6,300
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Systematic Reviews

The Diagnostic Utility of the Child Sexual Behavior Inventory for Sexual Abuse: A Systematic Review

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , , , , & ORCID Icon show all
Pages 729-751 | Received 10 Aug 2017, Accepted 02 Mar 2018, Published online: 11 Jun 2018

ABSTRACT

Children with alleged child sexual abuse (CSA) need to be assessed systematically. The use of validated instruments during the assessment, like the Child Sexual Behavior Inventory (CSBI), could add diagnostic value. We aim to assess the diagnostic utility of the CSBI to differentiate between sexually abused and non-abused children. We conducted a systematic review. We searched the electronic databases MEDLINE and PsychInfo for studies comparing CSBI scores in sexually abused children and non-abused children (2–12 years old). Two independent reviewers extracted data and assessed the methodological quality. We included 7 (out of 1048) articles. The CSBI total scores were significantly higher in CSA-victims compared with non-abused children (in case–control settings). However, in children with suspected CSA, the results were ambiguous. One study reported significant differences. Another study reported weak diagnostic ability for the CSBI-3 in children with suspected CSA (a sensitivity and specificity of 0.50, with a positive predictive value of 0.28, and a negative predictive value of 0.72). Research on the diagnostic utility of the CSBI for suspected CSA is limited and shows disappointing results. Until more research is done, the CSBI should not be used on its own to differentiate between sexually abused and non-abused children.

Child sexual abuse (CSA) is a worldwide problem and reported in all strata of society, with an estimated prevalence varying from 8% to 31% for girls and from 3% to 17% for boys (Barth, Bermetz, Heim, Trelle, & Tonia, Citation2013; Singh, Parsekar, & Nair, Citation2014; Stoltenborgh, Van, Euser, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, Citation2011). Many definitions are being used worldwide and influence the prevalence measured. Used definitions may include noncontact, contact abuse, forced intercourse, or a combination (Barth et al., Citation2013; Stoltenborgh et al., Citation2011). Various definitions use various cutoff ages for childhood, age differences between victim and perpetrator, and may or may not include sexual violence among peers (Stoltenborgh et al., Citation2011).

CSA often remains unrecognized due to lack of CSA-specific symptoms and disclosures (Adams, Harper, Knudson, & Revilla, Citation1994; Adams et al., Citation2015; Anderson et al., Citation2014; Heger et al., Citation2002; Heger, Ticson, Velasquez, & Bernier, Citation2002; Kendall-Tackett, Williams, & Finkelhor, Citation1993; McElvaney, Greene, & Hogan, Citation2014; Munzer et al., Citation2014; RCPCH, Citation2015; Stoltenborgh et al., Citation2011). A child’s disclosure is of great importance as the child is often the only eyewitness of the abuse. Unfortunately, disclosure of CSA is often delayed, sometimes even until adult age (Gagnier & Collin-Vézina, Citation2016; Munzer et al., Citation2014). Children can withhold from telling due to feelings of shame and guilt or fear of being blamed (Gagnier & Collin-Vézina, Citation2016; Hershkowitz, Lanes, & Lamb, Citation2007; Munzer et al., Citation2014; Quas, Goodman, & Jones, Citation2003). Likely children are instructed to keep the abuse a secret (Jensen, Gulbrandsen, Mossige, Reichelt, & Tjersland, Citation2005). Verbal limitations, especially in the younger age group (preschool children), also preclude children from telling (Brilleslijper-Kater, Friedrich, & Corwin, Citation2004).

Possibly, atypical sexual behavior displayed by children is a good indicator for CSA (Brilleslijper-Kater et al., Citation2004). What is considered “normal” sexual behavior depends on the developmental stage and gender of a child. Two-year-old children display relatively more sexual behavior when compared with 10- to 12-year-olds (Friedrich, Fisher, Broughton, Houston, & Shafran, Citation1998). Normal childhood sexual behavior happens spontaneously, intermittently, with mutual consent, and does not cause any distress (Chaffin et al., Citation2008). When sexual behavior becomes age-inappropriate, it can be an indication for CSA (Baker et al., Citation2008; Drach, Wientzen, & Ricci, Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation2001).

An instrument to assess sexual behavior in children is the Child Sexual Behaviour Inventory (CSBI), developed by Friedrich in 1997. The CSBI is commonly used in studies evaluating sexual behavior in children (Baker et al., Citation2008; Cale & Lussier, Citation2016; Cohen & Mannarino, Citation1998; Friedrich, Davies, Feher, & Wright, Citation2003; Mannarino & Cohen, Citation1996; Silovsky & Niec, Citation2002; St Amand, Bard, & Silovsky, Citation2008). Development of the CSBI started in 1983 with the sexual behavior items that were used in the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach 1991; Achenbach, Citation1992). The first version of the CSBI with 35 items (CSBI-1) was developed in 1991. This version was further evaluated causing another six items to be dropped, seven to be added, and three to be rewritten. The result was the second version of the CSBI with 35 items (CSBI-2). The currently used CSBI-3 consists of 38 items, 22 items from the CSBI-2, 13 items were rewritten, and 3 new items added (Friedrich, Citation1997). The questionnaire is designed for children aged 2–12 years. The CSBI provides clinical cutoff scores and norms depending on the age (2–5, 6–9, and 10–12 years) and gender of the child.

