378
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Longitudinal associations between parenting practices and children’s later decision-making competence

Abstract

Developmental research has suggested that certain parenting practices impact the development of children’s later risk behaviors. However, little evidence exists regarding how parenting may impact a child’s actual decision-making skills later in life. This study examined the degree to which earlier child-reports of parenting practices (assessed at age 10–12 and 12–14), specifically monitoring/involvement, firm/lax control, and psychological control, predicted decision-making competence (DMC), a measure of decision-making skills, at age 19. We conducted these analyses on a large, diverse sample of children (N = 775; 227 Female, 21.8% African-American); approximately half had at least one parent with a positive Substance Use Disorder (SUD) diagnosis. Analyses revealed that children reporting greater levels of psychological control demonstrated lower DMC performance at age 19, holding other parenting variables, level of neighborhood disadvantage, and parental SUD status constant. We relate these results to the broader effects of psychological control on decision-making and self-regulatory capabilities.

The period between early adolescence and emerging adulthood is typically a time when individuals get the opportunity to make substantially more consequential decisions, including driving without restrictions, gaining the legal ability to vote in national elections, choosing whether and where to attend college, deciding career paths, and starting a family. This developmental period also is associated with an increase in problem behaviors, such as delinquency, substance use, and health-risking sexual behaviors, which may indicate the start of a problematic lifelong trajectory (Arnett, Citation1992, Citation2015). It is often argued that problematic risk-taking is suggestive of suboptimal decision-making tendencies (Dahl, Citation2004; Kwan et al., Citation2012; Romer et al., Citation2017; Steinberg, Citation2008; Warren et al., Citation1997). Furthermore, considerable individual differences in normative decision-making skills exist during this time, as children and adolescents develop more cognitive sophistication, which may contribute to problem behavior engagement (Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Toplak et al., Citation2014; Toplak & Flora, Citation2021; Weller et al., Citation2012).

One potential source of this heterogeneity may arise from parent-child interactions. Developmental research has suggested that a host of parenting behaviors, such as low parental monitoring/involvement (i.e., knowing a child’s whereabouts, his/her friends, etc.), inconsistent discipline, and psychological control (i.e., parental attempts to indirectly control a child’s behaviours through manipulative techniques, like instilling guilt and anxiety), may impact the development of risk behaviors later in life (Arnett, Citation2015; Barber et al. Citation2005; Pinquart, Citation2017). Although these studies suggest that these parenting practices influence the choices that children and adolescents make, a dearth of research exists that examines the degree to which parenting practices are associated with suboptimal decision-making skills themselves. Instead, most research has focused on risky problem behaviors (e.g., substance use, delinquency, etc.) as being indicative of poor decision quality, rather than examining the association between parenting and how their children make decisions. To address this question, the current study leveraged a long-term longitudinal study to test the degree to which parental acceptance/rejection, monitoring/involvement, psychological control, and firm vs. lax behavioral control during pre- and early adolescence (age 10–12 and again at 14 years) are associated with performance on an objective measure of decision-making, assessed at approximately age 19.

Associations between parenting practices and children’s decision-making behavior

Numerous influential theoretical models have proposed that negative parenting practices, including, but not limited to, psychological control and harsh and/or inconsistent discipline (O’Connor, Citation2002), adversely impacts a child’s social development and engagement in risk behaviors later in life (Arnett, Citation2015; Baumrind, Citation1967; Gottfredson & Hirschi, Citation1990; Jessor, Citation1987). Meta-analytic research (Li et al., Citation2019) suggests that such practices are robustly associated with lower reported self-regulative tendencies, and invariant with respect to culture and the sex of both the parent and the child (Brody & Ge, Citation2001; Cheung & Cheung, Citation2010; Feldman & Wentzel, Citation1990; Hallquist et al. Citation2015; Li et al., Citation2019; Simons et al., Citation2007; Zhai et al., Citation2014). Similarly, such practices also have been linked to the development of executive functioning, which enables goal-oriented responses, planning, inhibitory control, emotion regulation, and responses in ambiguous situations (Fay-Stammbach et al. Citation2014; Gottfredson & Hirschi, Citation1990), Importantly, these functions support cognitive skills such as decision making, academic achievement, and prosocial behavior (Parker et al., Citation2018; Pinquart, Citation2017; Romer et al., Citation2017; Weller et al., Citation2015).

