13,752
Views
26
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Reading and Oral Vocabulary Development in Early Adolescence

ORCID Icon, , , &

ABSTRACT

International assessments show that 20% of adolescents cannot read simple texts with understanding. Despite this, research has focused on early reading in childhood and skilled reading in adulthood, neglecting reading development during adolescence. We report a longitudinal study assessing reading and vocabulary development at 12, 13 and 14 years in a sample of 210 adolescents who were unselected for ability. Word reading accuracy, word reading fluency, reading comprehension, receptive vocabulary and expressive vocabulary were assessed using standardized assessments. Latent variable models showed consistent rank order amongst individuals (high stability), significant progress over time, and evidence that achievement gaps between the least and most able adolescents were narrowing. Oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension were best conceptualized as indices of a common language construct. Low levels of reading proficiency were also observed in a substantial proportion of this sample, underlining the importance of providing ongoing reading and language support during adolescence.

Despite a widespread assumption that children can read when they leave primary education, international assessments show that 20% of adolescents are not able to read simple texts accurately and with understanding (Jerrim & Shure, Citation2016). These pupils will struggle to read independently, limiting access to the curriculum and hampering educational progress. Yet, very little is known about reading difficulties and reading development in adolescence, with research and theory focusing on reading in childhood and adulthood. Particularly lacking are longitudinal studies of adolescent reading: to our knowledge only one study has tracked reading within adolescence (see Francis, Shaywitz, Stuebing, Shaywitz, & Fletcher, Citation1996). We collected longitudinal data on word reading and reading comprehension at ages 12, 13 and 14 years. Spoken vocabulary knowledge was also assessed, as it is crucial for successful reading comprehension.

Reading development in adolescence

Beginning readers must learn how letters map onto sounds and then to read words and sentences accurately and efficiently. Once accurate and efficient reading is in place, a reader can allocate resources to reading comprehension, which is underpinned by spoken language comprehension (e.g., Gough & Tunmer, Citation1986). By late childhood, most children can read a range of texts accurately, efficiently and with comprehension, so it might be tempting to assume that little reading development takes place after this point. However, we know that the brain continues to develop during adolescence (Blakemore, Citation2018), and this includes the brain regions involved in reading (e.g., Ben-Shachar, Dougherty, Deutsch, & Wandell, Citation2011). Longitudinal studies of reading development in adolescence are scarce and existing studies have measured adolescent reading just once (e.g., Duff, Tomblin, & Catts, Citation2015; Eklund, Torppa, Aro, Leppänen, & Lyytinen, Citation2015; Landerl & Wimmer, Citation2008; Reynolds & Turek, Citation2012). However, there is evidence that reading abilities grow within adolescence. Francis et al. (Citation1996) observed growth in a reading composite until approximately 15 years, though growth slowed across childhood (see also, Lervåg, Melby-Lervåg, & Hulme, Citation2018; Quinn, Wagner, Petscher, & Lopez, Citation2015) and adolescence.

Tracking growth has been the focus of most longitudinal research but Bornstein, Putnick, and Esposito (Citation2017) highlighted another developmental process: stability, the rank order of individuals within the distribution. Bornstein, Hahn, Putnick, and Suwalsky (Citation2014) tracked language development from 20 months to 14 years, showing that stability increases with age (see also Bornstein, Hahn, & Putnick, Citation2016), and is extremely high between the ages of 10 and 14 years (r = .99). Bornstein et al. used different indicators to form a latent language construct for each time point; at 10 and 14 years the construct reflected reading as well as oral language. High stability between late childhood (9–10 years) and adolescence (14–16 years) has also been reported for latent word reading accuracy measures (Hulslander, Olson, Willcutt, & Wadsworth, Citation2010) and observed reading fluency measures (Eklund et al., Citation2015; Landerl & Wimmer, Citation2008).

Figure 1. Hypothetical illustration of how spread can be consistent (a), decrease, indicating a compensatory pattern (b), or increase, indicating a Matthew effect (c).

Note. Growth, stability and spread are statistically independent from each other. In this example, there is there is consistent mean growth (black dashed line) and perfect stability (gray solid lines), with earlier rank in distribution identical to later rank.
Figure 1. Hypothetical illustration of how spread can be consistent (a), decrease, indicating a compensatory pattern (b), or increase, indicating a Matthew effect (c).

Growth and stability can also be distinguished from spread (see ), which relates to whether the distribution of scores over time stays the same, decreases, indicating a compensatory pattern, or increases, such that the “rich get richer and the poor get poorer” (so called Matthew effects; Pfost, Hattie, Dörfler, & Artelt, Citation2014; Stanovich, Citation1986). Matthew effects have been reported for reading comprehension between 7 and 10 years (Quinn et al., Citation2015), and for vocabulary knowledge between childhood and 16 years (Duff et al., Citation2015). However, a compensatory pattern has also been reported for a reading composite between 6 and 12 years (Shaywitz et al., Citation1995). A recent meta-analysis shows inconsistency across studies but suggests that Matthew effects were more likely for sensitive and reliable reading measures (Pfost et al., Citation2014). In sum, limited longitudinal research indicates that in adolescence, reading growth slows to a plateau (Francis et al., Citation1996), rank order amongst individuals is high (Bornstein et al., Citation2014) and the spread of reading abilities might be consistent, widen or narrow (Pfost et al., Citation2014).

Reading comprehension, word reading and oral vocabulary in childhood and adolescence

The simple view of reading sees reading comprehension as the product of decoding and listening comprehension (Gough & Tunmer, Citation1986). However, this view may be “too simple” (e.g., Kirby & Savage, Citation2008). It has been suggested that other factors are also important for reading comprehension, including reading fluency, oral vocabulary knowledge, grammar and higher-level processes such as inference (Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC), Citation2015; Lervåg et al., Citation2018). Indeed, recent instantiations of the simple view refer more comprehensively to “word recognition” or “word reading” and “language comprehension” (e.g., Tunmer & Chapman, Citation2012). The simple view components underpin other theoretical frameworks such as the reading systems framework (Perfetti & Stafura, Citation2014) and direct and inferential mediation (DIME) model (e.g., Ahmed et al., Citation2016; Cromley & Azevedo, Citation2007), though these approaches additionally emphasize the importance of background knowledge (see also Barnes, Ahmed, Barth, & Francis, Citation2015) and integrating information to build a mental representation of the text (cf. Gernsbacher, Varner, & Faust, Citation1990; Kintsch, Citation1998).