The CSBI-3 has nine different domains: boundary problems, exhibitionism, gender role behavior, self-stimulation, sexual anxiety, sexual interest, sexual intrusiveness, sexual knowledge, and voyeuristic behavior (Friedrich, Citation1997). The mother or other primary female caregiver scores each item based on the degree of which the child has shown the specific behavior in the prior 6 months. There is a scale from 0 to 3: 0 = never, 1 = less than once a month, 2 = one to three times a month, and 3 = at least once a week. The range of the score is 0–114.

The CSBI results are in three scales. The CSBI total scale indicates the overall level of sexual behavior the child exhibits. The Developmentally Related Sexual Behaviour (DRSB) scale indicates sexual behaviors that can be considered normative for the child’s age and gender. The Sexual Abuse Specific Items (SASI) scale indicates sexual behaviors that can be viewed as relatively atypical for the child’s age and gender. Because sexual behavior differs between ages and gender, the norms are different for the following subgroups: 2–5, 6–9, and 10–12 years old for both boys and girls (Friedrich, Citation1997).

Psychometric properties of the CSBI are good. The internal consistency (alpha coefficient) varies from 0.47 to 0.86 in normative samples (Friedrich et al., Citation1998, Citation2001; Jin, Chung, Jeong, & Lee, Citation2013; Schoentjes, Deboutte, & Friedrich, Citation1999) and from 0.83 to 0.93 in CSA samples (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013). Correlation between two tests in normative samples varies from 0.85 to 0.91 (Friedrich et al., Citation2001; Friedrich, Grambsch, Broughton, Kuiper, & Beilke, Citation1991; Friedrich et al., Citation1992). The inter-rater reliability between primary caregivers (mothers and fathers) is good (r = 0.79, p < 0.01) (Friedrich et al., Citation2001). Studies on the external validity show that the CSBI is not generalizable to other countries and cultures. Significant cross-cultural differences in CSBI scores are reported between Caucasians and African Americans (Wherry, Jolly, Feldman, Adam, & Manjanatha, Citation1995), Swedish and Americans (Ingbeth Larsson, Svedin, & Friedrich, Citation2000), or Dutch and Americans (Friedrich, Sandfort, Oostveen, & Cohen-Kettenis, Citation2000).

In 2016, the Dutch Society of Paediatrics (“Nederlandse Vereniging voor Kindergeneeskunde”) published a national guideline on diagnostics for alleged CSA (Teeuw, Vrolijk-Bosschaart, & Langendam, Citation2016). While developing the guideline, the need for validated tools and instruments to improve diagnostics in the field of CSA was revealed, to better objectify psychosocial symptoms such as sexual behavior in children. The CSBI could be one of the possible instruments to use in the diagnostic process. Yet, before the CSBI can be used as a diagnostic instrument in children with alleged CSA, we need to know more about the diagnostic utility when specifically used for assessing suspected CSA. For this purpose, we conducted a systematic review to assess for the diagnostic utility of the CSBI in differentiating between children with and without a history of CSA.

Method

Eligibility

We included original studies if the study comprised children aged 2–12 years, was comparative in study design, and participants were allocated to a case or control group based on, respectively, the presence or absence of CSA history, and the study reported on the validity of the CSBI to differentiate abused from non-abused children.

Exclusion criteria for this study were specified as no original data available within an article, an article included other types of child abuse than sexual abuse, case reports, conference abstract, and editorials. There were no language restrictions.

Studies considered to best suit our research question (whether the CSBI can differentiate children with and without a history of CSA) were those performed in a clinical setting: cross-sectional studies among children with suspected CSA, comparing CSBI scores between abused and non-abused children. These studies represent the clinical situations. Nevertheless, we did not expect to identify many studies using this study design. Therefore, we also included other case–control studies, comparing CSBI scores between preselected abused (cases) and non-abused children (controls), though this design brought a substantial risk of bias because these studies do not resemble clinical situations.

Search methods

We performed a literature search in MEDLINE (PubMed) and PsychINFO on June 13, 2016. The PubMed search was questionnaire[tiab] OR child sexual behavior inventory[tiab] OR sexual behavior[tiab] AND “Child”[MESH]OR Child* [tiab] AND sexual abuse* [tiab]. The search in PsychINFO was child sexual behavior inventory.tiab. OR CSBI.tiab. Additional searches of reference lists from relevant articles were performed to identify studies that may have been missed.

Data collection and analysis

Selection of studies

Selection of studies was performed by two authors independently (TFVB and VDS). After removing the duplicates, titles and abstracts were screened for eligibility. Remaining studies were read in full text. In cases where agreement could not be reached, we asked the third author (MWL) to independently assess the study and consensus was reached via discussion.