Parental monitoring/involvement

One specific set of parenting behaviors that has garnered considerable attention is the degree to which parents monitor, and are involved with, the lives of their children. Parental monitoring/involvement is typically defined by behaviors such as knowing their child’s whereabouts, their friends, and the activities that interest the child. Lower reported parental monitoring/involvement has been associated with a range of risk behaviors in adolescents, including, antisociality, deviant peer association, substance use, and health-risking sexual behavior (Barnes et al., Citation2006; Bogenschneider et al., Citation1998; Borawski et al., Citation2003; Brown et al., Citation1993; Commendador, Citation2010; Cutrín et al., Citation2019, Citation2022; Hoeve et al., Citation2009; Kerr et al., Citation2010; S. Miller et al., Citation2008; Parkes, et al., Citation2011; Roche et al. Citation2005). Parental monitoring/involvement has been shown to be associated with increased parent-child cohesion, and communication, which is believed to help transmit values to children that may steer them away from problematic risk behaviors (e.g., Ceballo et al., Citation2003; Garbarino, Citation1999).

Parental control behaviours

Another aspect of parenting practices relates to controlling a child’s behavior. One way of achieving this goal is by utilizing proactive and direct actions, such as firm rule setting and consistent enforcement of rules (Grolnick et al., Citation1997). These practices, which we refer to as firm control, are believed to introduce and maintain structure in a child’s environment, by establishing clear and consistent rules for behavior. Such structure not only provides a predictable environment for a child but also direct feedback about decisions and actions that the child makes (Grolnick & Pomerantz, Citation2009). Children who perceive a greater degree of firm structure, and a consistent absence of harsh or coercive control behaviors, tend to show greater levels of self-control, achievement-motivations, and academic performance (Li et al., Citation2019; Rodríguez-Meirinhos et al., Citation2020; Wilder, Citation2014).

Psychological control

Direct, proactive behavioral control practices can be contrasted against indirect behavioral control parenting practices that aim to internally control the child, often referred to as psychological control (Barber, Citation1996). Providing structure via rulemaking and enforcement needs to be considered separate from coercive and overbearing attempts to control a child’s behavior, the latter which can hinder a child’s psychosocial development (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, Citation2010). A psychologically controlling parent will attempt to regulate child behaviors employing parental behaviors such as withdrawing love when a child has broken a rule, shaming a child for his/her actions, and instilling guilt or anxiety. Soenens and Vansteenkiste (Citation2010) highlight that whereas parental monitoring/involvement may promote competence and achievement goals, whereas psychological controlling parenting may hinder a child’s goal development (Barber, Citation1996; Lau et al., Citation2016). In addition, these manipulative, indirect control tactics are believed to hinder the development of a space that is supportive of a child’s increasing autonomy needs, which in turn, promotes the ability for a child to make choices for themselves (vs. dependence on others; Grolnick & Pomerantz, Citation2009). Further, imposing parental values through psychological control techniques, irrespective of a child’s own needs and values, can lead to disturbances in identity formation during adolescence and beyond, marked by low commitment-making and high levels of indecisiveness related to the roles and identities they pursue (Luyckx et al., Citation2007; Vansteenkiste, Zhou, et al., Citation2005).

Psychological control has been shown to have profound associations with a child’s psychosocial well-being as they develop, especially in the development of internalizing disorders (Barber, Citation1996; Soenens & Vansteenkiste, Citation2010). Parental psychological control also is related to delinquency, externalizing problems, academic difficulties, lower executive functioning, risky sexual behavior, and poor peer adjustment from childhood through emerging adulthood (Bean et al., Citation2003; Bernier et al., Citation2010; Faherty et al., Citation2020; Hoeve et al., Citation2009; Karavasilis et al., Citation2003; Kincaid et al., Citation2011; Lanza et al., Citation2013; Liga et al., Citation2017; Nelson & Crick, Citation2002; Roche et al. Citation2005; Sidze & Defo, Citation2013). More broadly, psychologically controlling environments may impact learning and performance. For instance, Vansteenkiste et al. (Citation2004) found that psychologically controlled learning contexts reduced individuals’ levels of persistence on tasks, depth of information processing, and task performance. In another study, Vansteenkiste, Simons, et al. (Citation2005) found that parental psychological control inhibited conceptual learning for 12-year-old children. Taken together, psychological control has the potential to not only impact a child’s well-being and adjustment but also the underlying mechanisms that may support the development of competent decision skills.

Individual differences in decision-making competence

Historically, researchers in the behavioral decision-making (BDM) literature have examined decision-making quality by means of comparing an individual’s choice to that which would be predicted by a normative model of rationality, such as Expected Utility Theory (EUT; Von Neumann & Morganstern, 1944). Because these latter choices are ones that would be made by a completely rational actor, observing an individual’s deviations have been instrumental in understanding the bounds of human thought processes. Specifically, such comparisons between how people should answer, and how they actually do, helps illuminate when and why one fails to respond rationally. However, it is important to note that, although choices predicted by these normative models, and other similar standards like probability theory, provide mathematical, logical benchmarks for the quality/correctness of a decision, they may not always lead to a desired outcome. Nonetheless, consistently applying these principles is presumed to lead to better choices over the long run. Thus, from this perspective, what makes a “good” or “quality” decision is evaluated by the processes leading to the choice, rather than the choice’s consequence (e.g., Keren & de Bruin, Citation2003; Vlek, Citation1984).

