752
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Little Evidence for the Role of Disgust Sensitivity in Implicit Disgust to Images of White People Engaged in Injecting Drug Use (IDU)

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

Abstract

Background: Previous research has shown that People Who Inject Drugs (PWID) are subject to public stigma, which affects access to, and provision and quality of, treatment and support services. Less is known about the socio-cognitive processes that support the development and maintenance of public stigma toward PWID. The present study investigated the role of disgust sensitivity in implicit disgust to injecting drug use. Methods: 126 participants took part in an online Implicit Association Task (IAT) measuring implicit disgust to pictorial stimuli of injecting drug use or medical injecting. Participants also completed The Disgust Scale Revised, Injecting Phobia Scale (Short Form), Attitudes to People Who Use Drugs (PWUD) scale and a substance use inventory. Results: Average IAT score was negative indicating significantly higher implicit disgust to injecting drug use. Hierarchical linear regression found that injecting phobia predicted implicit disgust to injecting drug use. Questionnaire measures of disgust did not predict implicit disgust. While animal reminder disgust and injecting phobia were significantly correlated with each other, animal reminder disgust did not predict implicit disgust scores. Conclusions: On the basis of our findings, stigma toward PWID may not be a result of feelings of disgust toward injecting drug use. We discuss findings in the context of the underlying cortical processes supporting implicit and explicit representations of disgust. Future research should seek to investigate neurophysiological evidence for disgust to and stigmatization of injecting drug use and the potential role of domains of disgust in this.

Introduction

Stigma refers to biased attitudes and/or behaviors toward individuals belonging to a particular group, who are perceived to possess a characteristic that is contrary to a social norm (Link & Phelan, Citation2006; Stafford & Scott, Citation1986). People Who Use Drugs (PWUD) are one such group who are highly stigmatized (Sumnall et al., Citation2021a), although this is dependent upon the substances used and the characteristics of those who are using it (McElrath & McEvoy, Citation2001, Pennay & Measham, Citation2016). People who inject drugs (PWID) are particularly stigmatized, with studies reporting participants’ low levels of warmth, greater social distance, and preference for punitive responses toward PWID, including by non-injecting PWUD (e.g., Broady et al., Citation2020; Capitanio & Herek, Citation1999; Herek et al., Citation2003). Negative attitudes toward PWUD further increase social inequality and reinforce discrimination as they result in less public and policy support for PWUD, which can undermine high quality and evidence-based service provision (Andersen & Kessing, Citation2019; Lancaster et al., Citation2017). To further compound this, when feelings of stigma are internalized by PWUD, these internalized feelings can present barriers to seeking treatment, and are associated with increased health risk behaviors (Bayat et al., Citation2020; Biancarelli et al., Citation2019; Rivera et al., Citation2014).

In addition to socio-demographic characteristics (e.g., Broady et al., Citation2020; Lim et al., Citation2013), there are many psychological factors that contribute to stigmatizing attitudes and discrimination practices toward PWUD. From a psychosocial perspective, beliefs about the controllability of stigmatized behavioral attributes, media representations of PWUD (e.g. Atkinson et al., Citation2019; Atkinson & Sumnall, Citation2020), the popular use of pejorative labels such as “addict” and “junkie” (Ashford et al., Citation2019; Lancaster et al., Citation2017), and framing substance use as a failure of personal morality (MacCoun, Citation2013) have all been identified as factors which promote negative outgroup stereotypes and perpetuate public stigma toward PWUD. Conversely, presentation of substance use as a behavior stemming from Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE), for example, or describing structural barriers to accessing treatment have been associated with less stigmatizing attitudes toward PWUD (Kennedy-Hendricks et al., Citation2017; Sumnall et al., Citation2021a). While such studies help to explain external processes, which can influence stigmatization of an outgroup, they do not identify internal cognitive mechanisms for development and maintenance of stigma toward PWUD. Dehumanization, making subtle or blatant judgements about the humanness of an out-group relative to the in-group, was found to be one such cognitive mechanism that could promote social distancing from, and increased stigmatization of, people who use heroin (Sumnall et al., Citation2021b). Subtle and blatant dehumanization toward people who use heroin was relatively greater when compared to the general population, other stigmatized groups (e.g., people who are homeless, obese, people with serious mental illness) and people who use less stigmatized drugs such as cannabis (Sumnall et al., Citation2021b). Dehumanizing and stigmatizing attitudes toward PWUD may also reflect in-group strategies of moral disengagement, whereby dehumanizing of out-group members is associated with reduced empathy toward the outgroup (e.g., PWUD) and also increases in hostility and stigmatization (Boysen et al., Citation2020). Increased social distancing, and perceptions of dangerousness, and higher levels of support for discriminatory and aggressive policies targeting out-groups have all been observed following subtle dehumanization of other stigmatized groups (Fontesse et al., Citation2021; Kersbergen & Robinson, Citation2019; Martinez, Citation2014; Martinez et al., Citation2011).

Another sociomoral factor suggested to have a role in dehumanization and stigma is disgust, an emotion with distinctive physiological, behavioral and cognitive elements (Tybur et al., Citation2009). While disgust has been defined as a defensive mechanism that has evolved to protect against harm by promoting withdrawal from food contaminants (Vicario et al., Citation2017), it has also been observed in relation to non-food stimuli suggesting that it has a more wide-ranging function that extends to interpersonal and social interactions (Rozin et al., Citation1993; Citation2008). Disgust has been observed toward sexual acts (Haidt et al., Citation1994), moral judgements, physical and psychological threats (Tybur et al., Citation2009). Stigma and disgust have been found to be strongly related to each other. In one fMRI study, comparing activation to stigmatized faces (obesity, facial piercings, transsexual, and unattractive categories) and control faces, control faces were rated as significantly less disgusting than all other categories (Krendl et al., Citation2006). In another study, desire for increased social distance from PWID was demonstrated in a survey investigating perceptions of people who use opioids; only 10% of respondents rated opioid users as strong (compared to weak), 27% as deserving (compared to worthless) and only 15% would want an opioid user marrying into their family (McGinty et al., Citation2018). In a US general population sample, 70% of respondents agreed that PWID are “disgusting” and “a threat to society”, which seems to have a moral basis as 90% of respondents agreed that injecting drug use was “just plain wrong” (Capitanio & Herek, Citation1999). Moreover, while some facets of disgust may have an evolutionary basis, it is likely that stigma and disgust have a reciprocal relationship and in the context of PWUD, the development of disgust and prejudices toward different social groups is dictated by learned behaviors and socio-political influence (see Tyler, 2020). One facet of disgust, disgust propensity (a general tendency to respond with disgust to a particular situation i.e. how likely a person is to be disgusted, see e.g. Schienle et al., Citation2022), has also been found to be related to stigma toward homosexuality (Olatunji, Citation2008), obesity (Vartanian, Citation2010), and cancer patients (Pryor et al., Citation2004). Disgust sensitivity (the strength of a disgust response to a particular stimulus i.e. how strong a disgust response is, see e.g. Tybur et al., Citation2018) has been shown to predict avoidance behavior to anxiety provoking stimuli (Nicholson & Barnes-Holmes, Citation2012), and is related to a range of psychopathologies (e.g. eating disorders, separation anxiety). Disgust sensitivity can facilitate anxiety and stress brought on by external stimuli through its interaction with other human emotions such as guilt and shame (Davey, Citation2011), and in the present study we propose that these mechanisms interact to strengthen disgust toward PWUD.