Data extraction and management

Two authors (TFVB and VDS) independently extracted the following data from the included studies: purpose of the study; study design; population characteristics; definition/identification of cases and controls; and number of participants, the version of the CSBI used, and outcome. We evaluated the construct validity (difference in CSBI score between abused and non-abused children, 95% confidence interval), and sensitivity and specificity. Discrepancies were resolved via discussion among research team.

Methodological assessment

The methodological quality of the studies was systematically assessed by two authors (TFVB and VDS), using a standardized checklist based on the New Castle–Ottawa Scale for case–control studies (Deeks, D’Amico, AJ, Sakarovitch, & Song et al., Citation2003) (available on request).

The following definitions for CSA were used in the order of increasing risk of bias: (1) abuse confirmed at a case conference or civil or criminal court proceeding or admitted by the perpetrator, independently witnessed or reported by the child; (2) abuse confirmed by stated criteria including multidisciplinary assessment; (3) abuse defined by stated criteria; (4) abuse stated but no supporting details given; and (5) suspected abuse.

Categories for selection of non-abused children (in order of increasing risk of bias): (1) abuse actively excluded using defined criteria; (2) random groups; and (3) no description.

Results

Study selection ()

The literature search identified 1048 unique articles, of which 28 articles were selected for full-text reading. Finally, seven articles met our inclusion criteria and were included in our review (Allen, Citation2017; Cosentino, Meyer-Bahlburg, Alpert, Weinberg, & Gaines, Citation1995; Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013; Wherry et al., Citation1995). No potentially relevant articles were identified by reviewing the references of these articles. Reasons for exclusion were as follows: internal or external validity of the CSBI is not reported (Allen, Thorn, & Gully, Citation2015; Baker et al., Citation2008; Cale & Lussier, Citation2016; Cohen & Mannarino, Citation1998; Forbes, Duffy, Mok, & Lemvig, Citation2003; Friedrich et al., Citation1991; Friedrich, Jaworski, Huxsahl, & Bengtson, Citation1997; Friedrich et al., Citation2000; Kenny & Wurtele, Citation2013; Larsson & Svedin, Citation2001; Larsson et al., Citation2000; Mannarino & Cohen, Citation1996; Merrick, Litrownik, Everson, & Cox, Citation2008; Sandfort & Cohen-Kettenis, Citation2000; Schoentjes et al., Citation1999; Silovsky & Niec, Citation2002; St Amand et al., Citation2008; Thigpen, Citation2009), age of included sample >12 years (Friedrich, Lysne, Sim, & Shamos, Citation2004), and the article reported only on a selection of CSBI items (Friedrich et al., Citation2003).

Figure 1. Study selection.

Figure 1. Study selection.

Study characteristics

Study characteristics are reported in detail in . Six studies either compared CSBI scores between abused and non-abused children (Allen, Citation2016; Friedrich et al., Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013); abused and non-abused psychiatric inpatients (Wherry et al., Citation1995); or abused, non-abused, and non-abused psychiatric outpatients (Cosentino et al., Citation1995; Friedrich et al., Citation2001). Two studies compared CSBI scores in a clinical setting with children with suspected CSA (Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation1992).

Table 1. Study characteristics.

Drach et al. (Citation2001) used a clinical sample of children assessed for suspected CSA. In this study, diagnosis of CSA was made by multidisciplinary assessment independent of CSBI scores. Subsequently, CSBI scores in abused and non-abused children were compared. Friedrich et al. (Citation1992) added a clinical subsample of 37 children with suspected CSA to their cohort (Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation1992); unfortunately, data on how CSA was diagnosed or excluded was not reported for this sample.

Risk of bias

The case definition (CSA victims) was considered adequate for all included studies (Allen, Citation2016; Cosentino et al., Citation1995; Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013; Wherry et al., Citation1995) (). Controls were mainly derived from community samples (Allen, Citation2016; Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013). Three of the seven studies scored moderate to high risk of bias due to the fact that definition of groups utilized in the studies reduced the likelihood of accurate results. In these studies, CSA was inadequately excluded in the control groups (self-reports (moderate risk of bias) (Wherry et al., Citation1995) or parent report (high risk of bias) (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992)). In the study by Jin et al. (Citation2013), it was unclear how CSA was excluded and therefore the risk of bias is unknown.

Table 2. Methodological assessment of the quality of the studies.

Differentiating abused from non-abused by using the CSBI ()

Studies performed in case–control setting (CSA vs. no-CSA)

By evaluating the six studies (Allen, Citation2016; Cosentino et al., Citation1995; Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013; Wherry et al., Citation1995) using preselected abused and non-abused children, significant differences in CSBI total scores were reported. Four studies found significantly higher CSBI total scores in abused children compared to non-abused children (Cosentino et al., Citation1995; Friedrich et al., Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013; Wherry et al., Citation1995). We were not able to compare distributions because of the different CSBI versions used in these studies.

Table 3. Results.

Allen (Citation2016) compared the prevalence of CSA among children with and without sexual behavior problems (SBP). The CSBI total score and the CSBI-SASI subscale were used to identify children with SBP. They found a significantly higher proportion of sexual abuse among the children with SBP compared to children without SBP (21.6% (n = 53) vs. 9.1% (n = 79); X2 = 28.6, p = 0.001) (Allen, Citation2016).