Several batteries have been developed to quantify such individual differences in the tendency to violate normative principles (Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Stanovich, Citation2016). One such measure/approach, referred to as decision-making competence (DMC; Bruine de Bruin et al., Citation2007; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005), tests performance on decision-making paradigms commonly encountered in the decision-making literature, such as resistance to framing and over/under-confidence effects. These paradigms sample skills that have been identified as facilitating decision quality (Edwards, Citation1954; Finucane & Lees, Citation2005; Raiffa, Citation1968), including (a) judging outcome likelihoods (belief assessment), (b) evaluating the outcomes themselves (value assessment), (c) appropriately combining of beliefs and values to arrive at a choice (integration), and (d) being self-aware of the limits of one’s knowledge and abilities (metacognition; for further discussion, see Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005).

Across these skills, DMC can be evaluated in terms of either consistency of judgements (e.g., Do preferences on one problem contradict those on another?), or accuracy, in cases where an objectively correct answer can be determined (Dawes & Hastie, Citation2001). For instance, well-articulated values should be insensitive to irrelevant changes in the decision context (Fischhoff, Citation1991; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005). The invariance axiom of EUT (Von Neumann & Morganstern, 1944), states that a decision maker’s preferences should not depend on the way objectively equivalent information is described or framed. However, individuals’ judgments often shift because of how an attribute is presented, violating this axiom by showing an inconsistency of responses across differently-framed, but identical, choices—thus, suggestive of lower value assessment skills (Kahneman & Tversky, Citation1984; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005). To illustrate, Levin and Gaeth (Citation1988) found that individuals rated ground beef as healthier and less greasy when labeled as 75% lean, compared to when it was labeled as 25% fat. Similarly, consistency for belief assessments can also be evaluated by violations of probability rules when making risk judgements. For instance, judging that the probability that an event in a subset, e.g., “What is the chance that you will get a cavity in 1 year?” cannot exceed the probability of its proper superset (e.g., “What is the chance that you will get a cavity in 5 years?; Bruine de Bruin et al., Citation2007; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005).

DMC can be assessed by accuracy as well, such as by examining the degree to which one makes optimal decisions by applying external decision rules or demonstrating a level of confidence in judgments commensurate with one’s ability (i.e., over/underconfidence). For example, someone who scores 60% on a 10-item test should report an average level of confidence of 60%. However, individuals often miscalibrate, leading to biased judgments (McGraw et al., Citation2004). Similarly, judging whether a statement is true or the probability of events occurring plays a role in normative decision theory (Camerer, Citation1992; Fischhoff & Beyth-Marom, Citation1983). In this regard, accurately recognizing peers’ beliefs and social norms can also be considered a test of DMC (see Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005).

Individual differences in DMC performance have been observed in youth, adolescent, and adult samples, conforming to a single latent trait (Bruine de Bruin et al., Citation2007; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Parker et al., Citation2018; Weller et al., Citation2012, Citation2021), and has been recovered using cross-cultural versions of the battery (e.g., Bavolar, Citation2013; Çevik et al., Citation2019). Further, research has suggested that lower DMC is associated with a variety of risk behaviors, interpersonal difficulties, and other disadvantageous decision processes across the lifespan (Bruine de Bruin et al., Citation2007; Ceschi et al., Citation2017; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Parker et al., Citation2018; Weller, Ceschi, et al., Citation2015; Weller, Moholy, et al., Citation2015). For both youths and adults, low DMC scores have been associated with both lower executive function and self-regulation, which are believed to support advantageous decision making (del Missier et al., Citation2012; Garofalo et al., Citation2021; Parker et al., Citation2018; Weller et al., Citation2012, Citation2015, Citation2021). Moreover, DMC also has been associated with environmental and home variables, such as neighborhood disadvantage, household socioeconomic status, and the presence of parental substance use (Parker et al., Citation2018; Weller et al., Citation2021), suggesting the impact of broader environmental influences on its subsequent development.

The current study

In the current study, we examine the degree to which earlier parenting practices are associated with later instantiations of decision-making competence during late adolescence/emerging adulthood. We examined this research question with a longitudinal study assessing parenting practices for mother and father separately, measured at both ages 10–12 and again 2 years later at age 12–14, whereas DMC performance was measured at age 19. We predicted that lower reported parental monitoring & involvement, lax parental control, and greater psychological control would be independently associated with lower DMC, accounting for relevant covariates (i.e., neighborhood disadvantage, presence of parental Substance Use Disorder; SUD).