The simple view predicts that the relative contribution of word reading and language comprehension to reading comprehension will change with development. For beginning readers, word reading abilities are the primary influence on reading comprehension but this shifts to language comprehension in mid-late childhood (Foorman, Koon, Petscher, Mitchell, & Truckenmiller, Citation2015; LARRC, Citation2015; Lervåg et al., Citation2018). Thus, word reading does not appear to be an important predictor of individual differences in reading comprehension during adolescence (Foorman et al., Citation2015). In contrast, language proficiency continues to explain variation in reading comprehension throughout the lifespan. Our study assesses these ideas in a longitudinal study (12–14 years). We focus on one aspect of language, oral vocabulary knowledge, or knowledge of the spoken (phonological) forms and meanings (semantics) of words. Logically, a reader must understand the words in a text to understand it. Indeed, the lexical quality hypothesis (Perfetti & Hart, Citation2002) emphasizes the importance of high quality lexical representations (i.e. those with well integrated word forms and meanings) for reading development. Other theoretical approaches also highlight the importance of lexical knowledge for the comprehension processes that allow readers to build a mental representation of the text (e.g., inference, integration; Cromley & Azevedo, Citation2007; Perfetti & Stafura, Citation2014). Ahmed et al. (Citation2016) showed that for adolescents (ages 12– 14), vocabulary knowledge made the strongest contribution to reading comprehension, both directly, and indirectly via inferencing, as compared to other key variables (word reading, background knowledge, inferencing and strategies).

Intervention studies and longitudinal research provide strong evidence that, in childhood, oral vocabulary knowledge plays a causal role in explaining variation in reading comprehension. In an intervention with 8–9 year olds, improvements in reading comprehension brought about by an oral language intervention were partly mediated by gains in oral vocabulary knowledge (Clarke, Snowling, Truelove, & Hulme, Citation2010; see also Elleman, Lindo, Morphy, & Compton, Citation2009). Longitudinally, Quinn et al. (Citation2015) showed that between 7 and 10 years, oral vocabulary knowledge predicted subsequent growth in reading comprehension. Reynolds and Turek (Citation2012) assessed oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension in adolescence (age 15 years) as well as childhood (ages 9 and 11). Again, vocabulary predicted growth in reading comprehension.

Reciprocal relations between oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension might also be expected because reading provides opportunities for new words to be learned and for existing lexical representations to be refined (Nation, Citation2017; Ricketts, Bishop, Pimperton, & Nation, Citation2011; Wagner & Meros, Citation2010). Such reciprocity has been reported in some longitudinal studies with children (e.g., Cain & Oakhill, Citation2011; Verhoeven, van Leeuwe, & Vermeer, Citation2011), but not in Quinn et al. (Citation2015). Arguably, the role of reading in oral vocabulary growth may become even more important in adolescence and adulthood as pupils encounter texts that contain more low frequency words that rarely occur in spoken language. However, Reynolds and Turek (Citation2012) did not observe this pattern. In the present study, we build on research by Reynolds and Turek by using latent variables to index oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension and extend Quinn et al.’s study by investigating development beyond childhood. We extend both studies by tracking development within adolescence and including a latent measure of word reading.

The present study

We measured oral vocabulary knowledge, word reading and reading comprehension longitudinally from 12 to 14 years to investigate how these capacities develop and interact within adolescence. Multiple measures of each construct were administered allowing latent variables to be used to control for measurement error. We addressed the following research questions:

  1. Do word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary show stability in the rank order of individuals over time?

  2. Is there growth in word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge over time?

  3. Do attainment gaps between the most and least able individuals widen, narrow or stay the same over time?

  4. What are the developmental relationships between word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge between 12 and 14 years?

We predicted that there would be high stability in reading and oral vocabulary as this has been observed for reading accuracy and fluency (Eklund et al., Citation2015; Hulslander et al., Citation2010), and language (Bornstein et al., Citation2016, Citation2014). We also expected to see growth in reading (Francis et al., Citation1996) and oral vocabulary knowledge (Duff et al., Citation2015). Since Francis et al. investigated growth in a composite reading measure, we sought to replicate growth when word reading and reading comprehension were modeled separately. We anticipated Matthew effects for oral vocabulary knowledge (Duff et al., Citation2015) but that spread for reading might remain constant, widen or narrow (Pfost et al., Citation2014). We expected oral vocabulary knowledge to predict growth in reading comprehension, but not vice versa (Reynolds & Turek, Citation2012). Finally, we did not expect word reading to make a unique contribution to reading comprehension during adolescence (Foorman et al., Citation2015).

Method

Participants

210 pupils (102 girls, 49%) aged 11– 12 years (Mage = 12.01, SD = .33) were recruited from three socially mixed mainstream schools in England. They had received six or seven years of schooling (and formal literacy instruction) by the beginning of the study. At recruitment (Time 1), 14% of participants were in receipt of free school meals (n = 29), an index of family income, 13% were recorded as being on the special educational needs register (n = 27) and 8% spoke English as an additional language (n = 17), consistent with concurrent national data for England (free school meals: 16%, special educational needs: 8%, English as an additional language: 13%). Participants were followed longitudinally at two further time points at ages 12– 13 (Mage = 13.07, SD = .34) and 13– 14 (Mage = 14.01, SD = .33). Testing points were approximately 12 months apart (Difference Time 1 vs. Time 2: M = 12.45 months, SD = .52; Difference Time 2 vs. Time 3: M = 11.11 months, SD = .51). Attrition rates were low, with 6% and 5% lost between Times 1 and 2, and Times 2 and 3, respectively (Time 2 N = 196; Time 3 N = 187). Ethical approval for the study was provided by the University of Reading Ethics Committee and the study conforms to the British Psychological Society Code of Ethics and Conduct. Participants assented to take part and informed consent was provided by their legal guardians.

