245
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Letters to the Editor

Letter to the Editor

Martin, M. A. (2003), “‘It's like…you know’: The Use of Analogies and Heuristics in Teaching Introductory Statistical Methods,” Journal of Statistics Education, 11(2). (ww2.amstat.org/publications/jse/v11n2/martin.html)

Link to a response from the author: http://ww2.amstat.org/publications/jse/v11n3/martin_letter_response.html

As my explorations (CitationLesser 1994; CitationLesser 2002) of the appropriate use of counterintuitive examples have always also been accompanied by the use of intuitive analogies, I read with great interest Michael Martin's article on this latter topic in the July 2003 issue of JSE. While I know it is nearly impossible for an article to be exhaustive, the quest to offer a more complete set of analogies is important because, as CitationDuit (1991, p. 665) notes, “[a] main problem with the approach is that there may not be enough good anchoring situations and bridging analogies available.” Towards this end, allow me to supplement Martin's substantial and well-articulated collection with five additional examples discussed in CitationLesser (1994) that I have found useful in my teaching:

First, the rich crime-and-punishment metaphor for hypothesis testing can be extended (see, for example, CitationEvans 1986) to a detective searching for clues in that chances of discovering significant evidence against the null hypothesis increase if the search can be narrowed by a priori considerations (that is, a one-tailed test), but decrease if the search must extend to both possible locations of significant evidence (that is, a two-tailed test).

CitationEvans (1986) also offers an analogy on the topic of correlation between two variables. He describes how the possible relative vertical positions of neighboring merry-go-round horses illustrate patterns of positive, negative and no correlation.

When teaching confidence intervals, I enjoy using the analogy of falling leaves, as described by CitationWeaver (1992, p.178): “As the trees shed their leaves, piles form around the trunks …. Imagine standing next to a tree's trunk [estimated population mean] and picking up a leaf [sample mean] from the [normal-shaped] pile… . How sure are you that this leaf came from the same tree and not a neighboring one?”

An analogy (that might be even more student-friendly than signal-to-noise ratio) for introducing experimental design is the game of basketball, as used by CitationPolyson and Blick (1985), who articulate counterparts to hypotheses, variables, replication, and counterbalanced design. Most of this analogy is not limited to basketball and can be applied to the team sport of one's choice.

Finally, CitationFreedman, Pisani, Purves, and Adhikari (1991, p.339) offer an accessible analogy to understand how sample quality (that is, randomness) can be far more important than sampling fraction: “Suppose you took a drop of liquid from a bottle, for chemical analysis. If the liquid is well mixed, the chemical composition of the drop would reflect quite faithfully the composition of the whole bottle, and it really wouldn't matter if the bottle was a test tube or a gallon jug.”

References

  • Duit, R. (1991), “On the role of analogies and metaphors in learningscience,” Science Education, 75(6), pp. 649–672.
  • Evans, G. F. (1986), “Getting through statistics with the help ofmetaphors,” Journal of Education for Business, 62(1), pp. 28–30.
  • Freedman, D., Pisani, R., Purves, R., and Adhikari, A. (1991), Statistics (3rd ed.), New York: W. W. Norton.
  • Lesser, L. M. (1994), “The Role of Counterintuitive Examples in StatisticsEducation,” unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.
  • Lesser, L. M. (2002), Letter to the Editor, Journal of StatisticsEducation [Online], 10(1). (ww2.amstat.org/publications/jse/v10n1/lesser_letter.html)
  • Polyson, J. A., and Blick, K. A. (1985), “Basketball game as psychologyexperiment,” Teaching of Psychology, 12(1), pp. 52–53.
  • Weaver, K. A. (1992), “Elaborating selected statistics concepts with commonexperience,” Teaching of Psychology, 19(3), pp. 178–179.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.