Method

Participants

This study involved secondary data analysis. Participants in the original study were recruited from a longitudinal study from the Center for Education and Drug Abuse Research (CEDAR). CEDAR participating families included the biological father, mother, and a son or daughter who was between 10 and 12 years of age at study entry. Families were recruited from diverse sources to maximize representation across multiple planned subgroups. CEDAR, a longitudinal family/high-risk study of etiology of substance use disorder (SUD) recruited probands who, in this study, were adult males with or without a lifetime DSM-III-R diagnosis (DSM-IV was introduced after the study started) of SUD consequent to use of illicit drugs (SUD + and SUD−, respectively), who had a 10–12-year-old biological child (Tarter & Vanyukov Citation2001). The SUD + probands were recruited from substance abuse treatment programs, social service agencies, newspaper and radio advertisements, public service announcements, and random digit telephone calls. The SUD − men were recruited using the same method as SUD + probands except that none were acquired from treatment facilities. This study ran from 1990 until 2014, was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pittsburgh (IRB0107007), adult participants provided written informed consent and minor children provided informed assent prior to implementing the research protocol. Participants were compensated for their time at the end of each assessment at a rate comparable to the U.S. minimum wage.

The protocol included a comprehensive assessment, including SUD and other mental disorder diagnoses of the parents and children, as well as collateral information about the children from one parent and the child’s teacher. At baseline (T1), the study families included 344 fathers with SUD positive (SUD+) status, 350 fathers with SUD negative (SUD-) status, and 81 fathers who met the criteria for a DSM-IV Axis 1 disorder, but not for SUD. Although it was not part of the recruitment plan, Mother’s SUD + status was also collected (n = 187). Collectively, 31.4% of the sample had one parent with SUD + diagnosis, and 18.4% with two SUD + parents. CEDAR has the following sample sizes, indicated by the number of families: T1 (ages 10–12 years): n = 775; Time 2, 2 years following baseline (T2; age 12–14): n = 653; Time 3(T3; age 19): n = 580. For the follow-up assessments, CEDAR maintained a 78–89% follow-up success rate between successive visits (M = 84%). Attrition was not progressive; subjects missing a follow-up visit commonly returned for subsequent visits. For the current study, the sample consisted of 70.7% Males, 75.5% Caucasian, 21.8% African American, 2.7% Other/did not report). Children in the study had a median number of one sibling in the household at age 10–12. Household income was not directly assessed; however, the median household income of participants’ 1990 US Census tract was $32,488; thus, approximately 5.7% of participants lived in a census tract that was below the 1990 poverty level of $13,359).

Measures

As part of the larger longitudinal study, participants completed the following measures:

Parenting practices

Parental monitoring/involvement

Children completed the Parental Supervision/Involvement Scale (Loeber et al., Citation1989) at both visits. Thirteen of these items asked the child solely about their father’s monitoring/involvement behaviors, and 13 items asked solely about their mother’s behaviors. These items assessed perceived supervision for each parent separately (e.g., “Does your mother/father know who you are with when you are not home?” with responses coded on a 3-point Likert-type scale from “Almost always” to “Almost never”), family talk (e.g., “When was the last time that you discussed with mom/dad your plans for the coming day?” with responses coded on a 4-point Likert-type scale from “more than a month ago” to “Yesterday/Today”), family activities (e.g., “How often do you have a friendly chat with your mom/dad?” with responses coded on a 3-point Likert-type scale from “Almost never” to “Often”), set time to be home (e.g., “Do you have a set time to be home on a school night?” with responses coded on a 3-point scale from “Never” to “Always”). A composite scale for monitoring/involvement was created for these items by averaging the z-scored items.

Children’s report on parental behavior inventory (CRPBI)

At T1 and T2, children rated their parent’s behavior on a three-point scale (1 = very true; 2 = somewhat true; 3 = not at all true). Each parent was assessed separately (Schludermann & Schludermann, Citation1970) on the following subscales.

Psychological Control assesses the use of indirect means of control through guilt (8 items) or instilling anxiety (8 items). Statements such as “My father/mother feels hurt when I don’t follow his/her advice” represent the construct of control through guilt, while statements such as “My father/mother thinks and talks about my bad behaviour long after it is over” measure instilling anxiety.

Firm (vs. Lax) Control assesses the degree to which a child perceives that a parent regulates and monitors his/her behavior by establishing firm rules (8 items) and their subsequent enforcement (8 items). An example item of firm control is “My father/mother makes me do my homework” and an item measuring rules enforcement is “My father/mother makes sure I obey even if I complain or protest.”