Materials and procedure

At each time point, participants completed a battery of standardized assessments to characterize the sample (see ). A subset were included in analyses to address our hypotheses. Tasks were administered individually in a fixed order across two 60-minute sessions.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics at Time 1 (11–12 years), Time 2 (12–13 years) and Time 3 (13–14 years).

Nonverbal reasoning

This was measured using the Matrix Reasoning subtest of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence – Second Edition (WASI-II; Wechsler, Citation2011), which is a pattern completion task. The WASI-II provides US norms (t-scores: M = 50, SD = 10).

Word reading

This was assessed using Form A of the Test of Word Reading Efficiency – Second Edition (TOWRE-2; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, Citation2011), the revised Castles and Coltheart test (CC2; Castles et al., Citation2009)Footnote1 and the Single Word Reading Test (SWRT) from the York Assessment of Reading Comprehension (Stothard, Hulme, Clarke, Barnby, & Snowling, Citation2010). The TOWRE-2 includes a Sight Word Efficiency (SWE) subtest in which word reading efficiency is indexed by the number of words read correctly in 45 seconds. The CC2 includes 40 regular words, 40 irregular words and 40 nonwords, with item type interleaved. Only the irregular word reading subtest from the CC2 was included in analyses (see below). The SWRT includes 70 words. The CC2 and SWRT are untimed measures and reading accuracy is measured as the number of items read correctly. Norms (standard scores: M = 100, SD = 15) for this age range were available for the TOWRE-2 (US norms) and SWRT (UK norms) but not for the CC2.

Reading comprehension

This was assessed with three passages (length in words: M = 468.67, SD = 3.79) from the York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension (YARC; Stothard et al., Citation2010): one fiction passage (Schoolboy: 1.1A) and two nonfiction passages (Bees: 1.2A, Louise Nevelson: 2.2A). For each passage, silent reading was followed by 13 open-ended comprehension questions that tapped literal and inferential understanding. Passages 1.1A and 1.2A are age-appropriate whereas passage 2.2A is more difficult (aimed at 15–16 year olds) and includes more complex language. The YARC provides UK norms (M = 100, SD = 15) and ability scores. Ability scores were calculated from the Rasch model and express reading ability on an interval scale; such scores are not age adjusted.

Oral vocabulary knowledge

This was indexed by the British Picture Vocabulary Scale – Third Edition (BPVS-3; Dunn & Dunn, Citation2009) and the Vocabulary subtest of the WASI-II (Wechsler, Citation2011) to capture both breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge. The BPVS-3 was used as a measure of receptive vocabulary knowledge. Participants are given a word and are asked to select the matching picture from an array of four options. The Vocabulary subtest of the WASI-II is a measure of expressive vocabulary in which children are asked to define words verbally. The BPVS-3 provides UK norms (M = 100, SD = 15), and the WASI-II generates US norms (M = 50, SD = 10).

Results

shows the means and standard deviations for all observed measures at each time point (Time 1– 3), along with maximum scores and reliabilities. The means and standard deviations for age-standardized measures indicated a distribution of scores that was commensurate with test norms. Notably, the mean standard scores for BPVS vocabulary at Time 1 and Time 2 was a little lower than the other means, though these means are still within the average range. Raw scores indicated ceiling effects on one measure (CC2 regular word reading) and large variation in vocabulary knowledge, word reading and reading comprehension in early adolescence. While there was no evidence of floor effects on any measure, there was evidence of low levels of vocabulary knowledge and reading proficiency.

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), measurement invariance and stability of word reading, reading comprehension and vocabulary

All further analyses were conducted with raw scores from each observed variable except reading comprehension where we used ability scores that account for differences in difficulty across passages (for correlations, see Supplementary Materials). We used structural equation models in Mplus 8.0 (Muthén and Muthén (Citation1998-2017)) and Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) was used to handle missing data, which was minimal (see ). The first step was to use confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to establish factor structures for word reading, reading comprehension and vocabulary at Times 1– 3 and assess measurement invariance across time. Assessing measurement invariance enabled us to investigate whether the three constructs could be defined in the same way across time. We tested whether, over time, factor loadings for observed variables were consistent (metric invariance) and whether both factor loadings and intercepts for observed variables were consistent (scalar invariance). Establishing measurement invariance was a necessary step before estimating growth in the latent variables.

In the first CFA ()), raw scores on three word reading measures were used as indicators for a latent word reading construct at each time point: the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE-2), the Castles and Coltheart test (CC2, irregular word reading only) and the Single Word Reading Test (SWRT). The CC2 regular word reading measure was omitted due to ceiling effects, and CC2 nonwords because it measured nonword rather than word reading. For the latent word reading construct there was only partial metric invariance (Δχ2 (2) = 3.794, p = .150) across time, with factor loadings for TOWRE-2 varying across time. This indicates that performance on word reading tests developed at an uneven rate. Given this partial metric invariance, factor loadings of TOWRE-2 and all intercepts were estimated freely across time. For this model we found strong factor loadings and excellent fit to the data, χ2 (17) = 22.352, RSMEA = .039 (90% CI = .000-.078), CFI = .998, TLI = .995, SRMS = .035. Coefficient H indicated excellent construct reliability, H = .930, H = .904 and H = .930, at Times 1– 3, respectively. We found high correlations between factors at Times 1– 3, indicating almost full stability in the rank order between children across time.

Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analyses with standardized estimates of: word reading (a), reading comprehension (b) and vocabulary (c).

Note. Time 1: 11–12 years; Time 2: 12–13 years; Time 3: 13–14 years
Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analyses with standardized estimates of: word reading (a), reading comprehension (b) and vocabulary (c).