Comparing children with a CSA history with normative populations, the sensitivity varies between 0.70 and 0.95 (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992) and the specificity varies between 0.35 and 0.55 (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992) depending on the age range and gender. However, when comparing a sexually abused sample with a psychiatric sample (non-CSA), sensitivity ranged from 0.40 to 0.55 and the specificity from 0.78 to 0.87 (Friedrich et al., Citation2001).

These results suggest that the CSBI is able to distinguish abused from non-abused children. However, the acceptable sensitivity and low specificity reported in these studies suggest that, depending on age and gender, a considerable proportion of CSA cases will be missed and that among the children who are not abused more than half will be falsely labelled as victims of CSA (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992). The opposite was found when comparing sexually abused children with a non-CSA psychiatric patients sample as control group (Friedrich et al., Citation2001).

Studies performed in a clinical setting (children with suspected CSA)

Two studies used a (sub)sample of children with suspected CSA (Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation1992). Friedrich et al. (Citation1992) found significantly higher CSBI-1-total scores in abused children compared to non-abused. They included 37 consecutive referrals to two of the seven clinical sites involved in their study, although it was not stated which ones. It was unclear whether the setting was a paediatric setting or a forensic setting. No CSBI data were used for the initial determination of CSA; however, they did not report on how CSA was determined. About 22 children were identified as sexually abused and their CSBI-1 total scores differed significantly compared to the non-abused children (mean CSBI-1-total score 23.3 (SD 12.8) vs. 11.5 (SD 7.5), p < 0.05). Sensitivity and specificity were not reported (Friedrich et al., Citation1992).

Drach and colleagues (2001) evaluated the CSBI-3 in 247 children evaluated for suspected CSA. The children included in this study were referred to a community-based multidisciplinary forensic child abuse evaluation center for suspected CSA. They found no significant differences for the CSBI total score, CSBI-DRSB, and CSBI-SASI between abused and non-abused children. Chi-square tests of association between the dichotomized sexual behavior variables (based on the T-scores: T < 65 low score, T > 65 high score) and sexual abuse diagnosis were nonsignificant, the proportion of high scorers and low scorers was approximately equal. The CSBI-3 was found to have a sensitivity and specificity of 0.50, with a positive predictive value of 0.28 and a negative predictive value of 0.72. Authors concluded that the scores for CSBI total, CSBI-DRSB, or CSBI-SASI were not related to the diagnosis or exclusion of sexual abuse (Drach et al., Citation2001).

Discussion

We conducted a systematic review to assess whether the CSBI is able to distinguish sexual abuse from non-abuse in children, aged 2–12 years, with suspected CSA. A total of 7 studies out of 1048 were included.

Three of the seven studies scored moderate to high risk of bias due to inadequate exclusion of CSA in the control groups, for example, by using parent reports to exclude (alleged) CSA (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992). The definition of groups utilized in these studies reduced the likelihood of accurate results. Researchers need to take into account that it is plausible that a parent is not aware of any abuse, or that the parent who answers the questionnaire is the abuser itself and will not report honestly. One study did not report how CSA was excluded (Jin et al., Citation2013), logically introducing a risk of bias.

The included studies were difficult to compare because of the different versions of the CSBI that were used. Ideally, only studies using the most recent CSBI version (CSBI-3) would be included. Provided we had done that, this would have resulted in only three studies to be included in our review (Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation2001; Jin et al., Citation2013).

Based on the available evidence, the validity of the CSBI as a diagnostic instrument in children with suspected CSA remains unclear. In and of itself, the CSBI seems to be a reliable questionnaire to measure sexual behavior in children. The CSBI is known to have a good internal consistency (Friedrich et al., Citation1998, Citation2001, Citation1992; Jin et al., Citation2013; Schoentjes et al., Citation1999), test-retest reliability (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1991, Citation1992), and inter-rater reliability between primary caregivers (Friedrich et al., Citation2001). However, the CSBI is not generalizable to other countries and cultures as several studies comparing CSBI scores between normative populations form different countries or cultural groups within one country show significant cross-cultural differences in CSBI scores (Kenny & Wurtele, Citation2013; Ingbeth Larsson et al., Citation2000; Wherry et al., Citation1995). Therefore, it is necessary for every country to generate their own normative data.

Our results on sensitivity and specificity show that a considerable proportion of CSA cases will be missed and that among the children who are not abused more than half will be falsely labelled as victims of CSA (Friedrich et al., Citation2001, Citation1992). The opposite was found when comparing sexually abused children with a non-CSA psychiatric patients sample as control group (Friedrich et al., Citation2001). Probably psychiatric patients display more atypical sexual behavioral without a history of CSA.

The determination of the CSBI’s accuracy in distinguishing children with a history of CSA from those without such a history requires a different methodology. Namely, there is a need for research using a sample of children with suspected CSA. We only found two studies using this study method (Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation1992).

There are several explanations for the limited ability of CSBI to distinguish abused from non-abused children in children with alleged CSA. First, not all sexually abused children display age-inappropriate sexual behavior. A meta-analysis of 13 studies (including studies on preschool children) reports that only one third of the sexually abused children had sexual behavior problems (Kendall-Tackett et al., Citation1993), meaning that two-thirds of the abused children did not display sexual behavior problems.