We derived composite scales, representing three specific parenting dimensions—Parental Monitoring/Involvement, Psychological Control, and Firm Control. At both T1 and T2, the correlations between child reports on mother and father were strongly correlated for these composites (Mean r = .74 and .59, respectively). Subsequently, for each of the parenting dimensions, we created a mean composite parental scale, averaging mother and father ratings within each wave for each dimension (see online Supplementary information).Footnote1

Youth decision-making competence (Y-DMC)

At the age 19 (T3) assessment, participants completed the Y-DMC (see Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Parker et al., Citation2018 for details about the Y-DMC measure and its scoring). The Y-DMC measures normative responding through five tasks: Applying Decision Rules, Resistance to Framing, Over/underconfidence, Consistency in Risk Perception, and Recognizing Social Norms.Footnote2

Applying Decision Rules assessed the ability to follow a prescribed decision rule to make a correct choice from several options in a multi-attribute matrix (adapted from Payne et al., Citation1993). An example is making a purchase for which the subject must decide which of 3 stores is best based on ratings of 4 features. There were seven items in total; thus, the Applying Decision Rules score reflects the number of correct applications of the specified decision rule given (0–7).

Resistance to Framing evaluates insensitivity to irrelevant information in a decision problem across five decision pairs. These items tested an individual’s tendency to make consistent judgements across two differently presented, but objectively identical, decision problems. The Resistance to Framing score equals the number of consistent choices across these problem pairs (0–5).

Over/Underconfidence tests the degree to which an individual’s confidence is calibrated to their knowledge on a variety of questions of general knowledge (e.g., “A robin’s eggs are orange”), sex, and HIV/AIDS (e.g., “You can usually tell if a person has HIV/AIDS”), and drugs/alcohol (e.g., “Drinking coffee makes a drunk person think more clearly”). Participants indicated if the statement was true or false and the probability that their answer is correct (i.e., their confidence level). An individual’s degree of overconfidence is calculated as one minus the absolute difference between mean confidence and percentage correct across items so that higher scores reflect better performance.

Consistency in Risk Perception assesses an individual’s ability to judge risk likelihood in a consistent, rational manner. Participants were asked to evaluate the likelihood of events occurring in their lives (e.g., getting pregnant or getting someone pregnant; using marijuana) and were asked to respond with the probability of the given event occurring to them in specific time frame (1 year vs. 5 years) on a scale from 0% indicating “no chance” to 100% indicating “certainty.” Likelihood ratings for a 1-year risk judgment that exceeded a 5-year risk judgment would demonstrate low consistency. A response was coded as correct response when the participant’s perception that an event occurring more proximally was no larger than the probability of the same event occurring later.

Recognizing Social Norms assesses the concordance between an individual’s judgment of normative behavior across a range of behaviors such as being on time for an appointment or drinking and driving with the perceived endorsement rate of others who are the same age (i.e., the sample endorsement rate). A Recognizing Social Norms score was calculated by the within-person rank order correlation (-1 to +1) between estimated social norms and the actual social norm endorsement rate of the sample.

Covariates

For the current study, we focus on three potential covariates. Child’s sex (male = 0; female =1) was included because of reported differences in emerging adult risk behavior (Byrnes et al., Citation1999; Chapple & Johnson, Citation2007). Parental SUD + and neighborhood disadvantage are predictors of youth risk behaviors (Ridenour et al, Citation2009), and previously have been shown to account for variance in DMC over time (Weller et al., Citation2021). Moreover, parental SUD + is a robust predictor of negative parenting practices, such as inconsistent supervision and discipline, and conflictual interactions (Dishion et al., Citation1999; Latendresse et al., Citation2008; Tarter et al., Citation2003; El-Sheikh & Flanagan, Citation2001). Assessed at age 10–12, Parental SUD+ status indicated the number of a child’s parents who met DSM-IV criteria for a SUD, as assessed by an expanded Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; Leckman et al., Citation1982).Footnote3

Neighborhood disadvantage (Ross & Mirowsky, Citation2001) is a metric based on United States census tract-level information regarding the family’s surrounding community, such as the percent of households living below poverty, single-parent households,Footnote4 the number of adults without higher education/degrees, and residences not inhabited by the owner. This variable represents a broad indicator of community disadvantage across various domains, including relative economic and educational disadvantage within a participants’ community, rather than an indicator of any one family’s own level of disadvantage. A constant was added to the index to make the score originate at zero. Higher values reflect greater disadvantage.

Data analytic plan

To handle missing data, we conducted a multiple imputation procedure in SPSS 28.0. Prior to the imputation procedure, a random seed was set using the Random Number Generator option with a Mersene Twister. Variable values were constrained to plausible values. We used the Automatic procedure in SPSS which optimally choses either a Markov-Chain Monte Carlo or a Monotone imputation based on the analysis of missing patterns. Five imputed datasets were derived from this procedure.