The model of disgust described by Olatunji et al. (Citation2007) including 3 domains of disgust, is used in the present study. Core disgust is characterized by innate disgust responses such as food rejection after ingestion of offensive stimuli e.g., eating monkey meat; Animal Reminder disgust is characterized by disgust toward stimuli that remind us of our animal origins and mortality e.g., touching a dead body; and Contamination disgust is characterized by disgust to disease/illness spread by humans e.g., drinking from someone else’s cup. It is proposed that disgust elicitors related to death, aggression and sexuality promote disgust as they remind us of our animal nature i.e., that we are not that different to animals. This results in social distance from, and exclusion of, outgroup members for fear of contamination. In this way, holding stigmatizing or dehumanizing attitudes and disgust appear to have developed from sometimes maladaptive protective mechanisms designed to prevent contamination/harm of the self (Hodson & Costello, Citation2007), and would be expected to have a reciprocal relationship whereby an increase in one would promote an increase in the other. Moreover, disgust-inducing stimuli not only promote hypo-mentalisation (attributing fewer human characteristics to stimuli we find disgusting) which could increase dehumanizing attitudes toward such stimuli (Sherman & Haidt, Citation2011), but also guide moral evaluations of others (Schnall et al. Citation2008). The links between animalistic dehumanization and disgust have been demonstrated using a dehumanization implicit association task (IAT), where disgusted participants show the highest dehumanizing biases (between outgroup and animals) relative to non-disgusted participants (Buckels & Trapnell, Citation2013).

Disgust sensitivity has been investigated in the context of substance use beliefs (Oosterhoff & Shook, Citation2017) where sexual and moral disgust sensitivity were associated with greater support for drug laws and lower substance use. The latter was partially mediated by individual beliefs regarding substance-related harm suggesting that disgust to other PWUD could be mediated by the same mechanism (i.e. fear of contamination/harm via contagion). Moreover, PWUD have a higher likelihood of contracting and thus transmitting infectious disease (Harris et al., Citation2016; Kolla et al., Citation2020; Zibbell et al., Citation2018) and public health campaigns have capitalized on this by promoting fear and disgust in their presentation of PWUD and smokers (see Lupton, Citation2015 for discussion). In line with this, it may be that PWID are viewed with heightened disgust relative to PWUD due to the method of administration (Herek et al., Citation2003), and the concept of Blood Injection Injury (BII) phobia. BII phobia has a median onset age of 5.5 years (Bienvenu & Eaton, Citation1998), and is influenced by learned behaviors including classical conditioning (repeated pairing of needle and pain) and fear modeling (from e.g. parents) (see Merckelbach et al., Citation1996 for review). Interestingly, heightened disgust sensitivity has been shown to increase fainting responses in those with BII phobia, and disgust evoking images can increase fear reporting in experimental paradigms suggesting that fear and disgust are linked (see Woody & Teachman, Citation2000 for review). Campaigns aimed at preventing substance use have capitalized on these disgust associations, utilizing images of IDU and paraphernalia to emphasize the perceived threat (see Mold, Citation2021 for review), further strengthening the threat-relevance of such items. Thus, BII phobia could potentially influence perceptions of disgust related to images of injecting drug use.

In addition while it is clear that attitudes to PWUD play a role in disgust sensitivity, there are a number of personal characteristics (e.g. gender, age) which can affect a disgust response, both alone and in combination with other personal and situational factors. For example, disgust sensitivity is subject to gender differences, with women exhibiting consistently higher levels of disgust than men (see Al-Shawaf et al. Citation2018 for review), a highly replicable state and trait response (Tybur et al., Citation2009) with large effect sizes (Al-Shawaf et al., Citation2015). Disgust Sensitivity has been shown to demonstrate a constant decline across the life course (Curtis et al., Citation2004) with older participants exhibiting lower levels of DS. However, age has a less clear-cut role in attitudes toward PWUD with both younger (16-29) and older (60+) cohorts reporting more negative attitudes (Singleton, Citation2010), while evidence on gender differences in attitudes to PWUD is sparse. In addition to characteristics of the perceiver, characteristics of the individual being judged have also been shown to play a role in the allocation of blame, stigma and disgust. While gender of the person being judged seems to yield more equivocal results with men and women equally as likely to be perceived negatively due to their substance use (Meyers et al., Citation2021), research indicates that there are significant racial biases in perceptions of disgust (Liu et al., Citation2015) with racial biases indicating that black Americans are more likely to be arrested and imprisoned for drug related offenses despite white Americans using substances to a similar level (El-Sabawi & Oliva, Citation2022). Similarly, there are racial and ethnic disparities in sentencing in the UK, including for drug offenses (Veiga et al., Citation2023). Black people in some UK cities are disproportionately searched in relation to drug possession offenses and are more likely to be arrested on drugs charges (Eastwood et al., Citation2013). Prior research has investigated representations of minority ethnic groups in public understanding of drug harms (cf El-Sabawi & Oliva, Citation2022), and identified historical over-representation of young black males in media depictions of drug-related crime which may have effects on public perception of drug use (Cushion et al., Citation2010).

In summary, PWUD and in particular PWID are stigmatized and dehumanized; people report feelings of disgust toward PWUD, and disgust, dehumanization and holding stigmatizing attitudes appear to serve the self as protectors of physical, psychological and moral integrity. Despite the general body of research in disgust, stigma and health, and examinations of stigma toward PWUD, there are few studies investigating the link between disgust and stigma toward PWUD. The link between animalistic dehumanization and disgust observed in previous research suggests that stimuli related to injecting drug use would promote disgust, which in turn could promote social distance and stigmatization of PWID. However, as research on unconscious bias and prejudice demonstrates, these biases are not always available to conscious awareness, with explicit and implicit prejudice predicting different aspects of stigmatizing attitudes (Boniecki & Jacks, Citation2002). Implicit disgust is similar to implicit or unconscious bias. Moreover, different implicit aspects of disgust have been shown to relate to avoidance behaviors with implicit disgust propensity related to obsessing and washing concerns after contamination and implicit disgust sensitivity predicting avoidance behaviors (Nicholson & Barnes-Holmes, Citation2012). Measuring implicit disgust (with an IAT as used in the present study) allows us to assess these unconscious biases, and mitigate any conscious control over explicit cognitions in relation to the images. For example, people may declare that they do not find IDU disgusting relative to medical injecting (which could either be their conscious knowledge about their own cognitions, or socially desirable responding in an attempt not to appear prejudiced). The present study therefore sought to investigate implicit cognitive processes (i.e. those which we are not consciously aware of). We assessed implicit disgust to injecting drug use using a pictorial Implicit Association Task; based on previous research, we included demographic characteristics (age, gender), attitudes to PWUD, Injecting Phobia and core, contamination, animal reminder and moral disgust as predictors (N = 8) in our model. It was hypothesized that implicit disgust to injecting drug use would be predicted by attitudes to PWUD and disgust sensitivity.