Second, although atypical sexual behavior in children can point to CSA (Baker et al., Citation2008; Drach et al., Citation2001; Friedrich et al., Citation2001), there is not one specific sexual behavior that is indicative for CSA (Kellogg, Citation2009). Other explanations for the behavior are possible, for instance, physical abuse, family violence, and other types of maltreatment (S. N. Brilleslijper-Kater et al., Citation2004; Chaffin et al., Citation2008; Everson & Faller, Citation2012; Kellogg, Citation2009). As far as we know, there is no specific pattern of psychosocial symptoms (including sexual behavior) that indicates CSA (Briere & Elliott, Citation1994; Finkelhor & Berliner, Citation1995; Kendall-Tackett et al., Citation1993). In about 30% of the children, no psychosocial signs are apparent at all (Conte, Citation1988; Friedrich, Urquiza, & Beilke, Citation1986; Mian, Klanjer-Diamond, Lebaron, & Winder, Citation1986). This might be due to specific aspects of the abuse (nature, frequency, duration, severity, and relationship with perpetrator), environmental factors (how safe is the child?), and the child’s character (Briere & Elliott, Citation1994).

Third, it is likely that there are several cases of suspected CSA in whom the reason for the suspicion of CSA is based on observed atypical sexual behavior. Possibly, the CSBI scores of the entire sample of children with suspected CSA were higher on average compared to the normative population, as was also reported in psychiatric control samples (Friedrich et al., Citation2001). This is an important point – there could be circularity that is impacting the accuracy of the CSBI in this context. When atypical sexual behavior is the reason for suspecting CSA, this may lead to false-positive accusations, as there are numerous other explanations for the displayed behavior (S. N. Brilleslijper-Kater et al., Citation2004; Chaffin et al., Citation2008; Everson & Faller, Citation2012; Kellogg, Citation2009).

The so-called golden standard to ascertain CSA is forensic evidence of CSA (for example, DNA) or CSA recorded on child pornographic images (photographs or videos). In most cases, this kind of evidence for CSA is lacking. The second-best reference standard is therefore a multidisciplinary assessment combining forensic, paediatric, and psychosocial evaluation of a case. Clinicians need to keep in mind that we do not exactly know the diagnostic value of this approach as it is practically impossible and unethical to investigate.

Strengths and limitations

We performed a systematic review on the CSBI as a diagnostic instrument for CSA. Multiple databases, two independent reviewers, and clear inclusion and exclusion criteria were used. Data were extracted systematically and studies were critically assessed on risk of bias. Nonetheless, we must acknowledge some substantial limitations.

We chose not to exclude studies based on the CSBI version used, causing heterogeneity between studies. The number of included studies was small. Therefore, we were unable to combine results empirically in a meta-analysis. Nevertheless, we believe we were able to formulate clear clinical implications.

Clinical implications and future research

We identified only one study reporting on the diagnostic validity of the CSBI-3 in children with suspected CSA (Drach et al., Citation2001). They reported a sensitivity and specificity both too weak to warrant use of the CSBI as an instrument to differentiate between children with and without a history of CSA. More studies are needed to confirm or reject our findings.

Based on what was published on the reliability of the CSBI in general, the CSBI seems to be a reliable instrument to objectively measure a child’s sexual behavior. However, the CSBI measures sexual behavior observed by a caregiver who answers the questionnaire. The CSBI relies on what is observed (and reported) by the caregiver and is therefore not fully objective. Perhaps assessing sexual knowledge instead of sexual behavior is more promising in discriminating CSA from non-CSA children. It is known that most young children only possess certain basic knowledge on genital differences, gender identity, sexual body parts, and (non-sexual) functions of the genitals. Their knowledge on pregnancy, birth, reproduction, and adult sexual behavior is very limited (S. N. Brilleslijper-Kater et al., Citation2004). In general, the younger the child, the less he or she knows (S.N.; Brilleslijper-Kater & Baartman, Citation2000). Sexual knowledge can be measured more objectively compared to sexual behavior. Sexual knowledge is also measured in the CSBI but not extensively.

Knowledge of sexuality is likely to be less dependent on abuse-related variables (Brilleslijper-Kater et al., Citation2004). However, there could be a multitude of reasons why sexual knowledge differs among children, such as differences in parental-provided sex education, exposure to sexual-related information from peers and siblings, and exposure to sexual media. Sexual knowledge can derive from several sources, just as sexual behaviors can. These limitations would limit the conclusions that could be drawn from an assessment indicating advanced sexual knowledge. Despite the limitations, future research should focus more on instruments to measure children’s sexual knowledge and whether sexual knowledge can be a more discriminating and objective measure.

Conclusion

Research on the diagnostic utility of the CSBI among children with suspected CSA is limited and shows disappointing results. Until more research is done, the CSBI can be used to measure sexual behavior but should not be used on its own to differentiate between children with and without a history of CSA.