Composite variables were then calculated. Analyses commenced with correlation analyses, and the pooled results were reported. For the correlational analysis only, a composite score for DMC was created by calculated by regression-based factor scores of a one-factor principal components analysis for the DMC tasks. To test the prospective associations between parenting practices and later DMC performance, we conducted a structural equation model (SEM) analysis using robust maximum likelihood using MPlus 8.7 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–Citation2017). DMC performance was modeled as a latent variable (i.e., measurement model), whereas the predictor variables were treated as observed variables. Direct paths between the covariates and the main variables of interest (parenting at T1 and DMC) were also included in the model. We used the IMPUTATION function to conduct the SEM with the imputed data. With this method, parameter estimates, and standard errors reported represent the averages across analyses of the five imputed data sets (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–Citation2017).

Attrition analyses

Prior to these analyses, attrition analyses first were conducted to test for any differences in parenting practices, DMC scores, and covariates between participants who took part in both T1 and T2 assessments to those with only T1 assessment (n = 118). With the exception that participants who had parent with a SUD + diagnosis were more likely to miss the T2 assessment than families without a SUD + parent (19.1% vs 11.3%; χ2 =8.91, p <.01), we found no significant differences for either parenting practices or the DMC composite score (all p values >.10, mean p = .62).

Results

Associations between parenting variables, DMC and covariates

reports the correlations between the study variables. As expected, we found that each of the parenting variables demonstrated moderate stability across visits, ranging from r = .36–.61. At both visits, the number of parents with SUD + status was significantly associated with lower monitoring/involvement, and greater psychological control. Greater neighborhood disadvantage was associated with greater psychological control at both visits and negatively associated with firm control and monitoring/involvement at T2.

Table 1. Pooled correlations for study variables.

Children with more SUD + parents scored lower on the Y-DMC measure. We also found that lower Y-DMC total scores were associated with greater neighborhood disadvantage. Y-DMC had no significant correlations with child’s sex. Children who reported lower psychological control, at both T1 and T2 performed better on the Y-DMC. Greater reported parental monitoring/involvement at T2 was associated with greater Y-DMC scores, and this association was significantly stronger than at T1, Z = 3.14, p<.01 (based on formula to compare dependent correlations; Steiger, Citation1980). Firm (vs. Lax) control was positively associated with Y-DMC scores at T1, but not T2. However, this effect size was small (r = .10), and the difference between T1 and T2 correlations with DMC performance was not significant, Z = .99, p = .32. Additionally, we tested the degree to which separate reports for mother and father were differentially associated with DMC scores. As shown in , the observed correlations for mother and father ratings were remarkably similar, and in the same direction. This finding, and the observed strong correlations between parental ratings, lead us to maintain the strategy of aggregating parental ratings for the main analysis (see online Supplementary Information SI1).

Table 2. Associations between DMC and parenting practices, mother and father separately.

SEM model

Given the stability of the parenting practices across assessments, we combined ratings for each parenting practice prior to conducting the analysis (parallel analyses can be found in online Supplementary Information for each assessment visit). The model fit the data well, mean CFI = .947, TLI = .926, RMSEA = .031. shows the results of the SEM analysis which tested the degree to which parenting practices, along with covariates, accounted for variance in the latent Y-DMC variable (see also for unstandardized estimates).Footnote5 We found that all DMC indicators significantly loaded on the latent factor, confirming that the included indicators reasonably conform to a single-factor solution. Next, we examined the degree to which the covariates independently contributed to the variance in DMC performance. Holding other variables constant, we found that only neighborhood disadvantage was significantly associated with DMC scores, with greater disadvantage being related to lower scores. With respect to parenting practices, we found that only greater psychological control was inversely associated with lower DMC scores, holding other variables constant. No other parenting variable accounted for variance in DMC scores.

Figure 1. SEM analysis results.

Figure 1. SEM analysis results.

Table 3. Parameter estimates-SEM.

Discussion

A growing body of research suggests that individual differences in decision-making competence are associated with both advantageous and disadvantageous outcomes (Parker et al., Citation2018; Parker & Fischhoff, Citation2005; Weller, Moholy, et al., Citation2015). However, less is known about antecedent variables that are associated with the development of these skills. The current study found that higher levels of psychologically controlling parenting during childhood and early adolescence were associated with lower decision-making performance later in life. These findings support past research emphasizing the importance of parenting on a child’s social and cognitive development (Lucassen et al., Citation2015).

Specifically, we found evidence that parental psychological control practices were associated with a child’s difficulty with making normative decisions 9 years later. This effect was found even after controlling for other parenting practices, as well as broad-reaching environmental and familial variables such as neighborhood disadvantage and parental SUD + status. These results support previous research which emphasizes the importance of instilling autonomy, rather than exerting excessive psychological control when raising children. Autonomy-granting has been found to be especially important, as it gives a child the freedom to make their own guided decisions and to form their own goals (Soenens & Vansteenkiste, Citation2010). Further, autonomy promotes intrinsic motivation and contributes to improved well-being and academic success (Lekes et al., Citation2010).