Method

Design

A cross-sectional design, where participants completed an online computerized Implicit Association Task (IAT) and questionnaire.

Participants

A convenience sample was recruited from the UK general public (via advertised call for participants on the research team’s professional twitter accounts) and students (call for participants via email) at two universities in the North-West of England. Participants were eligible to take part if they were aged over 18 years and resided in the UK. Participants were excluded if they reported ever having received structured drug treatment for a substance use disorder, if they had no access to the internet, or if they did not possess an internet compatible device. To reduce demand characteristics and potential for bias, the study was advertised as “Attention to Emotive Images”. A priori power calculations were performed using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., Citation2007) indicating that to detect a medium effect size (f2 = 0.15) for predicting implicit disgust in a multiple linear regression (Fixed model, R2 increase; power 0.95; α = 0.05; 8 predictors), a minimum sample of 107 was required.

Overall, 182 experiment attempts were recorded on Inquisit web (version 6.5.1). Incomplete datasets (n = 19) were excluded. Participants who attempted the task on a mobile device (N = 37) were also excluded as the IAT used was not compatible with mobile devices. The final sample comprised 126 complete responses (46 male; mean age 25.03 ± 9.02). Data was collected between March and June 2021.

Materials

An abridged version of the Substance use inventory (Montgomery et al., Citation2005) was used to record self-reported substance use in the last year and lifetime in the sample.

Attitudes toward PWUD were assessed as per Sumnall et al. (Citation2021b) using 19 questions taken from Singleton’s (Citation2010) attitude to people who use drugs survey. Questions were scored on a five-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly Disagree to 5 = Strongly Agree) and assess attitudes toward people with a history of drug dependence (e.g., Parents should not let their children play with the children of someone with a history of drug dependence; People with a history of drug dependence are too often demonized in the media (reversed scored)). Higher total scores represent more negative attitudes. Cronbach’s α = 0.84, indicating a good level of internal consistency.

The Disgust Scale Revised (TDS-R; Olatunji et al., Citation2007) is a 25-item questionnaire assessing 3 types of disgust—Core disgust (12-items, characterized by core disgust responses such as food rejection after ingestion of offensive stimuli e.g. eating monkey meat), Animal Reminder disgust (8 items, characterized by disgust toward stimuli that remind us of our animal origins and mortality e.g. touching a dead body) and contamination disgust (5 items, characterized by disgust to disease/illness spread by humans e.g. drinking from someone else’s cup). TDS-R has been shown to be a valid and reliable measures of these three facets of disgust in previous research (see e.g. van Overveld et al., Citation2011). Items in section 1 of the questionnaire are scored 0/False − 1/True; items in section 2 are scored 0/not disgusting, 0.5/slightly disgusting, 1/very disgusting. Total scores for each subscale are calculated by summing response to all items within that scale after reverse scoring, and a total score summing all items can be used to assess disgust sensitivity. Core disgust (Cronbach’s α = 0.70) and Animal Reminder disgust (Cronbach’s α = 0.73) showed good levels of internal consistency in the present study as did the overall scale (Cronbach’s α = 0.75). However, contamination disgust demonstrated low levels of internal consistency and the results pertaining to this subscale should be treated with caution (Cronbach’s α= .45).

Moral disgust was assessed using the seven-item subscale of the Three Domains of Disgust Scale (TDDS), which assesses disgust that motivates the avoidance of social-norm violators (e.g. Forging someone’s signature on a legal document; Intentionally lying during a business transaction) (Tybur et al., Citation2009). Items were scored on a Likert scale (0 = Not disgusting at all, to 6 = Extremely disgusting) with higher scores representing greater disgust. Previous research suggests that while the scale has excellent reliability demonstrated by high internal consistency, it is not highly correlated with other domains of disgust and is more variable across different socio-political contexts (Olatunji et al., Citation2012). In the present study, Cronbach’s α = 0.86, indicating a good level of internal consistency.

The Injecting Phobia Scale-Short Form (IPS-SF Olatunji et al., Citation2010) is a 7-item questionnaire comprised of items from the Injecting Phobia Scale-Anxiety Contact Fear items (e.g., Giving a blood sample by having a finger pricked; Getting an intravenous injection). The scale has been shown to be a valid and reliable measure of injecting phobia with excellent construct validity and test-retest reliability (see e.g. Olatunji et al., Citation2010). Participants were asked to read each item and rate how much anxiety they would experience if they were in the situation, on a scale from 0 = no anxiety to 4 = most anxiety. Cronbach’s α = 0.91, indicating a high level of internal consistency.

Implicit Association Test (IAT): The IAT (Greenwald et al., Citation1998) assesses the strength of subconscious associations between different concepts and has been used in experimental and social psychology to measure and assess implicit bias across a number of domains (e.g., racial biases, gender biases (Maina et al., Citation2018; Wang-Jones et al., Citation2017). While the IAT has been criticized for having poor test-retest reliability and being influenced by external factors such as time of day, mood and wider cultural context (de Houwer, Citation2002), we opted to use the IAT based the fact it is flexible and can be adapted to test a variety of abstract, related or unrelated concepts and can test associations and strength of associations between concepts (Greenwald et al., Citation2020). The IAT involves participants having to allocate stimuli quickly and accurately into two affective attribute categories (e.g., pleasant vs. disgusting) and two target categories (e.g., injecting drugs vs. medical injecting) by using a left and right response key. The attribute and target categories were assigned to the response keys in two separate possible combinations. The IAT used in the present study used 20 images depicting injecting drug use (taken from media representations of injecting drug users and online image banks under CCBY licence) and 20 images of medical injecting (taken from online image resources under CCBY licence) to provide structurally similar images showing the same injecting paraphernalia and skin penetration as injecting drug use, but without the association with drugs (e.g. clinical settings vs street injection; sterile gloves; clear injection solutions vs brown heroin solutions). Initially, image banks were created by two researchers who were not involved in the present study. The images in each bank were rated for injecting drug use/medical injecting content by the research team and 20 images from each image bank with the highest scores were selected to be used as stimuli in the IAT. Stimuli utilized ethnically white PWID and medical injectees only, to control for potential implicit racial bias. Control pictures for pleasant vs. disgusting were taken from the Open Affective Standardized Image Set (OASIS—Kurdi et al., Citation2017). Images with high negative valence and high arousal (N = 20) were selected to represent disgust, and images of positive valence (N = 20) were selected to represent pleasant. The image bank can be found here.