In the second CFA ()), raw scores on the three reading comprehension passages were used as indicators of a latent reading comprehension construct at each time point. The reading comprehension model showed full scalar invariance (Δχ2 (8) = 7.150, p = .521), showing that the indicators changed equivalently across time. Therefore, factor loadings and intercepts were fixed to be equal across time. This model had strong factor loadings and excellent fit, χ2 (23) = 19.154, RSMEA = .000 (90% CI = .000-.045), CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, SRMS = .041, and very good construct reliability, H = . 879, H = .868 and H = .861, for Times 1– 3, respectively. Again, high correlations between factors indicated very high stability over time.

In a final CFA ()), raw scores on the two observed vocabulary variables were used as indicators of a latent vocabulary construct at each time point. Here we found metric invariance (Δχ2 (2) = 3.809, p = .149) but only partial scalar invariance across time (Δχ2 (3) = 4.722, p = .192): the intercept of the BPVS at Time 2 was significantly different from Time 1 and Time 3. Therefore, we fixed factor loadings and intercepts to be equal across time, with the exception of the Time 2 BPVS intercept, which was estimated freely. This model had strong factor loadings and showed an excellent fit, χ2 (6) = 5.333, RSMEA = .000 (90% CI = .000-.084), CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, SRMS = .052, and excellent construct reliability, H = .930, H = .904 and H = .930, for Times 1–3, respectively. Once more, high correlations between the time points indicated extremely high stability. In sum, the CFAs yielded latent constructs for word reading, reading comprehension and vocabulary with good reliabilities and very high longitudinal stabilities, as expected.

Growth and spread in word reading, reading comprehension and vocabulary

Next, we estimated unconditional growth models to address our hypotheses about growth and spread. Reading comprehension and vocabulary had full and partial scalar measurement invariance across time, respectively. Therefore, we estimated growth factor models for these two processes. As only partial metric invariance was found for word reading, we estimated a parallel growth model with separate intercepts and slopes for each of the three measures of word reading. Reading comprehension, vocabulary and SWRT word reading fitted a linear growth pattern well and the slope factor was coded 0, 1, 2. As the TOWRE-2 and the CC2 word reading measures had non-linear growth curves, the first factor loading of the slope was coded 0, the factor loading in the middle was freely estimated and the last factor loading coded 2. In these models the intercept is defined as the initial status (performance at Time 1) and the slope reflects average growth per year between the first and the last time point.

The unstandardized means of the intercept (initial status) and the slopes (growth), together with their standard deviations and the standardized residuals, are shown in (the unstandardized residuals are fixed to be equal for the same variables across time). The means of the slopes can be interpreted as the mean growth per year. As illustrated by significant slope means, there was significant growth in all processes. The standard deviations of the slopes indicate little variation in the growth rates of the children. Variation in growth rates (around the mean growth) was significant for the latent vocabulary construct and one observed word reading variable (SWRT), but not significant for the latent reading comprehension construct. For the TOWRE-2 and CC2 word reading measures, we fixed the slope variance to zero as the original estimations gave a negative but nonsignificant variation (i.e., a nonsignificant Heywood case). The correlations between initial status and growth for the three estimated slopes (reading comprehension, vocabulary and SWRT) were negative, indicating decreased spread over time. All models had small residuals and fitted the data very well (reading comprehension: χ2 (26) = 34.475, RSMEA = .040 (90% CI = .000-.072), CFI = .994, TLI = .992, SRMS = .071; vocabulary: χ2 (9) = 9.805, RSMEA = .021 (90% CI = .000-.082), CFI = .999, TLI = .999, SRMS = .060; word-level reading: χ2 (31) = 56.760, RSMEA = .063 (90% CI = .036-.088), CFI = .989, TLI = .988, SRMS = .079). In sum, for word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge there was significant growth and decreased spread (cf. )), with the latter indicating a narrowing of performance gaps over time.

Table 2. Parameters for growth models.

Predicting growth between processes

Having investigated developmental processes (stability, growth and spread), we examined developmental relations between oral vocabulary knowledge and reading, addressing the following hypotheses: (1) oral vocabulary knowledge would predict growth in reading comprehension; (2) reading comprehension would not predict oral vocabulary knowledge; and (3) word reading would not predict growth in reading comprehension. To do this, we compared two models: a model with only paths from the intercept to the slope of each process (e.g., word reading intercept to word reading slope; see panel a of ); and a model that additionally included the three hypothesized paths from intercept to slope across the processes (see panel b of ). As before, we modeled the three word-level reading measures separately. Therefore, we compared three pairs of models, one including each word reading measure. Chi-square difference tests showed no significant difference between any of the three pairs of models: TOWRE-2: Δχ2 (3) = 6.604, p = .086, CC2: Δχ2 (3) = 1.901, p = .593, SWRT: Δχ2 (3) = 4.475, p = .215. Therefore, we favored the more parsimonious models, which did not include paths between processes. Panel c of shows the final model with SWRT word reading, with nonsignificant correlations between the residuals deleted (the other two models yielded equivalent findings, details can be found in the Supplementary Materials). Model fit for this model was very good: χ2 (130) = 155.699, p = .062, RSMEA = .031 (90% CI = .000-.047), CFI = .994, TLI = .993, SRMS = .065. The very high regressions between intercepts and their corresponding slopes show that initial status is a strong predictor of subsequent progress for word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge.

Figure 3. Models establishing prediction of growth between processes. Panel (a) shows a model with paths from the intercept to slope of each process, panel (b) shows paths from the intercept to slope plus hypothesized paths between processes, and panel (c) shows the final model of growth between processes with the SWRT measure of word reading with standardized estimates.

Notes. Three pairs of models were compared, one with each word reading measure (SWRT, TOWRE SWE, CC2 irregular). As the variance of the slopes in the TOWRE and the SWRT models were fixed to zero, the dashed lines in (a) and (b) were only estimated for the SWRT model. The TOWRE and CC2 models are presented in the Supplementary Materials.
Figure 3. Models establishing prediction of growth between processes. Panel (a) shows a model with paths from the intercept to slope of each process, panel (b) shows paths from the intercept to slope plus hypothesized paths between processes, and panel (c) shows the final model of growth between processes with the SWRT measure of word reading with standardized estimates.