We speculate that these associations between parenting practices and decision-making may, in part, occur by shaping global dispositions in a child’s self-regulatory tendencies. The association between traits related to disinhibition and self-control has been well-documented related to increases in risk behavior and suboptimal decision-making (e.g., Lauriola & Weller, Citation2018). Numerous studies have found that psychological control has been associated with lower self-control and emotional regulation (Finkenauer et al., Citation2005; Houtepen et al Citation2019; Manzeske & Dopkins Stright, Citation2009). In a recent meta-analysis, Pinquart (Citation2017) reported that psychological control showed the strongest effect sizes with self-control, relative to other parenting practices. Subsequently, self-control is believed to mediate relationships between psychological control and later outcomes, such as the development of internalizing disorders and interpersonal competencies (Barber, Citation1996; Finkenauer et al., Citation2005; Moilanen & Manuel, Citation2017). Additionally, psychological control has been implicated in externalizing behaviors, such as aggression, delinquency, and behavioral addictions (Faherty et al., Citation2020; Finkenauer et al., Citation2005; Lansford et al. Citation2014; Pinquart, Citation2017). Notably, in a study that leveraged the same sample as the current one, Weller et al. (Citation2021) found that youth with persistently elevated levels of psychological dysregulation, measured by an index including indicators of both self-control and negative affectivity-related tendencies, was associated with substantially lower DMC scores than their more self-regulated peers (c.f., Garofalo et al., Citation2021).

Contrary to our predictions, we did not find strong evidence for associations between other parenting variables and DMC performance. Greater levels of parental monitoring/involvement have been shown to provide a protective factor against health-risking and externalizing behaviors, in both cross-sectional and longitudinal designs (Bacchini et al., Citation2011, Barnes et al., Citation2006; Brody, Citation2003; Pilgrim et al., Citation2006; Siebenbruner et al., Citation2006). Whilst we did observe a small, but significant zero-order correlation between T2 monitoring/involvement and Y-DMC scores, it did not significantly account for DMC performance, holding other variables constant. Considering that the correlation between DMC performance and monitoring/involvement became stronger from T1 to T2, it is possible that more proximal assessments may suggest more prominent associations.

Similarly, we did not find strong evidence for associations between firm control and DMC performance. However, it must be noted that the scale used in the current study did not distinguish between firm punishments and excessively harsh ones per se, the latter of which would presumably be more detrimental (Pinquart, Citation2017). Parental rigidity and the enforcement of harsh punishments are considered characteristic of an authoritarian parenting style, which has been implicated in the development of both internalizing and externalizing disorders (Baumrind, Citation1967, Citation1991). Harsh parenting also has been associated with a child’s cognitive abilities. For instance, Lucassen et al. (Citation2015) found that harsh parenting (i.e., physical punishments, calling child names like “lazy” or “stupid,” etc.) was associated with lower levels of a child’s metacognitive abilities, and lower inhibitory self-control. In contrast, the scale used in this current study included non-enforcement of rules and lax parenting styles, without items directly reflecting perceived severity of the rules or punishments.

Additionally, although it was treated as a covariate, it should be noted that greater levels of neighborhood disadvantage predicted lower DMC performance, supporting past research. Mani et al. (Citation2013) found that greater levels of poverty and disadvantage were associated with lower cognitive functioning on tasks like Raven’s Progressive Matrices. Lower socioeconomic status also has previously been associated with lower performance on child-version of the Iowa Gambling Task (Mata et al., Citation2013). Notably, Weller et al. (Citation2021) found that neighborhood disadvantage was associated with lower DMC performance, beyond that explained by a child’s developmental trajectory of self-regulative tendencies. Taken together, these findings reinforce that the broader socio-economic context impacts decision making and should be examined in greater detail from a range of perspectives.

Limitations and future directions

Although this study provides unique insights into the associations between parenting practices and DMC, we must acknowledge several limitations. First, because DMC was only evaluated at one time point, this study is silent with respect to the actual development of these skills, and the degree to which parenting practices impact the developmental trajectory over time. A wide range of research reinforces a “cognitive sophistication” model, in which children’s decision making begin to more closely resemble a normatively rational choice (Toplak & Flora, Citation2021). However, no longitudinal research exists which links parenting with the rate of developmental change toward rational responding, or if parenting may account for heterogeneity in these trajectories. Adding to this complexity, different decision-making skills may follow different trajectories across the lifespan (Romer et al., Citation2017; Strough et al., Citation2015); thus, more research is needed within a developmental context.