The IAT consisted of seven blocks. In Blocks 1 and 2, participants practice categorizing the images as “pleasant” and “disgusting” and target stimuli into the categories “drugs” and “medicine”, using the response keys E (left) and I (right). All stimuli were presented twice in each block (N = 40 trials/block). In blocks 3 (N = 40 trials) and 4 (N = 80 trials), participants categorize stimuli belonging to one target category and one attribute category (e.g., drugs and pleasant) with one response key and stimuli belonging to the other target category or the other attribute category (e.g., medicine and disgusting) with the other response key. During block 5 (N = 80 trials), participants reversed the response option used in blocks 3 and 4 (e.g. if ‘drugs’ were previously sorted using the right response key, they were now sorted using the left response key). In blocks 6 (N = 40 trials) and 7 (N = 80 trials), participants performed the reversed combination of targets and attributes (e.g., drugs and unpleasant vs. medicine and pleasant. An IAT effect score (“d”) was calculated by computing the difference score for reaction times between the medicine/drug use categories and dividing this by the pooled SD of response times (Greenwald et al., Citation2003). IAT d scores were coded so that negative values indicated implicit disgust to IDU vs. medical injecting (RT for pleasant + medicine/disgust + IDU) - (RT for pleasant + IDU/disgust + medicine)).

Procedure

Participants completed a single online experiment and questionnaire hosted on Inquisit Web v6.5.1 (Millisecond Software LLC, Seattle, WA, USA), and this took approximately 20 min to complete. Participants completed the IAT followed by randomized blocks of questions on demographics (age, gender, education, ethnicity, employment); Attitudes to PWUD; substance use history (lifetime and last year use of a number of substances); TDS-R, Moral disgust and IPS-SF. At the end of the study, participants were presented with debriefing information summarizing the aims of the study and providing information about support services for substance use. Participants could also opt to enter their email address to be entered into a prize draw to win a store voucher. The study was approved by LJMU Research Ethics Committee.

Statistical analysis

Data were tested for normality using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests. Moral disgust, attitudes to PWUD and total disgust sensitivity were normally distributed (ps > .05). All other variables were significantly different from normal (p < .05), thus median and range are also reported for descriptive statistics. Hierarchical regression was used to assess the predictive power of individual factors on implicit disgust with implicit disgust to IDU was the outcome variable. It was decided that gender and age would be entered in block 1—both have been shown to be related to disgust sensitivity and removing variance due to demographic factors in model 1 would more clearly delineate the relative contributions of subsequent predictors; attitudes to PWUD were entered in block 2; Injecting phobia in block 3; finally, TDS-R and moral disgust scores we entered in block 4 to allow us to assess any unique variance in implicit disgust to IDU after removal of variance due to other predictors. Assumptions for regression were met, specifically, homoscedascity and linearity were not violated, and while the overall data were not normally distributed, inspection of residuals indicate that there were no extreme outliers in the analyses. There was limited evidence of multi-collinearity with Variance Inflation Factors <1.5 for all predictors.

Results

Participant characteristics (education, employment status, ethnicity, previous substance use) and descriptive statistics for the IAT and questionnaire measures are displayed in . The average d score for the IAT was negative indicating a stronger implicit association between implicit disgust and injecting drug use relative to medical injecting (with only 3 participants in the whole sample achieving positive scores indicating the opposite). A one sample Wilcoxon signed rank test showed that d scores differed significantly from zero W(125) = 50.00, p = .001.

Table 1. Participant characteristics & descriptive Statistics for IAT and questionnaire measures.

Correlations were performed to investigate the relationship between implicit disgust and attitudes to PWUD, injecting phobia and the four domains of disgust (animal reminder, moral disgust, contamination disgust and core disgust). The correlations are displayed in . Of note, injecting phobia was significantly correlated with all implicit and explicit disgust variables, with the exception of contamination disgust, while contrary to expectations, implicit disgust to injecting drug use was only correlated with injecting phobia. In line with previous research on demographic factors, gender was significantly positively related to core disgust and overall disgust sensitivity indicating females exhibited higher levels relative to males. Age was significantly negatively related to injecting phobia, animal reminder disgust and overall disgust sensitivity indicating lower scores with increasing age. Moral disgust was significantly positively related to age indicating higher moral disgust with increasing age.

Table 2. Correlations between implicit disgust, attitudes and TDS-R measures.

Hierarchical multiple linear regression was used to model the predictive effects of attitudes, injecting phobia and domains of disgust on implicit disgust to injecting drug use. Age and gender were entered in Step 1, attitudes to PWUD added in Step 2, injecting phobia added in Step 3, and TDS-R and moral disgust scores in Step 4. Model statistics are displayed in . Model 1 accounted for a non-significant 0.1% of the variance in implicit disgust. Model 2 accounted for an additional non-significant 0.2% of the variance in implicit disgust. Addition of injecting phobia in Model 3 resulted in a significant R2 change, with Model 3 accounting for 6% of the variance in implicit disgust (an increase of 5.9%) F(1,112) = 7.14, p = .009; injecting phobia was a significant individual predictor t(116) = 2.67, p = .009. Addition of the questionnaire measures of disgust in Step 4 accounted for an additional 3.2% of the variance in implicit disgust, however the overall model was non-significant (F(4,108) = .96, p = .43)). Injecting phobia was the only significant individual predictor t(116) = 2.32, p = .02.

Table 3. Coefficients and t tests for individual predictors in hierarchical multiple regression analysis.

Discussion

The present study found no evidence for the relationship between implicit disgust to injecting drug use and sensitivity to animal reminder, core, contamination and moral disgust. Similarly, attitudes to PWUD and demographic variables such as age and gender were not predictors of implicit disgust. Injecting phobia emerged as the only significant predictor of implicit disgust toward injecting drug use. Our findings suggest that stigma toward PWID reported in other studies may not be a result of disgust toward injecting drug use.

While this was not in line with our predictions, given that a primary function of disgust is believed to be to protect from psychological or physical contamination (Matchett & Davey, Citation1991; Rozin et al., Citation2008; Woody & Teachman, Citation2000), the relationship with injecting phobia was not surprising. However, injecting phobia was positively correlated with the IAT score. While we did not make a specific prediction in relation to the direction of this relationship, it seems reasonable to expect that higher levels of injecting phobia would be related to higher levels of disgust to injecting drug use (i.e., reflected by an inverse relationship in the present study), due to factors such as increased (perceived) risk of contamination and disease from injecting drug use relative to medical injecting (Capitanio & Herek, Citation1999; Lloyd, Citation2013). It is unclear why this was not the direction of the relationship in the present study, but we provide a tentative explanation below. Dehumanization and stigmatization of outgroups such as PWID results in increased social distancing and perceptions of dangerousness and fear (Martinez, Citation2014; Martinez et al., Citation2011; Sumnall et al., Citation2021b). The interruption of mentalisation processes, which are used to assign human characteristics to disgust inducing stimuli and stigmatized groups, leads to acceptance of stigmatizing attitudes and behaviors toward a particular group of people (Fiske et al., Citation2004), further distancing the outgroup from the self (Kilian et al. Citation2021; Tapias et al., Citation2007). This social distancing and avoidance of outgroups has been seen in laboratory research on emotion perception (Reicher et al., Citation2016), toward individuals with mental health conditions (Dawydiak et al., Citation2020), and other stigmatized groups (Amodio, Citation2014). In the present study, the images of injecting drug use may have been perceived to be so far from the self, and belonging to the outgroup, that they were not related to participants’ injecting phobia. Essentially, the context of the IDU images may not have been relatable or threat-relevant for participants. Conversely, the content of the medical injecting images were relatable, as most individuals could perceive receiving a medical injection (this study was undertaken during the COVID-19 pandemic, and the majority of the UK population had received a vaccination), and this resulted in higher injecting phobia being related to a higher d score (i.e. less implicit disgust to IDU relative to medical injecting). Disgust has been observed as the dominant response to threat-relevant stimuli (Sawchuk et al., Citation2002; Tolin et al., Citation1997), and in the present study we propose that medical injecting was more threat-relevant for our non-injecting drug use participants than the images of IDU. Future research should aim to investigate implicit disgust to IDU in high and low injecting phobia groups.