Are reading comprehension and vocabulary separable from each other?

) shows an extremely high correlation between reading comprehension and vocabulary intercepts. This led us to explore whether these constructs were separable from each other at the three time points. To do this we compared two CFA models (see ). Both models included latent variables for vocabulary and reading at all time points. One model included correlations between both processes at all time points (see panel a of ), while the second included a second-order language factor for each time point, defined by measures of vocabulary and reading comprehension (see panel b of ). A chi-square difference test showed no significant difference between the two models (Δχ2 (6) = 2.238, p = .897), therefore we favored the more parsimonious second-order model with fewer parameters. This model shows that at each time point, vocabulary and reading comprehension load on the same language factor with no evidence for separable reading comprehension and vocabulary factors.

Figure 4. Model comparison used to establish whether reading comprehension and vocabulary are separable. Panel (a) shows a model with correlations between processes at all time points, panel (b) shows a model with a second-order language factor for each time point.

Note. Time 1: 11–12 years; Time 2: 12–13 years; Time 3: 13–14 years
Figure 4. Model comparison used to establish whether reading comprehension and vocabulary are separable. Panel (a) shows a model with correlations between processes at all time points, panel (b) shows a model with a second-order language factor for each time point.

The final model appears in . The factor loadings of the three common language constructs in this model were very high varying from .971 to 1.0 and the correlations between the three time points were essentially perfect (rT1-T2 = 1.00, rT2-T3 = .989 and rT1-T3 = .988). A lower Bayesian Information Criteria (BIC) for a model where the correlations between the three common language constructs are fixed to one (16,570.097 vs. 16,582.799) suggested that the rank order was perfectly preserved across the three time points of the study. This model had an excellent fit to the data, χ2 (81) = 71.678, p = .761, RSMEA = .000 (90% CI = .000-.028), CFI = 1.00, TLI = 1.00, SRMS = .061.

Figure 5. Final model with second-order language factor for each time point.

Note. Time 1: 11–12 years; Time 2: 12–13 years; Time 3: 13–14 years; estimates are standardized
Figure 5. Final model with second-order language factor for each time point.

Discussion

We tracked word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge from 12– 14 years. For each construct, we observed high stability in the rank order of individuals, significant growth, and evidence that achievement gaps were narrowing. Most strikingly, our analyses indicated that, during this period, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge were best conceptualized as reflecting a single higher order language construct.

Adolescent development in reading and vocabulary: stability, growth, and spread

Stability in the rank order of individuals was extremely high for word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge latent constructs. Previous studies have noted similarly high stability from late childhood (age 9 or 10) to adolescence (age 14) in latent measures of language (Bornstein et al., Citation2014) and word reading accuracy (Hulslander et al., Citation2010), and observed measures of reading fluency (Eklund et al., Citation2015; Landerl & Wimmer, Citation2008). Notably though, stability appears to be lower earlier in development (e.g., Eklund et al., Citation2015), and for subgroups of children, as demonstrated by the existence of reading difficulties that resolve or emerge in late childhood and early adolescence (e.g., Catts, Compton, Tomblin, & Bridges, Citation2012; Torppa, Eklund, van Bergen, & Lyytinen, Citation2015).

Growth over time was significant for word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge between the ages of 12 and 14. While growth was fairly minimal in real terms (see ), this could still be functionally important. Only one study had tracked reading development within adolescence (Francis et al., Citation1996) and our findings resonate with this study in showing that reading continues to grow well into adolescence.

In relation to spread, our analyses revealed negative intercept-slope associations, indicating that for word reading, reading comprehension and vocabulary, attainment gaps between the highest and lowest performing pupils were reducing in early secondary school. Notably, this effect was more pronounced for reading comprehension and vocabulary than for word reading. Our findings are in line with Shaywitz et al. (Citation1995), who observed this for reading. Like them, we think it unlikely that this reflects poor measurement sensitivity due to ceiling effects (see ). Instead, we interpret this correlation as indicating a compensatory pattern of growth in early adolescence, such that lower achieving individuals show more growth than higher achieving peers who are approaching a natural plateau in development. Also, some of our participants may have received effective intervention, though we did not collect information about this. Importantly, our findings do not support the existence of “Matthew effects” for reading and oral vocabulary knowledge in early adolescence, contrasting with previous research (Duff et al., Citation2015; Quinn et al., Citation2015). Like Pfost et al. (Citation2014), we call for further longitudinal research to reconcile mixed findings. One possibility is that while achievement gaps widen in childhood, compensatory patterns characterize adolescent development.

Though achievement gaps may be narrowing in adolescence, they were not closing. Coupled with limited (though significant) growth and almost perfect stability between 12 and 14 years, this may indicate a rather bleak picture for addressing language and literacy difficulties at this point in time. However, without systematic instruction and intervention in secondary school, this conclusion is premature. In England, whilst there is a clear curriculum for teaching foundational readings skills (e.g., word reading, using comprehension strategies) in the primary classroom, these skills are rarely taught in secondary schools and teachers report that they lack the knowledge and resources to do this. It is entirely plausible that with professional development and appropriate instruction and intervention tools, secondary schools can promote reading abilities (cf. Hulslander et al., Citation2010). Indeed, promising intervention approaches have yielded modest improvements in reading comprehension for secondary pupils (Clarke, Paul, Smith, Snowling, & Hulme, Citation2017; Scammacca, Roberts, Vaughn, & Stuebing, Citation2015; Vaughn et al., Citation2013). For those at the bottom of our distribution even modest improvements could be functionally important.