Additional information

Notes on contributors

Thekla F. Vrolijk-Bosschaart

Thekla F. Vrolijk-Bosschaart is a medical physician, and former researcher and coordinator for the Child Abuse and Neglect Team in the Emma Children’s Hospital, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Eva Verlinden

Eva Verlinden is a child psychologist. She studied children and adolescents who have been exposed to one or more potentially traumatic events during their lives. She currently works as a project manager and post-doctoral researcher for the Department of Epidemiology, Health Promotion and Healthcare Innovation, Public Health Services, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Miranda W. Langendam

Miranda W. Langendam is a clinical epidemiologist. She works at the Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Vivienne De Smet

Vivienne de Smet is a medical student at the University of Amsterdam. For her studies she assisted in this study.

Arianne H. Teeuw

Arianne H. Teeuw is a paediatrician in social paediatrics, researcher and the chair of the Child Abuse and Neglect Team in the Emma Children’s Hospital, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Sonja N. Brilleslijper-Kater

Sonja N. Brilleslijper-Kater is a child psychologist, expert in child abuse and neglect, and researcher for the Child Abuse and the Neglect Team in the Emma Children’s Hospital, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Marc. A. Benninga

Marc. A. Benninga is a paediatrician and gastroenterologist specialized in functional gastrointestinal conditions at the Emma Children’s Hospital, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

Ramón J.L. Lindauer

Ramón J.L. Lindauer is a child and youth psychiatrist specialized in the diagnosis and treatment of trauma and stress-related disorders in children and adolescents at Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Academic Medical Centre, Amsterdam, the Netherlands and De Bascule, Academic Centre for Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Amsterdam, the Netherlands.