We also found that animal reminder disgust was highly positively correlated with injecting phobia. This is supportive of previous research where de Jong & Merckelbach (Citation1998) found that heightened Blood Injection Injury phobia was more highly correlated with animal reminder disgust than core disgust. In addition, different types of disgust sensitivity have been shown to predict medical students’ interest in medical careers with varying procedural intensity (Consedine et al., Citation2013); greater animal reminder disgust is associated with lower interest in emergency medicine while greater core disgust predicts lower interest in gynaecology/obstetrics. Moreover, disgust plays a key role in the development and maintenance of anxiety-related disorders such as spider phobia (Matchett & Davey, Citation1991) and Blood Injection Injury phobia (Olatunji et al., Citation2005). In line with this, individuals with heightened levels of anxiety have been shown to report significantly greater disgust sensitivity (Koch et al., Citation2002; Olatunji, Citation2009; Olatunji et al., Citation2007b). While these studies are supportive of the correlation between animal reminder disgust and injecting phobia, mediation analysis to test the relationship between animal reminder disgust, injecting phobia and implicit disgust showed no mediation.

When we consider the implicit nature of the IAT versus the explicit nature of the attitude scales and questionnaires used in the present study, the results are not as divergent from what might be expected. Initial automatic fear responses and moral judgements are rapid and involuntary, involving recruitment of the amygdala to detect threat and initiate the fight or flight response if required (Finnell, Citation2018). Amygdala activation has been observed in IATs of implicit racial bias (Phelps et al., Citation2000; Richeson et al. Citation2008) and implicit tasks involving presentation of dehumanized social groups such as PWUD and individuals who are homeless (Harris & Fiske, Citation2006; Krendl et al. Citation2012), corroborating its role in acute reactions to anxiety-provoking stimuli. However, after this initial amygdala activation, higher activity has been observed in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the medial and lateral prefrontal cortex, especially when required to moderate negative affect toward the presented images (Krendl et al. Citation2012). Taken together, these findings suggest that activation of the ACC allows an individual to identify the need for cognitive control in the task at hand, while areas of the prefrontal cortex allow an individual to regulate the unwanted prejudicial thoughts (Finnell, Citation2018). Requiring participants to deliberately control their objectivity in assessments of others’ distress has been shown to activate similar areas (Bruneau et al., Citation2015). Thus, the lack of relationship between implicit and explicit measures of disgust in the present study could be explained by the differential requirements of the tasks (immediate fear response vs. more rational cognitive control). A meta-analysis comparing IAT and explicit self-report measures (Hofmann et al. Citation2005) also found that motivational bias when completing explicit reports (e.g., not wanting to appear to hold stigmatizing views) and meta-cognitive deficits (which could either result in a lack of introspection or a failure in memory) can both affect the integrity of the relationship between the IAT and explicit measures of bias. Consequently, future research should seek to include implicit and explicit measures of disgust toward injecting drug use, and assess task related changes in the amygdala, ACC and prefrontal cortex, in addition to facial EMG to investigate the role of fear and disgust related micro expressions in implicit disgust (de Jong et al., Citation2002).

There were a number of limitations to the present study. Firstly, the IAT is a controversial measure and it has been argued that responding might reflect knowledge of cultural views rather than personal salience/preference (Ottaway et al., Citation2001; Rothermund & Wentura, Citation2004). Moreover, while the IAT has been used to predict behavior in a range of situations (e.g., employment outcomes, mental health risk) it has better predictive validity in some populations (e.g., those at risk of suicide and Self-Harm; Nock et al. Citation2010) than others, and the results of the present study cannot indicate actual behavior toward PWID based on the IAT results. Test-retest reliability of the IAT has been to shown to be relatively weak (Nosek et al., Citation2005) with previous studies demonstrating that IAT score may be based on a combination of state and trait characteristics. In light of this, future research investigating disgust to IDU should seek to include additional measures of implicit bias such as the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP; Barnes-Holmes et al., Citation2006) which is a newer measure, based on the IAT, with good construct validity (Vahey et al., Citation2015). It is also noteworthy that significant correlations between the moral disgust scale and animal reminder disgust and injecting phobia were negative indicating that higher levels of moral disgust were associated with lower levels of injecting phobia and animal reminder disgust, While we did not make a specific prediction about the direction of this relationship, it is likely that the direction of these correlations reflects the validity of the moral disgust subscale as a measure of disgust (see Olatunji et al., Citation2012 for discussion). Study participants were primarily drawn from university students, hence findings are not generalizable. We chose the DS-R to assess disgust because of our specific interests in animal reminder disgust and its relation to IDU. However, it is possible that as the DS-R contains descriptions of certain situations that an individual might find disgusting, it is dependent on the context of those statements/descriptions (Olatunji et al., Citation2007). Moreover, of particular note for the present study, Tybur et al. (Citation2009) have contended that there is empirical support for a distinct domain of animal reminder disgust and suggest that the domains are not necessarily conceptually separable. They also note that as TDS-R excludes moral disgust, it is not a comprehensive measure of disgust sensitivity. We included a separate measure of moral disgust from the TDDS in the present study to provide us with a more comprehensive assessment of facets of disgust. To confirm the relationship between facets of disgust and stigma toward IDU, future research should seek to further clarify the relationship between the different domains of disgust, physiological and neural response to substance related disgust elicitors and how such stimuli could develop and maintain stigmatization of PWUD. The images of medical injecting and IDU in the present study were also ethnically White to try and avoid the concomitant effects of implicit or explicit racial bias. While PWID are stigmatized as a group, in the USA Black people and people of color are more likely to be imprisoned for substance-related crimes (El-Sabawi & Oliva, Citation2022), while similar racial and ethnic disparities are observed in UK sentencing (Veiga et al., Citation2023) and arrest statistics (Eastwood et al., Citation2013). Thus it is possible that there are intersections between racism and substance related stigma that could have affected perceptions of the white injectees in our study. However, it is difficult to fully realize the link between these concepts in the scope of the present study; analyses of media representations of ‘problematic’ drug use in the UK have not tended to include analysis of ethnicity, and have focused on the representation of images of ‘polluted’ and ‘contaminated’ bodies (Ayres & Jewkes, Citation2012), the stigma of substance and welfare dependencies (Alexandrescu, Citation2020) and sympathetic representations of white middle class female decedents in drug-related deaths (Forsyth, Citation2001). As this study was conducted in the UK, focussing solely on White injectees, this limits the generalizability of the findings. Future research should replicate these findings using ethnically diverse participants and images, and seek to contextualize the language used when categorizing images (e.g., drug vs. medicine) in current cultural, socio-political and geographical environments. Finally, as data was collected during the Covid-19 pandemic, there may have been higher levels of disgust sensitivity, in particular contamination disgust, which may have affected our results.