Developmental relationships between reading and oral vocabulary in adolescence

For word reading, reading comprehension and oral vocabulary knowledge, initial status in each construct was an extremely strong predictor of growth in that construct, leaving very little scope for predicting additional variation in growth (cf. Hulslander et al., Citation2010). Further analyses showed word reading at 12 years did not predict subsequent growth in reading comprehension, and that reading comprehension and vocabulary were best captured as a single construct. Before we consider the theoretical implications of these findings, some methodological considerations are worthy of note. One reason for the particularly close relationship between oral vocabulary knowledge and reading could be that the reading comprehension task that we used is particularly dependent on oral vocabulary knowledge. We measured reading comprehension with three passages from the York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension (YARC; Stothard et al., Citation2010). The YARC categorizes comprehension questions by type (vocabulary but also literal, knowledge-based inference etc), and answering seven of the 39 questions at each time point indexed vocabulary knowledge for low frequency words: e.g., what does “commissions” mean? Post-hoc analyses estimated alternative latent constructs for reading comprehension that did not include the vocabulary-dependent questions. The relationship between reading comprehension and vocabulary knowledge remained extremely high (r = .95 – .99) indicating that these vocabulary items did not unduly influence our findings.

In a similar vein, our findings may have been influenced by our choice to administer multiple measures from a single reading comprehension test (Stothard et al., Citation2010), rather than multiple tests. We chose to use the YARC because it is the only age-appropriate individually administered reading comprehension test with UK norms. It is also valid and reasonably reliable given the complexity of measuring reading comprehension (see for Cronbach’s alpha, and Supplementary Materials for test-retest reliability). We also chose to administer the same YARC passages each year. It is possible (though perhaps unlikely) that our growth estimates are influenced by familiarity with the passages in addition to reading comprehension development. If this was the case, narrowing in achievement gaps could, in part, reflect poorer comprehenders showing greater benefit from annual re-reading. It would be useful for future research to replicate our findings with more than one standardized assessment of reading comprehension (cf. Catts, Citation2018) and by using different, but equated, passages across time.

A strength of our study is that our latent oral vocabulary construct captured breadth and depth, as well as receptive and expressive knowledge about words. However, vocabulary knowledge extends beyond lexical knowledge (Smith & Murphy, Citation2014) and our measures focused on knowledge of everyday words, rather than academic vocabulary or knowledge of words that is closely linked to the secondary school curriculum (Laufer & Nation, Citation1995; Snow, Citation2010). Further, oral vocabulary is just one aspect of oral language. Grammar, inferencing and listening comprehension also deserve attention in adolescence as these variables explain individual differences in reading comprehension in childhood, both concurrently (e.g., LARRC & Logan, Citation2017) and longitudinally (LARRC & Chiu, Citation2018; Lervåg et al., Citation2018). Inferencing is also a strong concurrent predictor of reading comprehension in adolescence (Ahmed et al., Citation2016) and vocabulary and grammar may be discrete constructs in adolescence (Tomblin & Zhang, Citation2006), and therefore show different longitudinal relationships with reading comprehension. Beyond oral language, there are other factors that may be important for adolescent reading comprehension including reader-level variables like attention, motivation for reading and amount of reading practice (Arrington, Kulesz, Francis, Fletcher, & Barnes, Citation2014; McGeown, Duncan, Griffiths, & Stothard, Citation2015; Mol & Bus, Citation2011), and text-level factors like text type (Francis, Kulesz, & Benoit, Citation2018).

The simple view of reading (Gough & Tunmer, Citation1986) posits that reading comprehension is the product of word reading and oral language comprehension processes. Returning to our findings, we observed correlations between word reading and reading comprehension that are equivalent to existing concurrent studies (García & Cain, Citation2014). However, and in line with cross-sectional findings (Foorman et al., Citation2015; Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC), Citation2015), our findings suggest that by 12 years, a shift has occurred such that word reading is no longer such a strong predictor of individual differences in reading comprehension in English. Rather, oral language (as indexed here by vocabulary knowledge) and reading comprehension are closely intertwined (cf. Ahmed et al., Citation2016), and these constructs are less closely related to word reading. This does seem to contradict Foorman et al.’s cross-sectional observation of that word reading fluency and oral vocabulary can be captured as a single lexical construct in adolescence. It also raises interesting questions about the dimensionality of oral and written language in adolescence that should be addressed in future longitudinal research.

Though we hypothesized that oral vocabulary knowledge would predict subsequent growth in reading comprehension (Reynolds & Turek, Citation2012), exploratory analyses indicated that oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension could be captured as a single construct. Key theoretical perspectives emphasize the importance of oral vocabulary knowledge for reading comprehension. In the reading systems framework (Perfetti & Stafura, Citation2014) and related approaches (e.g., Perfetti & Hart, Citation2002), strong connections between word forms and meanings are critical for reading comprehension. There are a number of mechanisms that might explain the close relationship between oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension. Oral vocabulary knowledge will make direct contributions to reading comprehension by allowing readers to understand individual words in a text. In addition, efficient retrieval of meaning will free resources so that the reader can engage in higher level comprehension processes like integration and inference (cf. Perfetti & Hart, Citation2002). The relationship between oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension may also be mediated by integration processes and the ability to make inferences (e.g., Barnes et al., Citation2015; Ahmed et al., Citation2016; LARRC, Currie & Muijselaar, Citation2019). On the one hand, knowledge of the meanings of specific words may be important for making inferences, and therefore for building a mental representation of the text. Conversely, being able to make inferences is thought to be important for using context to learn the meanings of new words. Oral vocabulary knowledge may also reflect the contribution of background knowledge more broadly. Indeed, reading comprehension will be influenced by a reader’s background knowledge of the text (e.g., Ahmed et al., Citation2016).

Conclusions

We tracked oral vocabulary knowledge and reading from 12– 14 years in 210 adolescents, obtaining multiple indicators for word reading, oral vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension so that we could model these constructs using latent variables that control for measurement error. Our findings indicate that reading and vocabulary show high stability in the rank order of individuals, and continue to grow in early adolescence, with decreasing variation amongst individuals. These findings underline the importance of conducting further language and literacy research with adolescents. Data are needed to conceptualize changes in adolescent reading so that we can constrain theories of reading development. This will add to a growing broader understanding of the important changes that occur during adolescence (Blakemore, Citation2018). A better understanding of adolescent reading development will inform educational strategies and interventions for supporting adolescent reading. Indeed, during adolescence, reading becomes an increasingly important mechanism for learning and yet we know little about how to support reading effectively at this point. Our findings indicate that any such support should emphasize oral vocabulary knowledge, as well as reading abilities.