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for child behavior checklist/2-3 and 1992 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont.
  • Adams, J. A, Harper, K, Knudson, S, & Revilla, J. (1994). Examination findings in legally confirmed child sexual abuse: it’s normal to be normal. Pediatrics, 94(3), 310-317.
  • Adams, J. A., Kellogg, N. D., Farst, K. J., Harper, N. S., Palusci, V. J., Frasier, L. D.,… Starling, S. P. (2015). Updated guidelines for the medical assessment and care of children who may have been sexually abused. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 29(2), 81–87.
  • Allen, B. (2017). Children with sexual behavior problems: Clinical characteristics and relationship to child maltreatment. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 48(2), 189–199.
  • Allen, B., Thorn, B. L., & Gully, K. J. (2015). A comparison of self-reported emotional and trauma-related concerns among sexually abused children with and without sexual behavior problems. Child Maltreatment, 20(2), 136–140. doi:10.1177/1077559514566864
  • Anderson, B., Thimmesch, I., Aardsma, N., D, M. T., Carstater, S., & Schober, J. (2014). The prevalence of abnormal genital findings, vulvovaginitis, enuresis and encopresis in children who present with allegations of sexual abuse. Journal of Pediatric Urology, 10(6), 1216–1221.
  • Baker, A. J., Gries, L., Schneiderman, M., Parker, R., Archer, M., & Friedrich, B. (2008). Children with problematic sexualized behaviors in the child welfare system. Child Welfare: Journal of Policy, Practice, and Program, 87(1), 5–27.
  • Barth, J., Bermetz, L., Heim, E., Trelle, S., & Tonia, T. (2013). The current prevalence of child sexual abuse worldwide: A systematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of Public Health, 58(3), 469–483. doi:10.1007/s00038-012-0426-1
  • Briere, J. N., & Elliott, D. M. (1994). Immediate and long-term impacts of child sexual abuse. Sexual Abuse of Children, 4(2), 54–69.
  • Brilleslijper-Kater, S. N., & Baartman, H. E. M. (2000). What do young children know about sex? Research on the sexual knowledge of children between the ages of 2 and 6 years. Child Abuse Review, 9, 166–182. doi:10.1002/1099-0852(200005/06)9:3<166::AID-CAR588>3.0.CO;2-3
  • Brilleslijper-Kater, S. N., Friedrich, W. N., & Corwin, D. L. (2004). Sexual knowledge and emotional reaction as indicators of sexual abuse in young children: Theory and research challenges. Child Abuse & Neglect, 28(10), 1007–1017. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.06.005
  • Cale, J., & Lussier, P. (2016). Sexual behaviour in preschool children in the context of intra-parental violence and sexual coercion. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 27(2), 176–190.
  • Chaffin, M., Berliner, L., Block, R., Johnson, T. C., Friedrich, W. N., Louis, D. G., … Madden, C. (2008). Report of the ATSA task force on children with sexual behavior problems. Child Maltreatment, 13(2), 199–218. doi:10.1177/1077559507306718
  • Cohen, J. A., & Mannarino, A. P. (1998). Factors that mediate treatment outcome of sexually abused preschool children: Six- and 12-month follow-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37(1), 44–51. doi:10.1097/00004583-199801000-00016
  • Conte, J. R. (1988). The effects of sexual abuse on children: Results of a research project. Annals of the New York academy of sciences, 528(1), 310–326.
  • Cosentino, C. E., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F., Alpert, J. L., Weinberg, S. L., & Gaines, R. (1995). Sexual behavior problems and psychopathology symptoms in sexually abused girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(8), 1033–1042. doi:10.1097/00004583-199508000-00013
  • Deeks, J. J., . D. J., D’Amico, R., AJ, S., Sakarovitch, C., Song, F., et al. (2003). Evaluating non-randomised intervention studies, Health Technol Assess, 7(27), 1–173.
  • Drach, K. M., Wientzen, J., & Ricci, L. R. (2001). The diagnostic utility of sexual behavior problems in diagnosing sexual abuse in a forensic child abuse evaluation clinic. Child Abuse & Neglect, 25(4), 489–503. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(01)00222-8
  • Everson, M. D., & Faller, K. C. (2012). Base rates, multiple indicators, and comprehensive forensic evaluations: Why sexualized behavior still counts in assessments of child sexual abuse allegations. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 21(1), 45–71. doi:10.1080/10538712.2012.642470
  • Finkelhor, D., & Berliner, L. (1995). Research on the treatment of sexually abused children: A review and recommendations. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(11), 1408–1423. doi:10.1097/00004583-199511000-00007
  • Forbes, F., Duffy, J. C., Mok, J., & Lemvig, J. (2003). Early intervention service for non-abusing parents of victims of child sexual abuse: Pilot study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 183(1), 66–72. doi:10.1192/bjp.183.1.66
  • Friedrich, W. N. (1997). Child sexual behavior inventory: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Friedrich, W. N., Davies, W. H., Feher, E., & Wright, J. (2003). Sexual behavior problems in preteen children: Developmental, ecological, and behavioral correlates. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 989, 95–104. discussion 144-153 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07296.x
  • Friedrich, W. N., Fisher, J., Broughton, D., Houston, M., & Shafran, C. R. (1998). Normative sexual behavior in children: A contemporary sample. Pediatrics, 101(4), E9. doi:10.1542/peds.101.4.e9
  • Friedrich, W. N., Fisher, J. L., Dittner, C. A., Acton, R., Berliner, L., Butler, J., … Wright, J. (2001). Child sexual behavior inventory: Normative, psychiatric, and sexual abuse comparisons. Child Maltreatment, 6(1), 37–49. doi:10.1177/1077559501006001004
  • Friedrich, W. N., Grambsch, P., Broughton, D., Kuiper, J., & Beilke, R. L. (1991). Normative sexual behavior in children. Pediatrics, 88(3), 456–464.
  • Friedrich, W. N., Grambsch, P., Damon, L., Hewitt, S. K., Koverola, C., Lang, R. A., … Broughton, D. (1992). Child sexual behavior inventory: Normative and clinical comparisons. Psychological Assessment, 4(3), 303–311. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.3.303
  • Friedrich, W. N., Jaworski, T. M., Huxsahl, J. E., & Bengtson, B. S. (1997). Dissociative and sexual behaviors in children and adolescents with sexual abuse and psychiatric histories. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 12(2), 155–171. doi:10.1177/088626097012002001
  • Friedrich, W. N., Lysne, M., Sim, L., & Shamos, S. (2004). Assessing sexual behavior in high-risk adolescents with the adolescent clinical sexual behavior inventory (ACSBI). Child Maltreatment, 9(3), 239–250. doi:10.1177/1077559504266907
  • Friedrich, W. N., Sandfort, T. G., Oostveen, J., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2000). Cultural differences in sexual behavior: 2–6 year old Dutch and American children. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 12(1–2), 117–129. doi:10.1300/J056v12n01_08