In conclusion, the present study found no relationship between explicit assessments of disgust sensitivity and implicit disgust to injecting drug use. Injecting phobia was related to disgust to medical injecting over disgust to injecting drug use. Whilst the current findings suggest that implicit disgust may not underlie stigma toward PWID, future research should seek to clarify the role of implicit versus explicit cognitions in stigmatization processes.

Disclosure statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Data availability statement

Data will be made available on the institutional data repository site after acceptance.

Additional information

Funding

The author(s) reported there is no funding associated with the work featured in this article.

References

  • Alexandrescu, L. (2020). Streets of the "spice zombies": Dependence and poverty stigma in times of austerity. Crime, Media, Culture: An International Journal, 16(1), 97–113. https://doi.org/10.1177/1741659019835274
  • Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2018). Sex differences in disgust: Why are women more easily disgusted than men? Emotion Review, 10(2), 149–160. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073917709940
  • Al-Shawaf, L., Lewis, D. M., & Buss, D. M. (2015). Disgust and mating strategy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 199–205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.11.003
  • Amodio, D. M. (2014). The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 15(10), 670–682. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3800
  • Andersen, D., & Kessing, M. L. (2019). Stigma, problem drug use, and welfare state encounters: Changing contours of stigmatization in the era of social investment. Addiction Research & Theory, 27(4), 277–284. https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2018.1508568
  • Ashford, R. D., Brown, A. M., McDaniel, J., & Curtis, B. (2019). Biased labels: An experimental study of language and stigma among individuals in recovery and health professionals. Substance Use & Misuse, 54(8), 1376–1384. https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2019.1581221
  • Atkinson, A. M., McAuley, A., Trayner, K., & Sumnall, H. R. (2019). ‘We are still obsessed by this idea of abstinence’: A critical analysis of UK news media representations of proposals to introduce Drug Consumption Rooms in Glasgow, UK. The International Journal on Drug Policy, 68, 62–74. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2019.03.010
  • Atkinson, A. M., & Sumnall, H. R. (2020). Neo-liberal discourse of substance use in the UK reality TV show, The Jeremy Kyle Show. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 27(1), 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687637.2018.1498456
  • Ayres, T. C., & Jewkes, Y. (2012). The haunting spectacle of crystal meth: A media-created mythology? Crime, Media, Culture: An International Journal, 8(3), 315–332. https://doi.org/10.1177/1741659012443234
  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32(7), 169–177.
  • Bayat, A. H., Mohammadi, R., Moradi-Joo, M., Bayani, A., Ahounbar, E., Higgs, P., Hemmat, M., Haghgoo, A., & Armoon, B. (2020). HIV and drug related stigma and risk-taking behaviors among people who inject drugs: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Addictive Diseases, 38(1), 71–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/10550887.2020.1718264
  • Biancarelli, D. L., Biello, K. B., Childs, E., Drainoni, M., Salhaney, P., Edeza, A., Mimiaga, M. J., Saitz, R., & Bazzi, A. R. (2019). Strategies used by people who inject drugs to avoid stigma in healthcare settings. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 198, 80–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.01.037
  • Bienvenu, O. J., & Eaton, W. W. (1998). The epidemiology of blood-injection-injury phobia. Psychological Medicine, 28(5), 1129–1136. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291798007144
  • Boniecki, K. A., & Jacks, J. Z. (2002). The elusive relationship between measures of implicit and explicit prejudice. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 26, 1–14.
  • Boysen, G. A., Isaacs, R. A., Tretter, L., & Markowski, S. (2020). Evidence for blatant dehumanization of mental illness and its relation to stigma. The Journal of Social Psychology, 160(3), 346–356. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2019.1671301
  • Broady, T. R., Brener, L., Cama, E., Hopwood, M., & Treloar, C. (2020). Stigmatising attitudes towards people who inject drugs, and people living with blood borne viruses or sexually transmissible infections in a representative sample of the Australian population. PloS One, 15(4), e0232218. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0232218
  • Bruneau, E. G., Jacoby, N., & Saxe, R. (2015). Empathic control through coordinated interaction of amygdala, theory of mind and extended pain matrix brain regions. NeuroImage, 114, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.04.034
  • Buckels, E. E., & Trapnell, P. D. (2013). Disgust facilitates outgroup dehumanization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 16(6), 771–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430212471738
  • Capitanio, J. P., & Herek, G. M. (1999). AIDS-related stigma and attitudes toward injecting drug users among Black and White Americans. American Behavioral Scientist, 42(7), 1148–1161. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764299042007007
  • Consedine, N. S., Yu, T. C., & Windsor, J. A. (2013). Nursing, pharmacy, or medicine? Disgust sensitivity predicts career interest among trainee health professionals. Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 18(5), 997–1008. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-012-9439-z
  • Cushion, S., Moore, K. & Jewell, J. (2010). Media representations of black young men and boys Report of the REACH media monitoring project. Department for Communities and Local Government.
  • Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl 4), S131–S133. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2003.0144
  • Davey, G. C. (2011). Disgust: The disease-avoidance emotion and its dysfunctions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3453–3465. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0039
  • Dawydiak, E. J., Stafford, H. E., Stevenson, J. L., & Jones, B. C. (2020). Pathogen disgust predicts stigmatization of individuals with mental health conditions. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 6(1), 60–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40806-019-00208-x
  • De Houwer, J. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33(2), 115–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7916(02)00024-1
  • de Jong, P. J., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Blood-injection-injury phobia and fear of spiders: Domain specific individual differences in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 24(2), 153–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00178-5
  • de Jong, P. J., Peters, M., & Vanderhallen, I. (2002). Disgust and disgust sensitivity in spider phobia: Facial EMG in response to spider and oral disgust imagery. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(5), 477–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00167-6
  • Eastwood, N., Shiner, M., & Bear, D. (2013). The numbers in black and white: Ethnic disparities in the policing and prosecution of drug offences in England and Wales. Release Publications.
  • El-Sabawi, T., & Oliva, J. (2022). The Influence of White Exceptionalism on Drug War Discourse. 94 Temple Law Review, 649.
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03193146
  • Finnell, D. S. (2018). A call to action: Managing the neural pathway of disgust, bias, prejudice, and discrimination that fuels stigma. Substance Abuse, 39(4), 399–403. https://doi.org/10.1080/08897077.2019.1576091
  • Fiske, S., Harris, L., & Cuddy, A. (2004). Social psychology. Why ordinary people torture enemy prisoners. Science (New York, N.Y.), 306(5701), 1482–1483. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1103788
  • Fontesse, S., Rimez, X., & Maurage, P. (2021). Stigmatization and dehumanization perceptions towards psychiatric patients among nurses: A path-analysis approach. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 35(2), 153–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apnu.2020.12.005
  • Forsyth, A. J. (2001). Distorted? a quantitative exploration of drug fatality reports in the popular press. International Journal of Drug Policy, 12(5-6), 435–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0955-3959(01)00092-5
  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 1464–1480. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.74.6.1464
  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197