Author contributions

Jessie Ricketts conceived of and led the study, and took the lead in manuscript writing. Arne Lervåg led the data modelling and contributed to manuscript writing. Nicola Dawson and Lucy A. Taylor contributed to study design and manuscript preparation. Charles Hulme contributed to study design, data analysis and manuscript writing.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Rachael Sperring, Fay Bainbridge, Rachel Tomkinson, Natascha Ahmed, Keely Pridden, Rosie McGuire, Grace Pocock and Eva Dvorakova for their assistance with data collection and all schools and families for participating.

Additional information

Funding

This research was primarily supported by an Economic and Social Research Council grant awarded to Jessie Ricketts (grant number ES/K008064/1), with additional funding from the University of Reading. Data can be accessed as follows: Ricketts, J. (2018). Vocabulary and reading in secondary school: Evidence from longitudinal and experimental studies . [data collection]. UK Data Service. SN: 852865, http://doi.org/10.5255/UKDA-SN-852865

Notes

1. The CC2 can be accessed freely from Macquarie University: https://www.motif.org.au/home/test/cc2.

References

  • Ahmed, Y., Francis, D. J., York, M., Fletcher, J. M., Barnes, M., & Kulesz, P. (2016). Validation of the direct and inferential mediation (DIME) model of reading comprehension in grades 7 through 12. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 44-45, 68–82. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2016.02.002
  • Arrington, C. N., Kulesz, P. A., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., & Barnes, M. A. (2014). The contribution of attentional control and working memory to reading comprehension and decoding. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(5), 325–346. doi:10.1080/10888438.2014.902461
  • Barnes, M. A., Ahmed, Y., Barth, A., & Francis, D. J. (2015). The relation of knowledge-text integration processes and reading comprehension in 7th- to 12th-Grade students. Scientific Studies of Reading, 19(4), 253–272. doi:10.1080/10888438.2015.1022650
  • Ben-Shachar, M., Dougherty, R. F., Deutsch, G. K., & Wandell, B. A. (2011). The development of cortical sensitivity to visual word forms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2387–2399. doi:10.1162/jocn.2011.21615
  • Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Avoiding social risk in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(2), 116–122. doi:10.1177/0963721417738144
  • Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Putnick, D. L. (2016). Long-term stability of core language skill in children with contrasting language skills. Developmental Psychology, 52(5), 704–716. doi:10.1037/dev0000111
  • Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., Putnick, D. L., & Suwalsky, J. T. D. (2014). Stability of core language skill from early childhood to adolescence: A latent variable approach. Child Development, 85(4), 1346–1356. doi:10.1111/cdev.12192
  • Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Esposito, G. (2017). Continuity and stability in development. Child Development Perspectives, 11(2), 113–119. doi:10.1111/cdep.12221
  • Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2011). Matthew effects in young readers: Reading comprehension and reading experience aid vocabulary development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44, 431–443. doi:10.1177/0022219411410042
  • Castles, A., Coltheart, M., Larsen, L., Jones, P., Saunders, S., & McArthur, G. (2009). Assessing the basic components of reading: A revision of the Castles and Coltheart test with new norms. Australian Journal of Learning Difficulties, 14(1), 67–88. doi:10.1080/19404150902783435
  • Catts, H. W. (2018). The simple view of reading: Advancements and false impressions. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 317–323. doi:10.1177/0741932518767563
  • Catts, H. W., Compton, D., Tomblin, J. B., & Bridges, M. S. (2012). Prevalence and nature of late-emerging poor readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 166–181. doi:10.1037/a0025323
  • Chiu, Y. D.; Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC). (2018). The simple view of reading across development: Prediction of grade 3 reading comprehension from prekindergarten skills. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 289–303.
  • Clarke, P. J., Paul, S.-A. S., Smith, G., Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2017). Reading intervention for poor readers at the transition to secondary school. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1–20. doi:10.1080/10888438.2017.1318393
  • Clarke, P. J., Snowling, M. J., Truelove, E., & Hulme, C. (2010). Ameliorating children’s reading comprehension difficulties: A randomised controlled trial. Psychological Science, 21, 1106–1116. doi:10.1177/0956797610375449
  • Cromley, J. G., & Azevedo, R. (2007). Testing and refining the direct and inferential mediation model of reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99(2), 311–325. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.2.311
  • Currie, N. K., & Muijselaar, M. M. L., Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC). (2019). Inference making in young children: The concurrent and longitudinal contributions of verbal working memory and vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, doi:10.1037/edu0000342
  • Duff, D., Tomblin, J. B., & Catts, H. (2015). The influence of reading on vocabulary growth: A case for a Matthew Effect. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58(3), 853–864. doi:10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-13-0310
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M., & NFER. (2009). British Picture Vocabulary Scale - Third Edition (3rd ed.). London, UK: GL Assessment Ltd.
  • Eklund, K., Torppa, M., Aro, M., Leppänen, P. H. T., & Lyytinen, H. (2015). Literacy skill development of children with familial risk for dyslexia through grades 2, 3, and 8. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 126–140. doi:10.1037/a0037121
  • Elleman, A. M., Lindo, E. J., Morphy, P., & Compton, D. L. (2009). The impact of vocabulary instruction on passage-level comprehension of school-age children: A meta-analysis. Journal of Research on Educational Effectiveness, 2(1), 1–44. doi:10.1080/19345740802539200
  • Foorman, B. R., Koon, S., Petscher, Y., Mitchell, A., & Truckenmiller, A. (2015). Examining general and specific factors in the dimensionality of oral language and reading in 4th–10th grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(3), 884. doi:10.1037/edu0000026
  • Francis, D. J., Kulesz, P. A., & Benoit, J. S. (2018). Extending the simple view of reading to account for variation within readers and across texts: The Complete View of Reading (CVRi). Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 274–288. doi:10.1177/0741932518772904
  • Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A., & Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: A longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 3–17. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.88.1.3
