5,516
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Prevalence and Psychosocial Correlates of Party-Drug Use and Associated Problems among University Students in the Netherlands

&

ABSTRACT

Background: Recent developments in drug use patterns call for an investigation of current party-drug use and associated problems among college students, who appear to be an important target population for harm reduction interventions. Objectives: In addition to reporting on party-drug use prevalence, we investigated whether initial use and continuation of party-drug use among students was associated with demographic, personality and psychosocial factors. Methods: An online questionnaire was administered to 446 students from a Dutch university, inquiring about party-drug use, demographic characteristics, social norms and personality (big five, impulsiveness, aggression). Univariate and multivariate bootstrapped linear regression analyses were used. Results: Of all students, 22.9% indicated having used party-drugs at least once, with a notable sex difference (39.2% of men vs. 16.2% of women). In contrast to the reported trends in Dutch nightlife, GHB was used rarely (lifetime 1.6%) and new psychoactive substances (NPS; 6.7%) appeared almost equally popular as amphetamines (7.6%) and cocaine (7%). Mild health/psychosocial problems (e.g., doing embarrassing things, feeling unwell) were common (65%), whereas serious problems (e.g., being hospitalized) were rare. Neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness and impulsiveness were associated with lifetime but not regular party-drug use. Of all predictors, lifetime and regular party-drug use were most strongly related to lenient injunctive and descriptive norms in friends, and a low motivation to comply with parents. Conclusions: Our findings indicate that harm reduction/preventive interventions might profit from focusing on social norms, and targeting students who are highly involved in a pro-party-drug environment while experiencing less parental influence.

Introduction

Drug use in Dutch nightlife is constantly developing, with recent increases in use of GHB, ecstasy (MDMA) and the so-called “new psychoactive substances” (NPS; Van Goossens, Frijns, Hasselt, & Van Laar, Citation2013; Linsen et al., Citation2015; Van Amsterdam, Nabben, Keiman, Haanschoten, & Korf, Citation2015). There is evidence suggesting a “normalization” of hard drug use in various Western countries (e.g., Australia; Duff, Citation2005), including the Netherlands (Goossens et al., Citation2013; Van der Poel, Rodenburg, Dijkstra, Stoele, & Van de Mheen, Citation2009; Van Van der Sar et al., Citation2012). A recent study showed that 61% of Dutch people under 35 who frequently visit clubs or festivals, have used ecstasy at least once in the past year (Goossens et al., Citation2013). Collectively, these hard drugs used predominantly in clubs or festivals are referred to as “party-drugs” (Maxwell, Citation2009). The category often includes stimulants (i.e., ecstasy, MDMA, amphetamines, cocaine, crystal meth, and most NPS) as well as sedatives (e.g., GHB, ketamine), but the included drugs vary per study (e.g., Maxwell, Citation2009; Van Van Havere, Vanderplasschen, Broekaert, & De Bourdeaudhui, Citation2009; White et al., Citation2006).

These developments in party-drug use coincide with an increased observation of drug-related health problems that require immediate medical attention (Goossens et al., Citation2013; Krul, Girbes, & Sanou, Citation2012). The majority of party-drugs are associated with adverse health effects, both short-term as well as long-term (e.g., Devlin & Henry, Citation2008; Schep, Knudsen, Slaughter, Vale, & Mégarbane, Citation2012). On short-term, party-drugs can cause a variety of acute health problems, including coma and respiratory failure (e.g., GHB; Schep et al., Citation2012) or cardiac arrest (Devlin & Henry, Citation2008; Trimbos Institute, Citation2015). On long-term, stimulant use has been linked to heart conditions (e.g., cocaine; Phillips et al., Citation2009), neurotoxic effects (e.g., serotonergic neurotoxicity in ecstasy use; Parrott, Citation2013), as well as psychological and cognitive complaints (Parrott, Citation2013; Trimbos Institute, Citation2015). Although intoxicated individuals seeking help at first aid posts in the Netherlands predominantly seek relief for undesired ecstasy effects, mild to severe GHB intoxication in these individuals has risen from 34% in 2009 to 73% in 2015 (Trimbos Institute, Citation2015).

In order to prevent (party-)drug use or reduce its harmful effects, interventions are often targeted at young adults, most notably vocational, college, and university students, as drug initiation and regular use peak around young adulthood (Arria et al., Citation2008; Arria, Vincent, & Caldeira, Citation2009; Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013; Wittchen et al., Citation2008). However, descriptions of party-drug use among students, including the use of trending party-drugs such as NPS, are lacking. Compared with research on alcohol and cannabis use, in students, relatively little research has been done on psychosocial predictors and adverse consequences of party-drug use (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013; Martens et al., Citation2006). Yet, given the high prevalence of party-drug use in people who frequently visit clubs (Goossens et al., Citation2013) and known health and psychosocial consequences of drug use, an examination of party-drug use and possible adverse consequences among students seems important. Additionally, it is unclear which groups of students may be more at risk for lifetime or subsequent regular party-drugs (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013). Identifying psychosocial correlates of the latter group may be especially relevant in light of harm reduction strategies.

Given the varying definition of party-drugs and the limited number of studies on psychosocial correlates of illicit drug use in students, we here include studies on a variety of party-drugs (within our definition) in order to formulate our hypotheses. Factors associated with initiation of drug use among college students were recently reviewed by Dennhardt and Murphy (Citation2013). Being male is highly predictive of drug use (Compton, Thomas, Stinson, & Grant, Citation2007; Kunst, Bachrach, & Bekker, Citation2015; Nolen-Hoeksema, Citation2012; Seedat et al., Citation2009), although some evidence exists that female drug users progress faster to becoming addicted than male users (e.g., Becker, Perry, & Westenbroek, Citation2012). Belonging to a fraternity or sorority has also been found to predict lifetime drug use (e.g., Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013; Maggs, Williams, & Lee, Citation2011; McCabe et al., Citation2005; Yacoubian, Citation2003), and may also be associated with regular drug use, but this has not been previously examined. Additionally, students who live alone or in student homes have been found to binge drink alcohol more frequently than those who still live with their parents (e.g., Kypri et al., Citation2009; Wicki, Kuntsche, & Gmel, Citation2010), but as yet it is unknown whether this effect also exists for (party-)drug use.

