A_euphemism_for_failure.
  • Pope, C., Ziebland, S., and Mays, N., 2000. Qualitative research in health care: analysing qualitative data. British medical journal, 320, 114–116. doi:10.1136/bmj.320.7227.114.
  • Power, N., et al., 2024. The psychology of interoperability: a systematic review of joint working between the UK emergency services. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 97 (1), 233–252. doi:10.1111/joop.12469.
  • Power, N. and Alison, L., 2017. Offence or defence? Approach and avoid goals in the multi-agency emergency response to a simulated terrorism attack. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 90 (1), 51–76. doi:10.1111/joop.12159.
  • Reicher, S., Spears, R., and Haslam, S.A., 2010. The social identity approach in social psychology. In: M.S. Wetherell and C.T. Mohanty, eds. Social identities handbook. London: Sage, 45–62.
  • Saunders, J., 2022a. Manchester Arena inquiry volume 2: emergency response. Volume 2-I. Report of the public inquiry into the attack on Manchester Arena on 22nd May 2017. Available from: https://files.manchesterarenainquiry.org.uk/live/uploads/2022/11/03141804/MAI-Volume-2-Part-i.pdf.
  • Saunders, J., 2022b. Manchester Arena inquiry volume 2: emergency response. Volume 2-II. Report of the public inquiry into the attack on Manchester Arena on 22nd May 2017. Available from: https://files.manchesterarenainquiry.org.uk/live/uploads/2022/11/03141957/MAI-Volume-2-Part-ii.pdf.
  • Skryabina, E., et al., 2017. What is the value of health emergency preparedness exercises? A scoping review study. International journal of disaster risk reduction, 21, 274–283. doi:10.1016/j.ijdrr.2016.12.010.
  • Tajfel, H. and Turner, J., 1979. An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In: W.G. Austin and S. Worchel, eds. The social psychology of intergroup relations. Monterey, CA: Wadsworth, 56–65.
  • Taylor, J., 1990. The Hillsborough stadium disaster. Final report. Available from: https://www.jesip.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2022/03/Hillsborough-Stadium-Disaster-final-report.pdf.
  • Turner, J.C., et al., 1987. Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Waring, S., Moran, J., and Page, R., 2020. Decision-making in multiagency multiteam systems operating in extreme environments. Journal of occupational and organizational psychology, 93 (3), 629–653. doi:10.1111/joop.12309.
  • Whelan, C., 2017. Security networks and occupational culture: understanding culture within and between organisations. Policing and society, 27 (2), 113–135. doi:10.1080/10439463.2015.1020804.
  • Wijnmaalen, J., et al., 2019. Intergroup behavior in military multiteam systems. Human relations, 72 (6), 1081–1104. doi:10.1177/0018726718783828.
  • Wilkinson, B., Cohen-Hatton, S.R., and Honey, R.C., 2019. Decision-making in multi-agency groups at simulated major incident emergencies: In situ analysis of adherence to UK doctrine. Journal of contingencies and crisis management, 27 (4), 306–316. Portico. doi:10.1111/1468-5973.12260.
  • Wilkinson, B., Cohen-Hatton, S.R., and Honey, R.C., 2022. Variation in exploration and exploitation in group decision-making: evidence from immersive simulations of major incident emergencies. Journal of contingencies and crisis management, 30 (1), 82–91. Portico. doi:10.1111/1468-5973.12355.