  • García, J. R., & Cain, K. (2014). Decoding and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis to identify which reader and assessment characteristics influence the strength of the relationship in English. Review of Educational Research, 84(1), 74–111. doi:10.3102/0034654313499616
  • Gernsbacher, M. A., Varner, K. R., & Faust, M. E. (1990). Investigating differences in general comprehension skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 430–445. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.16.3.430
  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10. doi:10.1177/074193258600700104
  • Hulslander, J., Olson, R. K., Willcutt, E. G., & Wadsworth, S. J. (2010). Longitudinal stability of reading-related skills and their prediction of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14(2), 111–136. doi:10.1080/10888431003604058
  • Jerrim, J., & Shure, N. (2016). Achievement of 15-Year-Olds in England: PISA 2015 National Report. London, UK: Department for Education.
  • Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading?. Literacy, 42(2), 75–82. doi:10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x
  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 150–161. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.100.1.150
  • Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC). (2015). Learning to read: Should we keep things simple? Reading Research Quarterly, 50(2), 151–169. doi:10.1002/rrq.99
  • Language and Reading Research Consortium (LARRC) & Logan, J. (2017). Pressure points in reading comprehension: A quantile multiple regression analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(4), 451–464. doi:10.1037/edu0000150
  • Laufer, B., & Nation, P. (1995). Vocabulary size and use: Lexical richness in L2 written production. Applied Linguistics, 16(3), 307–322. doi:10.1093/applin/16.3.307
  • Lervåg, A., Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2018). Unpicking the developmental relationships between oral language skills and reading comprehension: It’s simple, but complex. Child Development, 89(5), 1821–1838. doi:10.1111/cdev.12861
  • McGeown, S. P., Duncan, L. G., Griffiths, Y. M., & Stothard, S. E. (2015). Exploring the relationship between adolescent’s reading skills, reading motivation and reading habits. Reading and Writing, 28(4), 545–569. doi:10.1007/s11145-014-9537-9
  • Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 267–296. doi:10.1037/a0021890
  • Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (1998-2017). Mplus User’s Guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
  • Nation, K. (2017). Nurturing a lexical legacy: Reading experience is critical for the development of word reading skill. Npj Science of Learning, 2(1), 3. doi:10.1038/s41539-017-0004-7
  • Perfetti, C., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbro, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (Vol. 11), 189–213. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, US: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
  • Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18, 22–37. doi:10.1080/10888438.2013.827687
  • Pfost, M., Hattie, J., Dörfler, T., & Artelt, C. (2014). Individual differences in reading development: A review of 25 years of empirical research on Matthew Effects in reading. Review of Educational Research, 84, 203–244. doi:10.3102/0034654313509492
  • Quinn, J. M., Wagner, R. K., Petscher, Y., & Lopez, D. (2015). Developmental relations between vocabulary knowledge and reading comprehension: A latent change score modeling study. Child Development, 86(1), 159–175. doi:10.1111/cdev.12292
  • Reynolds, M. R., & Turek, J. J. (2012). A dynamic developmental link between verbal comprehension-knowledge (Gc) and reading comprehension: Verbal comprehension-knowledge drives positive change in reading comprehension. Journal of School Psychology, 50(6), 841–863. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2012.07.002
  • Ricketts, J., Bishop, D. V. M., Pimperton, H., & Nation, K. (2011). The role of self-teaching in learning orthographic and semantic aspects of new words. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 47–70. doi:10.1080/10888438.2011.536129
  • Scammacca, N. K., Roberts, G., Vaughn, S., & Stuebing, K. K. (2015). A meta-analysis of interventions for struggling readers in Grades 4–12: 1980–2011. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 369–390. doi:10.1177/0022219413504995
  • Shaywitz, B. A., Holford, T. R., Holahan, J. M., Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Francis, D. J., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1995). A Matthew Effect for IQ but not for reading: Results from a longitudinal study. Reading Research Quarterly, 30(4), 894–906. doi:10.2307/748203
  • Smith, S., & Murphy, V. (2014). Measuring productive elements of multi-word phrase vocabulary knowledge among children with English as an additional or only language. Reading and Writing, 1–23. doi:10.1007/s11145-014-9527-y
  • Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language and the challenge of reading for learning about science. Science, 328(5977), 450–452. doi:10.1126/science.1182597
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–406. doi:10.1598/RRQ.21.4.1
  • Stothard, S., Hulme, C., Clarke, P. J., Barnby, P., & Snowling, M. (2010). The York Assessment of Reading for Comprehension (YARC): Passage Reading Secondary. London, UK: GL Assessment.
  • Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(6), 1193–1208. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086)
  • Torppa, M., Eklund, K., van Bergen, E., & Lyytinen, H. (2015). Late-emerging and resolving dyslexia: A follow-up study from age 3 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(7), 1389–1401. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0003-1
  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2012). The simple view of reading redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(5), 453–466. doi:10.1177/0022219411432685
  • Vaughn, S., Swanson, E. A., Roberts, G., Wanzek, J., Stillman-Spisak, S. J., Solis, M., & Simmons, D. (2013). Improving reading comprehension and social studies knowledge in middle school. Reading Research Quarterly, 48(1), 77–93. doi:10.1002/rrq.039
  • Verhoeven, L., van Leeuwe, J., & Vermeer, A. (2011). Vocabulary growth and reading development across the elementary school years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15(1), 8–25. doi:10.1080/10888438.2011.536125
  • Wagner, R. K., & Meros, D. (2010). Vocabulary and reading comprehension: Direct, indirect, and reciprocal influences. Focus on exceptional children, 2010, 1G1-245473138.
  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2011). Test of word reading efficiency - second edition (TOWRE-2). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
  • Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence - Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, US: TX Pearson.