A meta-analysis on the role of personality factors in drug use (Kotov, Gamez, Schmidt, & Watson, Citation2010) indicated that drug users are on average more neurotic and less extravert, conscientious, and agreeable than nondrug users. In students, the relation between neuroticism and drug use has been confirmed, but differences in the other personality traits are small or absent (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013; Kashdan, Vetter, & Collins, Citation2005). Accumulating evidence further suggests that impulsivity precedes as well as follows substance abuse, playing a role in drug use initiation as well as development of drug related problems (Verdejo-Garcia, Lawrence, & Clark, Citation2008). In addition, substance abuse has been associated with anti-social personality traits (Compton et al., Citation2007). Results on whether anti-social personality traits are also associated with more frequent substance use are mixed (e.g., Pietrzak & Petry, Citation2005; Westermeyer & Thuras, Citation2005) and this has not been investigated in student populations (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013).

In light of social factors, the prominent motives for ecstasy (Peters, Kok, & Schaalma, Citation2008) and cocaine (Van Van der Poel et al., Citation2009) initiation and continuation included curiosity and social factors, respectively (Van der Poel et al., Citation2009; Vervaeke, Van Deursen, & Kof, Citation2008). Many authors report that social norms regarding substance use highly affect adolescents and young adults (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013). College students who overestimate the extent to which their social environment uses a substance (the descriptive norm), tend to use that substance more themselves (e.g., Larimer et al., Citation2011; Lewis et al., Citation2011; Martens et al., Citation2006; McMillan & Conner, Citation2003). Subjective norms, the extent to which an individual feels it is expected of them to (not) take a substance, can be subdivided into injunctive norms, i.e., the perceived (dis)approval by others regarding substance use, and motivation to comply, i.e., the wish (not) to comply with this norm (Ajzen, Citation1991). Both norms have been found to predict students' use of ecstasy (e.g., Peters, Kok, & Abraham, Citation2007; Umeh & Patel, Citation2004) and other illicit substances (e.g., McMillan & Conner, Citation2003). However, in comparison with studies on alcohol and cannabis, relatively little research has been done on the role of social norms in party-drug use (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013; Martens et al., Citation2006).Also, information is missing on which social factors predict drug use intensification to regular use (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013).

In sum, the rapid developments within drug use patterns call for a more in-depth examination of the current drug use and associated problems among student populations, including trending party-drugs such as GHB and NPS. While many factors have been elucidated that predict which students will likely initiate (party-)drug use, much less effort has been devoted to distinguishing which students are likely to continue their use after initial use (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013). Regular use may increase their chance to experience serious negative consequences from use, and/or develop health and psychosocial problems as a result. Furthermore, most studies in students have so far focused on alcohol and cannabis use. The current study addresses these issues by answering the following research questions:

RQ1:

What is the prevalence of (lifetime and past-year) party-drug use and associated acute health/psychosocial problems among Dutch university students?

RQ2:

Which psychological and social factors are associated with lifetime party-drug use?

RQ3:

Which psychological and social factors are associated with regular party-drug use?

Regular party-drug use was defined in the present study as using any kind of party-drugs twice or more in the last 12 months, based on the distribution of party-drug use in this sample (see Method) and to reflect a usage pattern that is more than sporadically (excluding students who have used party-drugs for the first time in the last year). We hypothesize that demographic factors (age, sex, living situation, member of fraternity/sorority), personality (big five personality traits, impulsiveness, trait aggression), and social norms (of friends and parents) are related to individuals’ initial as well as regular party-drug use.

Method

Procedure

A web-based survey was distributed among college students attending a large urban Dutch university (Leiden University) by means of social media and face-to-face recruitment at the university campus. The sample's characteristics were aimed to match to the overall Leiden student population, regarding age, sex, faculty and membership of a fraternity or sorority. In order to prevent self-selection bias, we masked the study aims by introducing the questionnaire as investigating general characteristics of Leiden University students. The substance use questions were also placed between general questions on nightlife and musical preference. The study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee on March 11th 2015 (PREC15-0224/10).

Respondents

Of the 584 individuals who clicked the link to the questionnaire, 448 (76.7%) completed the items on substance use. Respondents included 448 students (Mage = 21.6, SD = 3.5, 70.4% female), enrolled at the faculties of social sciences (41.9%), humanities (23.3%), law (16.1%), governance and global affairs (6.1%), medicine (5.4%), science (5.4%) and archaeology (1.8%). A minority of the students lived with their parents (23.3%) and almost half (44.4%) belonged to a fraternity or sorority. The sample contained more women (70.4%) than the Leiden student population (59.5%; VSNU, Citation2017) and a larger proportion of social sciences students (41.9% vs. 21.6%; Leiden University, Citation2014). Due to partial data, part of the analysis was carried out for 418 respondents.

Measures

Party-drug use was assessed using a list of substances (), on which participants could indicate which substances they had used at least once, and how many times they had used it in the last 12 months (“not,” “once,” “2 to 4 times,” “5 to 8 times,” “9 to 13 times,” “14 to 20 times,” or “more than 20 times”). Amongst lifetime party-drug users, two categories (“low” and “regular” users) were created based on the variable distributions. For each substance, 50% or more party-drug users indicated to have “not” or “once” used the substance in the last 12 months. In all substances except ecstasy/MDMA, more than 75% of party-drug users indicated not having used the substance in the last 12 months. “Regular” use was therefore defined as having used any type of party-drugs twice or more in the last 12 months; using “once or more” as criterion would not be appropriate, as individuals who have only once used any type of party-drugs in their lives (in the last 12 months) would therefore be categorized as a “regular” user.