  • Greenwald, A. G., Brendl, M., Cai, H., Cvencek, D., Dovidio, J., Friese, M., et al. (2020). The Implicit Association Test at age 20: What is known and what is not known about implicit bias.
  • Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7
  • Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847–853. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x
  • Harris, A. M., Iqbal, K., Schillie, S., Britton, J., Kainer, M. A., Tressler, S., & Vellozzi, C. (2016). Increases in acute hepatitis B virus infections – Kentucky, Tennessee, and West Virginia, 2006-2013. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 65(3), 47–50. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.mm6503a2
  • Herek, G. M., Capitanio, J. P., & Widaman, K. F. (2003). Stigma, social risk, and health policy: Public attitudes toward HIV surveillance policies and the social construction of illness. Health Psychology : official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 22(5), 533–540. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.5.533
  • Hodson, G., & Costello, K. (2007). Interpersonal disgust, ideological orientations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes. Psychological Science, 18(8), 691–698. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01962.x
  • Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A metaanalysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 1369–1385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205275613
  • Kennedy-Hendricks, A., Barry, C. L., Gollust, S. E., Ensminger, M. E., Chisolm, M. S., & McGinty, E. E. (2017). Social stigma toward persons with prescription opioid use disorder: Associations with public support for punitive and public health–oriented policies. Psychiatric Services (Washington, D.C.), 68(5), 462–469. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201600056
  • Kersbergen, I., & Robinson, E. (2019). Blatant dehumanization of people with obesity. Obesity (Silver Spring, Md.), 27(6), 1005–1012. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22460
  • Kilian, C., Manthey, J., Carr, S., Hanschmidt, F., Rehm, J., Speerforck, S. and Schomerus, G. (2021). Stigmatization of people with alcohol use disorders: An updated systematic review of population studies. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 45(5), 899–911. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.14598
  • Koch, M. D., O’Neill, H. K., Sawchuk, C. N., & Connolly, K. (2002). Domain-specific and generalized disgust sensitivity in blood-injection-injury phobia: The application of behavioral approach/avoidance tasks. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(5), 511–527. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00170-6
  • Kolla, B. P., Oesterle, T., Gold, M., Southwick, F., & Rummans, T. (2020). Infectious diseases occurring in the context of substance use disorders: A concise review. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 411, 116719. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jns.2020.116719
  • Krendl, A. C., Kensinger, E. A., & Ambady, N. (2012). How does the brain regulate negative bias to stigma? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 715–726. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr046
  • Krendl, A. C., Macrae, C. N., Kelley, W. M., Fugelsang, J. A., & Heatherton, T. F. (2006). The good, the bad, and the ugly: An fMRI investigation of the functional anatomic correlates of stigma. Social Neuroscience, 1(1), 5–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910600670579
  • Kurdi, B., Lozano, S., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Introducing the open affective standardized image set (OASIS). Behavior Research Methods, 49(2), 457–470. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0715-3
  • Lancaster, K., Seear, K., & Ritter, A. (2017). Reducing stigma and discrimination for people experiencing problematic alcohol and other drug use. University of New South Wales.
  • Lim, T., Zelaya, C., Latkin, C., Quan, V. M., Frangakis, C., Ha, T. V., Minh, N. L., & Go, V. (2013). Individual-level socioeconomic status and community-level inequality as determinants of stigma towards persons living with HIV who inject drugs in Thai Nguyen, Vietnam. Journal of the International AIDS Society, 16(3 Suppl 2), 18637. https://doi.org/10.7448/IAS.16.3.18637
  • Link, B. G., & Phelan, J. C. (2006). Stigma and its public health implications. The Lancet, 367(9509), 528–529. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68184-1
  • Liu, Y., Lin, W., Xu, P., Zhang, D., & Luo, Y. (2015). Neural basis of disgust perception in racial prejudice. Human Brain Mapping, 36(12), 5275–5286. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23010
  • Lloyd, C. (2013). The stigmatization of problem drug users: A narrative literature review. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 20(2), 85–95. https://doi.org/10.3109/09687637.2012.743506
  • Lupton, D. (2015). The pedagogy of disgust: the ethical, moral and political implications of using disgust in public health campaigns. Critical Public Health, 25(1), 4–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/09581596.2014.885115
  • Maina, I. W., Belton, T. D., Ginzberg, S., Singh, A., & Johnson, T. J. (2018). A decade of studying implicit racial/ethnic bias in healthcare providers using the implicit association test. Social Science & Medicine (1982), 199, 219–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.05.009
  • Martinez, A. G. (2014). When “they” become “I”: Ascribing humanity to mental illness influences treatment-seeking for mental/behavioral health conditions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33(2), 187–206. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.2.187
  • Martinez, A. G., Piff, P. K., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2011). The power of a label: mental illness diagnoses, ascribed humanity, and social rejection. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 30(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2011.30.1.1
  • Matchett, G., & Davey, G. C. L. (1991). A test of the disease avoidance model of animal phobias. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 29(1), 91–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(09)80011-9
  • MacCoun, R. J. (2013). Moral outrage and opposition to harm reduction. Criminal Law and Philosophy, 7(1), 83–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11572-012-9154-0
  • McElrath, K., & McEvoy, K. (2001). Heroin as evil: Ecstasy users’ perceptions about heroin. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 8(2), 177–189.
  • McGinty, E., Barry, C. L., Stone, E. M., Niederdeppe, J., Kennedy-Hendricks, A., Linden, S., & Sherman, S. G. (2018). Public support for safe consumption sites and syringe services programs to combat the opioid epidemic. Preventive Medicine, 111, 73–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.02.026
  • Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P. J., Muris, P., & van den Hout, M. A. (1996). The etiology of specific phobias: A review. Clinical Psychology Review, 16(4), 337–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(96)00014-1
  • Meyers, S. A., Earnshaw, V. A., D’Ambrosio, B., Courchesne, N., Werb, D., & Smith, L. R. (2021). The intersection of gender and drug use-related stigma: A mixed methods systematic review and synthesis of the literature. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 223, 108706. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.108706
  • Montgomery, C., Fisk, J. E., Newcombe, R., & Murphy, P. N. (2005). The differential effects of ecstasy/polydrug use on executive components: shifting, inhibition, updating and access to semantic memory. Psychopharmacology, 182(2), 262–276. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-005-0065-9
  • Mold, A. (2021). Just say know: Drug education and its publics in 1980s Britain. The International Journal on Drug Policy, 88, 103029. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugpo.2020.103029
  • Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). Developing an implicit measure of disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Examining the role of implicit disgust propensity and sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(3), 922–930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.02.001
  • Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: implicit cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21(4), 511–517. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610364762
  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 166–180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271418
  • Olatunji, B. O., Lohr, J. M., Sawchuk, C. N., & Westendorf, D. H. (2005). Using facial expressions as CSs and fearsome and disgusting pictures as UCSs: Affective responding and evaluative learning of fear and disgust in blood-injection-injury phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19(5), 539–555. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.05.002
  • Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., & Elwood, L. S. (2007). The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281–297. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.19.3.281
  • Olatunji, B. O., Smits, J. A., Connolly, K. M., Willems, J., & Lohr, J. M. (2007b). Examination of the rate of decline in fear and disgust during exposure to threat-relevant stimuli in blood-injection-injury phobia. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(3), 445–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.05.001
  • Olatunji, B. O. (2008). Disgust, scrupulosity and conservative attitudes about sex: Evidence for a mediational model of homophobia. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(5), 1364–1369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.001
  • Olatunji, B. O., Sawchuk, C. N., Moretz, M. W., David, B., Armstrong, T., & Ciesielski, B. G. (2010). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the Injection Phobia Scale-Anxiety. Psychological Assessment, 22(1), 167–179. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018125
  • Olatunji, B. O. (2009). Incremental specificity of disgust propensity and sensitivity in the prediction of health anxiety dimensions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40(2), 230–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.10.003
  • Olatunji, B. O., Adams, T., Ciesielski, B., David, B., Sarawgi, S., & Broman-Fulks, J. (2012). The three domains of disgust scale: Factor structure, psychometric properties, and conceptual limitations. Assessment, 19(2), 205–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111432881
  • Oosterhoff, B., & Shook, N. J. (2017). From drug laws to recreational substance use: The adaptationist role of disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 544–553. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.015
  • Ottaway, S. A., Hayden, D. C., & Oakes, M. A. (2001). Implicit attitudes and racism: Effects of word familiarity and frequency on the implicit association test. Social Cognition, 19(2), 97–144. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.2.97.20706
  • Pennay, A. M., & Measham, F. C. (2016). The normalisation thesis – 20 years later. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 23(3), 187–189. https://doi.org/10.3109/09687637.2016.1173649
  • Phelps, E. A., O’Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 729–738. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562552
  • Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., Yeadon, C., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (2004). A dual-process model of reactions to perceived stigma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(4), 436–452. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.436
  • Reicher, S. D., Templeton, A., Neville, F., Ferrari, L., & Drury, J. (2016). Core disgust is attenuated by ingroup relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(10), 2631–2635. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1517027113
  • Richeson J. A., Todd A. R., Trawalter S., Baird A. A. (2008). Eye-gaze direction modulates race-related amygdala activity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 11, 233–246. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207088040
  • Rivera, A. V., DeCuir, J., Crawford, N. D., Amesty, S., & Lewis, C. F. (2014). Internalized stigma and sterile syringe use among people who inject drugs in New York City, 2010–2012. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 144, 259–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.09.778
  • Rothermund, K., & Wentura, D. (2004). Underlying processes in the Implicit Association Test (IAT): Dissociating salience from associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(2), 139–165. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.139
  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575–594). Guilford Press.
  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757–776). Guilford Press.
  • Sawchuk, C. N., Meunier, S. A., Lohr, J. M., & Westendorf, D. H. (2002). Fear, disgust, and information processing in specific phobia: The application of signal detection theory. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(5), 495–510. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0887-6185(02)00168-8
  • Schienle, A., Zorjan, S., & Wabnegger, A. (2022). A brief measure of disgust propensity. Current Psychology, 41(6), 3687–3693. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00883-1
  • Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096–1109. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208317771
  • Sherman, G. D., & Haidt, J. (2011). Cuteness and DISGUST: The humanizing and dehumanizing effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 3(3), 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911402396
  • Singleton, N. (2010). Attitudes to drug dependence. Results form a survey of people living in private households in the UK. UK Drug Policy Commission.
  • Stafford, M., & Scott, R. (1986). Stigma deviance and social control: Some conceptual issues. In S. C. Ainlay, G. Becker, & L. M. Coleman (Eds.), The dilemma of difference. Plenum.
  • Sumnall, H. R., Hamilton, I., Atkinson, A. M., Montgomery, C., & Gage, S. H. (2021a). Representation of adverse childhood experiences is associated with lower public stigma towards people who use drugs: An exploratory experimental study. Drugs: Education, Prevention and Policy, 28(3), 227–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/09687637.2020.1820450
  • Sumnall, H., Atkinson, A., Gage, S., Hamilton, I., & Montgomery, C. (2021b). Less than human: Dehumanisation of people who use heroin. Health Education, 121(6), 649–669. https://doi.org/10.1108/HE-07-2021-0099
  • Tapias, M. P., Glaser, J., Keltner, D., Vasquez, K., & Wickens, T. (2007). Emotions and prejudice: Specific emotions towards outgroups. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(1), 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430207071338
  • Tolin, D. F., Lohr, J. M., Sawchuk, C. N., & Lee, T. C. (1997). Disgust and disgust sensitivity in blood-injection-injury and spider phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35(10), 949–953. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00048-x
  • Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015474
  • Tybur, J. M., Çınar, C., Karinen, A. K., & Perone, P. (2018). Why do people vary in disgust? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B373 20170204 20170204 http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0204.
  • Vahey, N. A., Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). A meta-analysis of criterion effects for the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) in the clinical domain. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 59–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.004
  • van Overveld, M., de Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., & Schouten, E. (2011). The disgust scale-R: A valid and reliable index to investigate separate disgust domains? Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 325–330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.023
  • Vartanian, L. R. (2010). Disgust and perceived control in attitudes toward obese people. International Journal of Obesity (2005), 34(8), 1302–1307. https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2010.45
  • Veiga, A., Pina-Sánchez, J., & Lewis, S. (2023). Racial and ethnic disparities in sentencing: What do we know, and where should we go? The Howard Journal of Crime and Justice, 62(2), 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1111/hojo.12496
  • Vicario, C. M., Rafal, R. D., Martino, D., & Avenanti, A. (2017). Core, social and moral disgust are bounded: A review on behavioral and neural bases of repugnance in clinical disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 185–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.008
  • Wang-Jones, T. “. S., Alhassoon, O. M., Hattrup, K., Ferdman, B. M., & Lowman, R. L. (2017). Development of gender identity implicit association tests to assess attitudes toward transmen and transwomen. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 4(2), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000218
  • Woody, S. R., & Teachman, B. A. (2000). Intersection of disgust and fear: Normative and pathological views. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 7(3), 291–311. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.7.3.291
  • Zibbell, J. E., Asher, A. K., Patel, R. C., Kupronis, B., Iqbal, K., Ward, J. W., & Holtzman, D. (2018). Increases in acute hepatitis C virus infection related to a growing opioid epidemic and associated injection drug use, United States, 2004 to 2014. American Journal of Public Health, 108(2), 175–181. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2017.304132