  • Friedrich, W. N., Urquiza, A. J., & Beilke, R. L. (1986). Behavior problems in sexually abused young children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 11(1), 47–57. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/11.1.47
  • Gagnier, C., & Collin-Vézina, D. (2016). The disclosure experiences of male child sexual abuse survivors. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 25(2), 221–241. doi:10.1080/10538712.2016.1124308
  • Heger, A. H., Ticson, L., Guerra, L., Lister, J., Zaragoza, T., McConnell, G., & Morahan, M. (2002). Appearance of the genitalia in girls selected for nonabuse: Review of hymenal morphology and nonspecific findings. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 15, 27–35. doi:10.1016/S1083-3188(01)00136-X
  • Heger, A. H., Ticson, L., Velasquez, O., & Bernier, R. (2002). Children referred for possible sexual abuse: Medical findings in 2384 children. Children Abuse Negl, 26, 645–659. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(02)00339-3
  • Hershkowitz, I., Lanes, O., & Lamb, M. E. (2007). Exploring the disclosure of child sexual abuse with alleged victims and their parents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31(2), 111–123. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.09.004
  • Jensen, T. K., Gulbrandsen, W., Mossige, S., Reichelt, S., & Tjersland, O. A. (2005). Reporting possible sexual abuse: A qualitative study on children’s perspectives and the context for disclosure. Child Abuse & Neglect, 29(12), 1395–1413. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2005.07.004
  • Jin, Y., Chung, U. S., Jeong, S. H., & Lee, W. K. (2013). The reliability and validity of the Korean version of the child sexual behavior inventory. Psychiatry Investig, 10(4), 336–345. doi:10.4306/pi.2013.10.4.336
  • Kellogg, N. D. (2009). Clinical report: The evaluation of sexual behaviors in children. Committee on Child Abuse and Neglect, American Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics, 124(3), 992–998. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1692
  • Kendall-Tackett, K., Williams, L., & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical studies. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164–180. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.164
  • Kenny, M. C., & Wurtele, S. K. (2013). Child sexual behavior inventory: A comparison between Latino and normative samples of preschoolers. Journal of Sex Research, 50(5), 449–457. doi:10.1080/00224499.2011.652265
  • Larsson, I., & Svedin, C. G. (2001). Sexual behaviour in Swedish preschool children, as observed by their parents. Acta Paediatrica (Oslo, Norway : 1992), 90(4), 436–444. doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2001.tb00446.x
  • Larsson, I., Svedin, C.-G., & Friedrich, W. N. (2000). Differences and similarities in sexual behaviour among pre-schoolers in Sweden and USA. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 54(4), 251–257. doi:10.1080/080394800448110
  • Mannarino, A. P., & Cohen, J. A. (1996). Family-related variables and psychological symptom formation in sexually abused girls. Journal of Children Sexual Abuse: Research, Treatment, & Program Innovations for Victims, Survivors, & Offenders, 5(1), 105–120. doi:10.1300/J070v05n01_06
  • McElvaney, R., Greene, S., & Hogan, D. (2014). To tell or not to tell? factors influencing young people’s informal disclosures of child sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 29(5), 928–947. doi:10.1177/0886260513506281
  • Merrick, M. T., Litrownik, A. J., Everson, M. D., & Cox, C. E. (2008). Beyond sexual abuse: The impact of other maltreatment experiences on sexualized behaviors. Child Maltreatment, 13(2), 122–132. doi:10.1177/1077559507306715
  • Mian, M. W., . W., Klanjer-Diamond, H., Lebaron, D., & Winder, C. (1986). Review of 125 children 6 years of age and under who were sexually abused. Child Abuse & Neglect, 10, 223–229. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(86)90083-9
  • Munzer, A., Fegert, J. M., Ganser, H. G., Loos, S., Witt, A., & Goldbeck, L. (2014). Please Tell! barriers to disclosing sexual victimization and subsequent social support perceived by children and adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. doi:10.1177/0886260514555371
  • Quas, J. A., Goodman, G. S., & Jones, D. (2003). Predictors of attributions of self-blame and internalizing behavior problems in sexually abused children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 44(5), 723–736. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00158
  • RCPCH. (2015). The physical signs of child sexual abuse. London: An evidence-based review and guidance for best practice. Royal College of Paediatrics and Child Health.
  • Sandfort, T. G., & Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2000). Sexual behavior in Dutch and Belgian children as observed by their mothers. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 12(1–2), 105–115. doi:10.1300/J056v12n01_07
  • Schoentjes, E., Deboutte, D., & Friedrich, W. (1999). Child sexual behavior inventory: A Dutch-speaking normative sample. Pediatrics, 104(4 Pt 1), 885–893. doi:10.1542/peds.104.4.885
  • Silovsky, J. F., & Niec, L. (2002). Characteristics of young children with sexual behavior problems: A pilot study. Child Maltreatment, 7(3), 187–197. doi:10.1177/1077559502007003002
  • Singh, M. M., Parsekar, S. S., & Nair, S. N. (2014). An epidemiological overview of child sexual abuse. Journal Family Medica Primary Care, 3(4), 430–435. doi:10.4103/2249-4863.148139
  • St Amand, A., Bard, D. E., & Silovsky, J. F. (2008). Meta-analysis of treatment for child sexual behavior problems: Practice elements and outcomes. Child Maltreatment, 13(2), 145–166. doi:10.1177/1077559508315353
  • Stoltenborgh, M., Van, I., . M. H., Euser, E. M., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2011). A global perspective on child sexual abuse: Meta-analysis of prevalence around the world. Child Maltreatment, 16(2), 79–101. doi:10.1177/1077559511403920
  • Teeuw, A. H., Vrolijk-Bosschaart, T. F., & Langendam, M. W. (2016). [Guideline: Diagnostics for (alleged) child sexual abuse] richtlijn: kindermishandeling, diagnostiek bij (een vermoeden van) seksueel misbruik bij kinderen; nederlandse vereniging voor kindergeneeskunde (NVK). Retrieved from Utrecht: https://www.nvk.nl/tabid/1558/articleType/ArticleView/articleId/1693/default.aspx
  • Thigpen, J. W. (2009). Early sexual behavior in a sample of low-income, African American children. Journal of Sex Research, 46(1), 67–79. doi:10.1080/00224490802645286
  • Wherry, J. N., Jolly, J. B., Feldman, J., Adam, B., & Manjanatha, S. (1995). Child sexual behavior inventory scores for inpatient psychiatric boys: An exploratory study. Journal of Children Sexual Abuse: Research, Treatment, & Program Innovations for Victims, Survivors, & Offenders, 4(3), 95–105. doi:10.1300/J070v04n03_07