Table 1. Prevalence of alcohol, cannabis and party-drug use among students (N = 446); men (132) and women (314).

Participants were also asked whether they ever combine party-drug use with alcohol use, and whether they ever take multiple types of drugs on one occasion. Alcohol and cannabis use were assessed by asking respondents to indicate their average use per month (alcohol in units of glasses, cannabis in number of “cannabis joints”).

As predictor variables, we included demographic (age, sex, living situation, member of fraternities/sororities), personality and social aspects. Personality was subdivided into the “big five” traits, impulsivity and aggressive personality traits. The Dutch Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, Citation2003) assessed the big five traits (Openness to experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness and Neuroticism) on items ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree), e.g., “I see myself as someone who is – extravert, enthusiastic.” Each scale ranges from 2 to 14.

Lower than reported by Gosling et al. (Citation2003), the TIPI's internal consistency in this sample was α = .23 for the factor Agreeableness, α = .33 for Openness, α = .55 for Conscientiousness, α = .64 for Neuroticism and α = .67 for Extraversion. A post-hoc investigation of the literature on the TIPI indicates that the subscales Neuroticism and Extraversion seem to capture the corresponding big five concepts very well, whereas Conscientiousness, Agreeableness and most notably Openness perform worse (Hofmans, Kuppens, & Allik, Citation2008) and are less reliable (Ehrhart et al., Citation2009). Due to the unacceptable psychometric qualities, the scales Agreeableness and Openness were excluded from further analysis.

Impulsivity was assessed using the 8-item Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Brief (BIS-brief; Fields et al., Citation2015; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, Citation1995). It showed acceptable internal consistency in the current sample (Cronbach's α = .76) and scores could range from 0 to 8. As an indicator of anti-social tendencies, a Dutch (Meester, Muris, Bosman, Schouten, & Beuving, Citation1996) abbreviated (Hornsveld, Muris, Kraaimaat, & Meesters, Citation2009) version of the Agression Questionnaire (AQ; Buss & Perry, Citation1992) was used. AQ scores could range from 12 to 60 and the AQ had a good internal consistency in this sample, Cronbach's α = .80.

As factors related to the social environment, injunctive and descriptive norms of the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, Citation1991) were assessed regarding party-drug use, with parents and peers as reference groups (e.g., injunctive norm: “My parents/peers would approve of me using party-drugs”; descriptive norms: “How many of your friends/parents would you say use party-drugs?”). Further, we assessed respondents’ motivation to comply with the injunctive norm (e.g., “Regarding party-drug use, to what extent do you want to do what your friends/parents would want you to do?”). All social norm variables were scored from 0 to 100, with higher levels representing more lenient social norms towards party-drugs.

In absence of a validated questionnaire on party-drug related problems, we adapted the Dutch version of the Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire (B-YAACQ; Kahler, Strong, & Read, Citation2005) to measure adverse consequences of party-drug use (see ). As some items of the B-YAACQ included alcohol-specific effects (e.g., nausea), we only included subscales on risky behavior, social/interpersonal functioning and academic/occupational functioning. In addition, we created items on adverse sexual situations, physical harm, and delinquency related to party-drug use, and driving under the influence of party-drugs (see ). All items could be answered by indicating never, once or more than once (in the last 12 months).

Table 2. Frequency of health and psychosocial problems due to party-drug use (N = 99Footnote*).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 20.0. In all analyses an α of .05 with two-tailed hypothesis testing was used. Of the 448 respondents, data of two respondents were removed due to invalid responses (total N = 446 for prevalence statistics and N = 418 for the ANOVA and logistic regression analyses, due to partial data). The variable “descriptive norms of parents” was excluded, because only three respondents indicated that one of their parents used party-drugs.

In reporting the prevalence of party-drugs use (RQ1), Chi squared and Mann–Whitney tests were used to assess sex differences in substance use, due to skewed distributions. Regarding the mean psychological and social correlates (RQ2 and RQ3), the statistical assumptions for the ANOVA's () were also violated due to skewed variable distributions per group and heterogeneity of variances (Neal & Simon, Citation2007). As transforming these variables proved insufficient, all ANOVA's were bootstrapped (1000 repetitions, bias corrected accelerated (BCA) confidence intervals (CI), stratified sampling) in line with recommendations of Barber & Thompson, Citation2000; Neal & Simon, Citation2007; and Parra-Frutos, Citation2014.

Table 3. Bootstrapped means and standard deviations of characteristics among never-party-drug users, low frequent (≤1 last year) party-drug users and regular (≥2 last year) party-drug users.

To investigate RQ2 and RQ3, two logistic regression analyses were carried out, both including the following predictors: Demographic factors (age, sex, living situation, fraternity/sorority membership), personality (big five traits, impulsivity, aggressiveness) and social norms (injunctive and descriptive norms, motivation to comply). Predictors were assessed in univariate and multivariate analyses. No multicollinearity problems were detected. In line with recommendations by Vittinghoff and McCulloch (Citation2006), bootstrapping was used in order to obtain bias-corrected confidence intervals, as the analyses included less than 10 events per predictor variable (1000 repetitions, BCA CI's and stratified sampling).

For RQ2, the logistic regression analysis contained lifetime party-drug use (no or yes) as dependent variable and it was carried out for all participants who had completed the questionnaire (N = 418). The logistic regression analysis for RQ3 was carried out for respondents who had used party-drugs at least once in their lives and who also completed the questionnaire (N = 95). The dependent variable was a dummy variable indicating low use (once or less in the last 12 months) or regular use (twice or more in the last 12 months). To prevent overfitting problems, in this logistic regression analysis only univariate predictors with p < .10 were entered in the multivariate analysis.

Results

Prevalence of party-drug use and associated problems

Of all 446 students, 22.9% (N = 102) had used at least one type of party-drugs at least once in their lives (). A larger proportion of males reported to have tried one or more party-drugs (38.6%) than women (16.2%), and men generally reported more substance use (). Alcohol and cannabis were used at least once by a large majority (92.8%) and almost half (41.9%) of the participants respectively. Considering the “trending” party-drugs GHB and NPS, as expected, some use was detected in our sample, although GHB use was rare (1.6%; ). Among the NPS, 4-Fluoroamphetamine (4-FA) appeared to be most popular: Lifetime 4-FA use was 5.4%, whereas lifetime use of other NPS were 1.6% or less. Only two respondents reported having used crystal meth, and none reported using it in the last 12 months.

Among party-drug users, half (51%) reported sometimes drinking alcohol while being under the influence of any drugs (combined use with ecstasy 28.4%, cannabis 22.5%, cocaine 14.7%, MDMA 12.7%, amphetamine 6.9%, NPS 3.9%, and GHB 1%). Additionally, 31.4% of users reported sometimes combining different drugs on one occasion. Almost all possible combinations between cannabis, ecstasy, MDMA, amphetamine, cocaine, GHB, and NPS were reported at least once. Ecstasy was combined with other drugs most frequently (with cannabis 10.8%, NPS 8.8%, amphetamine 7.8%, MDMA 6.9%, cocaine 3.9%, and GHB 1%).

Over half (64.6%) of party-drug users reported to have experienced at least one acute health or psychosocial problem in the last 12 months. The most reported events were feeling ill after taking party-drugs (30.3%), doing embarrassing things (25.3%), missing classes or work due to a hangover (24.2%), and taking foolish risks (21.1%; ). Least frequently reported negative events were being taken to the hospital (1%), having sex against one's will (1%), and being involved in an accident (1%). No one reported to have driven a car after having taken “too much” party-drugs (0%; ), but seven individuals (6.9%) reported having driven a car after taking party-drugs at least once.

Prediction of initial and regular party-drug use

Next, we investigated in two regression analyses which of the demographic, personality and social factors could distinguish whether a student would be classified as a (1) lifetime never-user or lifetime user (RQ2), and (2) low frequent (≤1 last year) or regular (≥2 last year) party-drugs user (RQ3). The means and standard deviations of the predictors by group are displayed in . Group differences were found in almost all predictors, except for age, motivation to comply with friends regarding party-drug use and aggressiveness. Games-Howell post hoc comparisons () revealed that most differences occurred between the never-user group and the (low and/or regular) user groups. Compared to the never-user group, user groups also contained relatively more men, χ² (2) = 27.69, p < .001, students of fraternities/sororities, χ² (2) = 14.43, p < .001, and students who do not live with their parents, χ² (2) = 8.29, p < .05. The low frequent and regular user groups only differed with respect to injunctive and descriptive norms of friends, and motivation to comply with parents ().

displays the first logistic regression analysis, in which lifetime party-drug use was predicted in univariate and multivariate analyses (RQ2). The univariate predictors revealed male students and individuals who consume more alcohol monthly, were also more likely to be classified as lifetime party-drug users. The same applies to individuals who do not live with parents, who are member of a fraternity or sorority and whose parents and friends seem more lenient towards party-drugs. Inversely, higher motivation to comply with parents was associated with a smaller chance to be a lifetime party-drug user. In light of personality, higher levels of extraversion and impulsiveness were positively associated with lifetime party-drug use, whereas higher levels of neuroticism and conscientiousness lowered the chance to be classified as a party-drug user. Contrary to our hypotheses, age, monthly cannabis use, motivation to comply with friends and aggressiveness were not associated with lifetime party-drug use. When entered together in the multivariate model, only (male) sex and several social norms (most regarding friends) emerged as significant predictors of lifetime party-drug use.

Table 4. Predictors of lifetime party-drug use (no/yes; N = 418).

In the second logistic regression analysis (), low frequent or regular party-drug use was predicted (RQ3). In both the univariate and multivariate analyses, only three social norms emerged as significant predictors: The injunctive norm of friends, the descriptive norm of friends, and the motivation to comply with parents regarding party-drug use. Thus, party-drug users who report more lenient injunctive norms among friends and whose friends are perceived to use party-drugs, are more likely to be classified as regular party-drug users. Individuals who have a stronger wish to comply with their parents regarding party-drugs, seem to use party-drugs less frequently.

Table 5. Predictors of regular (≥2 last year) party-drug use (N = 95).

Discussion

The aims of the current study were to report on prevalence of party-drug use and associated psychosocial problems among Dutch university students, and to identify psychosocial correlates of lifetime and regular party-drug use. Of all students, 22.9% had ever used party-drugs, and 14.6% at least twice in the last 12 months. In line with earlier reports (Goossens et al., Citation2013; Dorsselaer & Goossens, Citation2015), the party-drug ecstasy/MDMA was by far the most popular, followed by amphetamines, cocaine and NPS. The prevalence statistics are similar to other Dutch (De Hoogh, & De Jong, Citation2014; Universiteitskrant Groningen, Citation2014) and USA studies (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013). Only a handful of students reporting lifetime GHB use. In contrast, NPS were used as frequently as more well-known hard drugs, such as amphetamines and cocaine.

The acute health and psychosocial problems assessed in this study do not seem alarming: Few students reported serious impairment in social or academic functioning, acute health problems, or severe consequences (e.g., violence, accidents, hospitalization). Although experiencing some psychosocial or health problem due to party-drugs was common (64.6%), most were “mild” (feeling unwell, doing embarrassing things). Somewhat worrisome are the relatively high rates of poly drug-use (31.4%) and the occurrence (6.9%) of driving under the influence of party-drugs.

In order to inform preventive/harm reduction strategies, the second aim of this study was to identify psychosocial correlates of lifetime and regular party-drug use. Males more frequently reported to have ever used party-drug than females (39% vs. 16 %; conform results by, e.g., Seedat et al., Citation2009). However, male sex was not a significant predictor for regular party-drug use, indicating that male students may not necessarily continue their party-drug use more frequently than females (Becker et al., Citation2012). Age was not associated with lifetime or regular use, even though older students were expected to have experienced more opportunities to initiate (party-)drug use (Arria et al., Citation2008).

Lifetime and regular party-drug users appeared to be less neurotic and conscientious, and more extravert and impulsive compared with never-users; aggressiveness was not significantly associated. These results are not entirely consistent with studies on personality of individuals with substance abuse disorders, who are often found to be more neurotic and less extravert compared with never-users (Kotov et al., Citation2010). One might speculate that substance use is differently related to personality in patients than in students: In some students, being highly sociable (low in neuroticism, high in extraversion) might contribute to more “partying” and thereby more opportunities to come into contact with (party-)drugs; whereas in patients, vulnerability for mental disorders such as depression and anxiety (higher neuroticism and introversion) might contribute to drug use susceptibility. Similarly, party-drug use in this student sample may not be embedded in a criminal setting, thereby not producing a significant correlation between party-drug use and aggression (Compton et al., Citation2007).

Additionally, it should be noted that personality traits may also change as a consequence of prolonged exposure to psychoactive substances. Impulsivity, for instance, has been found to increase in substance abuse patients (Verdejo-Garcia et al., Citation2008). However, longitudinal studies on personality and substance use provide more evidence for a model in which development of personality factors precedes substance use initiation and continuation (Verheul & Van den Brink, Citation2005). Indicators of behavioral disinhibition and novelty seeking, for instance, can be observed during childhood and have been shown to predict later substance use. This indicates that development of personality either precedes substance use and abuse (“primary personality disorder model”), or that third factors affect both early personality development as well as later substance use and abuse (“common factor model”; Verheul & Van den Brink, Citation2005).

Furthermore, students who do not live with parents and who are members of fraternities/sororities were more likely to have used party-drugs at some time in their lives. These findings coincide with studies on alcohol (e.g., Kypri et al., Citation2009; Maggs et al., Citation2011). However, party-drug users who do not live with their parents and who are members of fraternities/sororities were not more likely to be regular users. Furthermore, injunctive and descriptive norms of friends also related strongly to both lifetime party-drug use and regular party-drug use: Students who feel that their friends are lenient about party-drugs, and/or who think many friends use party-drugs, are more likely to use party-drugs themselves. It should be noted that the causality of this association may be reversed: Students who use party-drugs might also (erroneously) project favourable party-drug related attitudes onto their friends (Martens et al., Citation2006). However, longitudinal studies on the Theory of Planned Behavior (Ajzen, Citation1991) show that perceived social norms can also precede substance use, indicating that a causal pathway from perceived social norms to later substance use is plausible (e.g., Malmberg et al., Citation2012).

Surprisingly, motivation to comply with friends was in itself not associated with lifetime or regular party-drug use. A possible explanation is that compliance with friends may mean “not using party-drugs” in some instances, and “using (more) party-drugs” in other instances. Alternatively, individuals may not experience their decision to initiate party-drugs to stem from adherence to others’ norms (i.e., “peer pressure”): In the Groningen study, only 3% of ecstasy users indicated having used ecstasy out of peer pressure, perhaps due to social desirability (De Hoogh & De Jong, Citation2014).

The injunctive norms of parents did not predict lifetime and regular party-drug use, possibly due to little variation in this variable: Most students indicated that their parents would disapprove of party-drug use. Similarly, only a handful of students indicated that their parent(s) used party-drugs. The motivation to comply with parents, though, was a significant (negative) predictor of both lifetime and regular party-drug use: Students who feel less urge to comply with their parents regarding party-drug use, have a larger chance to use party-drugs (lifetime and regular use).

Finally, we assessed the relative strength of these predictors in the multivariate analyses. Interestingly, only male sex, social norms regarding friends and the motivation to comply with parents distinguished lifetime users from never-users. A similar pattern emerged when looking at the prediction of regular party-drug use: only injunctive and descriptive norms regarding friends and motivation to comply with parents were predictors of regular party-drug use in the multivariate analysis. A possible interpretation of these findings is that party-drug use among students may be best predicted by a tendency to be close with other students who (also) enjoy substance use and “partying,” while being relatively distanced from parental influences (i.e., are less inclined to adhere to their standards). Students who show these tendencies may be relatively more extravert, impulsive, and emotionally stable, and thus open to exploring new and stimulating environments; to live in student homes and to be members of fraternities/sororities. Through these environments they may have a larger chance to come into contact with students who use party-drugs. In the same line, a recent study on alcohol consumption also found that proximate social factors were stronger predictors for drinking status than parental norms and personality (Lac & Donaldson, Citation2016).

Our findings suggest that both preventive and harm reduction interventions may profit from targeting proximate social norms. Several programs targeting these social norms have been developed. For instance, Stock, Vallentin-Holbech, and Rasmussen (Citation2016) created a program in which pupils first indicate their own substance use and perceived norms through online questionnaires, and subsequently receive corrective information about substance use rates and social norms of their peers. Similarly, Neighbors et al. (Citation2011) found that college students in the control condition consumed more alcohol weekly than participants receiving corrective information about peers’ alcohol consumption. Social norms interventions adapted specifically for party-drugs may be promising to prevent or decrease party-drug use among college students.

Several limitations of the current study should be noted. First, there are obvious limitations related to the use of self-report measures (e.g., underreporting party-drug use out of social desirability). Second, despite our efforts, the sample was not entirely representative of the student population studied: There were fewer men, and most students were enrolled at the faculty of social sciences, possibly lowering the total estimated party-drug use prevalence. Notably, it seems unlikely that self-selection effects have clouded the results, as we carefully masked the aims of the study. Third, some of the utilized measurement instruments had inferior psychometric quality than expected, leading to the exclusion of two relevant big five dimensions (agreeableness and openness). Replication using a sounder instrument is therefore recommended. Similarly, universal agreed-upon definitions are lacking regarding “party-drugs” and “regular use.” Finally, as discussed, the correlational nature of our study prevents us from drawing strong conclusions regarding the causal relations between party-drug use and perceived social norms and personality traits. Longitudinal studies assessing the relation between social norms, personality and party-drug use may address this issue.

Within the scope of these limitations we highlight several strengths, conclusions and implications. To our knowledge, this is the most recent study in the Netherlands on current drug use among college students, including trending substances such as NPS and GHB. Our results indicate that party-drug use is not uncommon among students. Our findings extend previous research by reporting on consequences of drugs other than cannabis, a shortcoming in the current literature (Dennhardt & Murphy, Citation2013), and preliminarily investigating correlates of regular party-drug use, in addition to lifetime use.

Even though serious acute health and psychosocial problems seemed rare in students, party-drug use in this population nevertheless requires monitoring, as long-term health outcomes of party-drug use are largely unknown (Linsen et al., Citation2015). Some clusters of individuals seem at risk of developing a more regular drug use pattern, and possibly developing acute health or psychosocial problems. These individuals may be characterized by a tendency to be close to people who (also) enjoy “partying” and party-drugs, and relatively far away from parental influences. These results indicate that interventions among students may benefit from targeting proximate social influences (e.g., Helmer, Muellmann, Zeeb, & Pischke, Citation2016).

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Ferdi Honders, Prema Koendan, Stijn van der Linden, Bart van der Meer, Laura van Mil, Daphne Nelis, Josine Vanmaris, and Bastiaan de Zwart for their highly valued assistance in data collection.

Supplemental material

ISUM_A_1455700_Supplementary-materials.docx

Download MS Word (15.7 KB)

Declaration of interest

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the article. This manuscript has not been published elsewhere and has not been submitted for publication somewhere else simultaneously.

Additional information

Funding

This study was supported by Leiden University.

References

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision, 50(2), 179–211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
  • Arria, A. M., Caldeira, K. M., O'Grady, K. E., Vincent, K. B., Fitzelle, D. B., Johnson, E. P., & Wish, E. D. (2008). Drug exposure opportunities and use patterns among college students: Results of a longitudinal prospective cohort study. Substance Abuse, 29(4), 19–38. doi: 10.1080/08897070802418451
  • Arria, A. M., Vincent, K. B., & Caldeira, K. M. (2009). Measuring liability for substance use disorder among college students: Implications for screening and early intervention. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 35, 233–241. doi: 10.1080/00952990903005957
  • Barber, J. A., & Thompson, S. G. (2000). Analysis of cost data in randomized trials: An application of the non-parametric bootstrap. Statistics in Medicine, 19, 3219–3236. doi: 10.1002/1097-0258(20001215)19:23<3219::AID-SIM623>3.0.CO;2-P
  • Becker, J. B., Perry, A. N., & Westenbroek, C. (2012). Sex differences in the neural mechanisms mediating addiction: A new synthesis and hypothesis. Biology of Sex Differences, 3(14), 1–35. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-3-14
  • Buss, A. F., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452–459. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
  • Compton, W. M., Thomas, Y. F., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2007). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence in the United States – results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(5), 566–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.830
  • De Hoogh, R., & De Jong, T. (2014). 1 op 3 EUR studenten gebruikt harddrugs. Credo Magazine, April 2014.
  • Dennhardt, A. A., & Murphy, J. G. (2013). Prevention and treatment of college student drug use: A review of the literature. Addictive Behaviors, 38, 2607–2618. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.06.006
  • Devlin, R., & Henry, J. (2008). Clinical review: Major consequences of illicit drug consumption. Critical Care, 12(1), 1–7. doi:10.1186/cc6166
  • Dorsselaer, S. van, & Goossens, F. X. (2015). Alcohol-, tabaks- en drugsgebruikdoor studenten Inventarisatie van (onderzoeken naar) prevalentieschattingen onder MBO-, HBO- en WO-studenten in Nederland. Trimbos Instituut, Utrecht.
  • Duff, C. (2005). Party drugs and party people: Examining the ‘normalization’ of recreational drug use in Melbourne, Australia. International Journal of Drug Policy, 16, 161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2005.02.001
  • Ehrhart, M., Ehrhart, K., Roesch, S., Chung-Herrera, B., Nadler, K., & Bradshaw, K. (2009). Testing the latent factor structure and construct validity of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 900–905. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.07.012
  • Fields, S., Edens, J. F., Smith, S. T., Rulseh, A., Donnellan, M. B., Ruiz, M. A., … Douglas, K. S. (2015). Examining the psychometric properties of the Barratt ImpulsivenessScale–Brief Form in justice-involved samples. Psychological Assessment, 27(4), 1211–1218. doi: 10.1037/a0039109
  • Goossens, F. X., Frijns, T., van Hasselt, N. E., & Van Laar, M. W. (2013). Het grote uitgaansonderzoek 2013. Uitgaanspatronen, middelengebruik en risicogedrag onder uitgaande jongeren en jongvolwassenen. Utrecht: Trimbos-instituut.
  • Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1
  • Helmer, S. M., Muellmann, S., Zeeb, H., & Pischke, C. R. (2016). Development and evaluation of the efficacy of a web-based ‘social norms’-intervention for the prevention and reduction of substance use in a cluster-controlled trial conducted at eight German universities. BMC Public Health, 16, 252–258. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-2898-z
  • Hofmans, J., Kuppens, P., & Allik, J. (2008). Is short in length short in content? An examination of the domain representation of the Ten Item Personality Inventory scales in Dutch language. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(8), 750–755. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.004
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Muris, P., Kraaimaat, F. W., & Meesters, C. (2009). Psychometric properties of the aggression questionnaire in Dutch violent forensic psychiatric patients and secondary vocational students. Assessment, 16(2), 181–192. doi: 10.1177/1073191108325894
  • Kahler, C. W., Strong, D. R., & Read, J. P. (2005). Towards efficient and comprehensive measurement of the alcohol problems continuum in college students: The Brief Young Adult Alcohol Consequences Questionnaire. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 29, 1180–1189. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000171940.95813.A5
  • Kashdan, T. B., Vetter, C. J., & Collins, R. L. (2005). Substance use in young adults: Associations with personality and gender. Addictive Behaviors, 30(2), 259–269. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.05.014
  • Kotov, R., Gamez, W., Schmidt, F., & Watson, D. (2010). Linking "big" personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 768–821. doi:10.1037/a0020327
  • Krul, J., Girbes, A. R. J., & Sanou, B. T. (2012). Increase in serious ecstasy-related incidents in the Netherlands. The Lancet, 380, 1385. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61800-5
  • Kunst, L. E., Bachrach, N., & Bekker, M. H. J. (2015). Aandacht voor genderverschillen in het GGZ-gerelateerde onderwijs: Relevante informatie en praktische tips voor docenten. Tijdschrift voor Genderstudies, 18(4), 383–399. doi: 10.5117/TVGN2015.4.KUNS
  • Kypri, K., Paschall, M. J., Langley, J., Baxter, J., Cashell-Smith, M., & Bourdeau, B. (2009). Drinking and alcohol-related harm among New Zealand university students: Findings from a national web-based survey. Alcoholism – Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(2), 307–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00834.x
  • Lac, A., & Donaldson, C. D. (2016). Alcohol attitudes, motives, norms, and personality traits longitudinally classify non-drinkers, moderate drinkers, and binge drinkers using discriminant function analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 61, 91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.05.006
  • Larimer, M. E., Neighbors, C., LaBrie, J. W., Atkins, D. C., Lewis, M. A., Lee, C. M., … Walter, T. (2011). Descriptive drinking norms: For whom does reference group matter? Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72(5), 833–884. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.833
  • Leiden University (2014). Kerncijfers Universiteit Leiden [Key figures of Leiden University]. Retrieved from http://organisatie.leidenuniv.nl/feiten-en-cijfers/.
  • Lewis, M. A., Litt, D. M., Balyney, J. A., Lostutter, T. W., Granato, H., Kilmer, J. R., & Lee, C. M. (2011). They drink how much and where? Normative perceptions by drinking contexts and their association to college students' alcohol consumption. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 72(5), 844–853. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.844
  • Linsen, F., Koning, R. P. J., Van Laar, M., Niesink, R. J. M., Koeter, M. W., & Brunt, T. N. (2015). 4-Fluoroamphetamine in the Netherlands: More than a one-night stand. Addiction, 110, 1138–1143. doi: 10.1111/add.12932
  • Maggs, J. L., Williams, L. R., & Lee, C. M. (2011). Ups and downs of alcohol use among first-year college students: Number of drinks, heavy drinking, and stumble and pass out drinking days. Addictive Behaviors, 36(3), 197–202. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005
  • Malmberg, M., Overbeek, G., Vermulst, A., Monshouwer, K., Vollebergh, W., & Engels, R. (2012). The theory of planned behavior: Precursors of marijuana use in early adolescence? Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 123(1-3), 22–28. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.10.011
  • Martens, M. P., Page, J. C., Mowry, E. S., Daman, K. M., Taylor, K. K., & Cimini, C. D. (2006). Differences between actual and perceived student norms: An examination of alcohol use, drug use, and sexual behavior. Journal of American College Health, 54(5), 295–300. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.5.295-300
  • Maxwell, J. (2009). Party drugs: Properties, prevalence, patterns, and problems. Substance Use & Misuse, 40(9-10), 1203–1240. doi:10.1081/JA-200066736
  • McCabe, S. E., Schulenberg, J. E., Jognston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Klonska, D. D. (2005). Selection and socialization effects of fraternities and sororities on US college student substance use: A multi-cohort national longitudinal study. Addiction, 100(4), 512–524. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.03.008
  • McMillan, B., & Conner, M. (2003). Applying an extended version of the theory of planned behavior to illicit drug use among students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(8), 1662–1683. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01968.x
  • Meester, C., Muris, P., Bosman, H., Schouten, E., & Beuving, S. (1996). Psychometric evaluation of the Dutch version of the aggression questionnaire. Behavior Research and Therapy, 34, 839–845. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(96)00065-4
  • Neal, D. J., & Simon, J. S. (2007). Inference in regression models of heavily skewed alcohol use data: A comparison of ordinary least squares, generalized linear models, and Bootstrap Resampling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(4), 441–452. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.4.441
  • Neighbors, C., Jensen, M., Tidwell, J., Walter, T., Fossos, N., & Lewis, M. (2011). Social-norms interventions for light and nondrinking students. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 14(5), 651–669.
  • Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2012). Emotion regulation and psychopathology: The role of gender. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 161–187. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143109
  • Parra-Frutos, I. (2014). Controlling the Type I error rate by using the nonparametric bootstrap when comparing means. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 67, 117–132. doi: 10.1111/bmsp.12011
  • Parrott, A. C. (2013). Human psychobiology of MDMA or ‘Ecstasy’: An overview of 25 years of empirical research. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental, 28(4), 289–307.
  • Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–774. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1
  • Peters, G. Y., Kok, G., & Abraham, C. (2007). Social cognitive determinants of ecstasy use totarget in evidence-based interventions: A meta-analytical review. Addiction, 103, 109–118. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.02041.x
  • Peters, G. Y., Kok, G., & Schaalma, H. P. (2008). Careers in ecstasy use: Do ecstasy users cease of their own accord? Implications for intervention development. BMC Public Health, 8, 376–386. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-8-376
  • Phillips, K., Luk, A., Soor, G. S., Abraham, J. R., Leong, S., & Butany, J. (2009). Cocaine cardiotoxicity: A review of the pathophysiology, pathology, and treatment options. American Journal of Cardiovascular Drugs, 9(3), 177–196. doi: 10.1007/BF03256574
  • Pietrzak, R. H., & Petry, N. M. (2005). Antisocial personality disorder is associated with increased severity of gambling, medical, drug and psychiatric problems among treatment-seeking pathological gamblers. Addiction, 100(8), 1183–1193. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01151.x
  • Schep, L., Knudsen, K., Slaughter, R., Vale, J., & Mégarbane, B. (2012). The clinical toxicology of gamma-hydroxybutyrate, gamma-butyrolactone and 1,4-butanediol. Clinical Toxicology, 50(6), 458–470. doi:10.3109/15563650.2012.702218
  • Seedat, S., Scott, K. M., Angermeyer, M. C., Berglund, P., Bromet, E. J., Brugha, T. S., … Kessler, R. C. (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 785–795. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.36
  • Stock, C., Vallentin-Holbech, L., & Rasmussen, B. (2016). The GOOD life: Study protocol for a social norms intervention to reduce alcohol and other drug use among Danish adolescents. BMC Public Health, 16(1), . doi:10.1186/s12889-016-3333-1
  • Trimbos Institute (2015). Monitor Drug-related incidents: Report 2015. Utrecht: Trimbos Institute. Obtained through https://assets.trimbos.nl/docs/eb36c36b-f945-49f6-ad06-4cd56659a4f0.pdf
  • Umeh, K., & Patel, R. (2004). Theory of planned behaviour and ecstast use: An analysis of moderator-interactions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 25–38. doi: 10.1348/135910704322778704
  • Universiteitskrant Groningen (2014). Harddrugsgebruik onder RUG-studenten. Obtained through http://archief.ukrant.nl/wp-content/uploads/2014/12/UK-XTC-en-harddrugs-resultaten.pdf.
  • Van Amsterdam, J. G. C., Nabben, T., Keiman, D., Haanschoten, G., & Korf, D. (2015). Exploring the attractiveness of New Psychoactive Substances (NPS) among experienced drug users. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 47(3), 177–181. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2015.1048840
  • Van der Poel, A., Rodenburg, G., Dijkstra, M., Stoele, M., & Van de Mheen, D. (2009). Trends, motivations and settings of recreational cocaine use by adolescents and young adults in the Netherlands. International Journal of Drug Policy, 20, 143–151. doi:10.1016/j.drugpo.2008.02.005
  • Van der Sar, R., Ødegård, E., Rise, J., Brouwers, E. P. M., Van de Goor, L. A. M., & Garretsen, H. F. L. (2012). Acceptance of illicit drug use in the Netherlands and Norway: A cross-national survey. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 19(5), 397–405. doi: 10.3109/09687637.2012.671859
  • Van Havere, T., Vanderplasschen, W., Broekaert, E., & De Bourdeaudhui, I. (2009). The influence of age and gender on party drug use among young adults attending dance events, clubs, and rock festivals in Belgium. Substance Use & Misuse, 44(13), 1899–1915. doi:10.3109/10826080902961393
  • Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience of Biobehavioral Review, 32(4), 777–810. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003
  • Vervaeke, H. K. E., Van Deursen, L., & Kof, D. J. (2008). The role of peers in the initiation and continuation of Ecstasy use. Substance Use & Misuse, 43(5), 633–646. doi: 10.1080/10826080701204854
  • Vittinghof, E., & McCulloch, C. E. (2006). Relaxing the rule of ten events per variable in logistic and Cox regression. American Journal of Epidemiology, 165(6), 710–718. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwk052
  • VSNU [Association of Universities The Netherlands; Vereniging van Samenwerkende Nederlandse Universiteiten]. (2017). Ingeschreven studenten per 1 oktober [number of enrolled students on October 1st]. Retrieved from http://www.vsnu.nl/f_c_studenten_downloads.html
  • Verheul, R., & Brink, W. (2005). Causal pathways between substance use disorders and personality pathology. Australian Psychologist, 40(2), 127–136.
  • Westermeyer, J., & Thuras, P. (2005). Association of antisocial personality disorder and substance disorder morbidity in a clinical sample. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 31(1), 93–110. doi:10.1081/ada-200047895
  • White, B., Degenhardt, L., Breen, C., Bruno, R., Newman, J., & Proudfoot, P. (2006). Risk and benefit perceptions of party drug use. Addictive Behaviors, 31, 137–142. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.04.003
  • Wicki, M., Kuntsche, E., & Gmel, E. (2010). Drinking at european universities? A review of students’ alcohol use. Addictive Behaviors, 35(11), 913–924. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.06.015
  • Wittchen, H. U., Behrendt, S., Höfler, M., Perkonigg, A., Lieb, R., Bühringer, G., & Beesdo, K. (2008). What are the high risk periods for incident substance use and transitions to abuse and dependence? Implications for early intervention and prevention. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 17(S1), S16–S29. doi: 10.1002/mpr.254
  • Yacoubian, G. S. (2003). Correlates of ecstasy use among students surveyed through the 1997 college alcohol study. Journal of Drug Education, 33(1), 61–69. doi: 10.2190/DVEE-3UML-2HDB-D4XV