This limitation also leaves the current study silent with respect to potential issues of bi-directionality. Specifically, it is possible that parents enact behaviors that are responsive to their adolescents’ decision-making skills. For instance, Li et al. (Citation2019) found that not only can parenting may influence the development of self-control, but self-control may also impact which parenting practices are enacted. Similarly, Billen et al. (Citation2022) found evidence for a bi-directional model between self-control and deviance, suggesting that self-control may not solely temporally precede risk behaviors. Longitudinal designs that include decision-making at each time period can help to further explicate the nature of the associations reported in the present study.

It is important to note that the components included in the DMC measure only represent a subset of decision skills that may indicate normatively rational responding. At its inception, and its introduction into this secondary dataset, it was intended to demonstrate that (a) individual differences in decision-making tendencies could be quantified via different decision-making paradigms encountered in the literature, and (b) these individual differences were associated with outcomes that may indicate poor real-life decision processes. Since then, other measures, such as Stanovich’s (Citation2016) Comprehensive Assessment of Rational Thought, include other components of normatively rational responding, along with self-reported “thinking dispositions” (e.g., superstitious thinking, scientific reasoning) that may support rational choice. One skill not directly measured with the DMC, but particularly relevant, is advantageous decision making when faced with risky choices, or expected value (EV) maximization (i.e., making choices that have the most favorable long-run outcomes).Footnote6 This skill involves integration skills of outcome probabilities and its magnitude; making risky choices when the EV suggests a more favorable option is available has been associated with other poor decision-making skills and neural regions implicated in self-regulation (Parker & Weller, Citation2015; Stanovich, Citation2016; Weller et al., Citation2007). Understanding how parenting practices are associated with risky decision making processes would further illuminate the scope of their potential impact.

With respect to our measures of parenting practices, we note two specific limitations. First, because these data come from an existing longitudinal study and the measures were previously collected, we only had parenting assessments for the age 10–12 and age 14 assessments. Subsequently, parenting practices during middle adolescence were not accounted for, but naturally, could be associated with DMC performance. Second, we acknowledge that some of the parenting constructs have since been expanded since the original assessments. In particular, the constructs of psychological control and autonomy have been elaborated upon further (Soenens et al., Citation2006, Citation2010). Although these scales have strong conceptual overlap with the ones used in this study, the newer scales may offer more refined assessments to further parse the relations between decision-making and specific components of parenting, especially with respect to psychological control.

Finally, we need to acknowledge several potential limitations with the sample. Although this sample includes a range of participants in terms of ethnicity, gender, and socioeconomic backgrounds, the fact that parenting is embedded in the aims of a larger study that was designed to examine associations with paternal SUDs, with only biological parents recruited, leaves other research questions open. Whilst the absence of significant associations between parental SUD + and DMC performance, after controlling for parenting and broader neighborhood disadvantage, provides confidence that the observed associations are not directly related to parental SUD status, we cannot speak to how parenting may differ in households where at least one biological parent is not present, for instance. Additionally, the observed associations with respect to parenting must be considered within the culture in which they are assessed. Shigeto et al. (Citation2019) found that longitudinal associations between psychological control and depressive symptoms was similar for Latinx and non-Latinx families, but the associations between behavioral control (measured with items from the CRPBI Firm Control scale), differed in sign across cultural background. In another study across twelve cultural groups, Lansford et al. (Citation2018) found that the normativeness of parenting practices across cultures moderated effects between youth adjustment and parenting (c.f., Deater-Deckard & Dodge, Citation1997). As parenting practices vary across cultures, these associations should be assessed and examined with other populations to help gain a more global representation of these associations.

Overall, these findings may have implications for prevention and intervention purposes. Specifically, this research reinforces the importance of environmental factors, especially parenting. Knowing how parenting may be associated with decision-making abilities is important for both identifying children who may be susceptible to making maladaptive choices later in life, as well as potentially developing interventions that may contribute to improved decision-making skills. At the school level, interventions of this kind have been tested with positive preliminary results. Jacobson et al. (Citation2012) found that adding a decision-making skills supplementary component to a high school history course not only improved grades, but also improved later decision-making abilities. Similarly, Weller et al. (Citation2015) found that a family-based, skill building intervention (given at age 11) improved later integration skills for risky choices (i.e., making choices consistent with expected value, also related to DMC; Parker & Weller, Citation2015), measured at age 16, for maltreated adolescent girls in foster care, compared both to girls receiving foster care services as usual, and non-maltreated peers. It is likely that interventions that target both decision skills and supporting competencies like self-control and parent-child communication, will yield maximum benefits.

In summary, these findings contribute to and support past decision-making research that has highlighted parenting and cognitive abilities. The developmental period under study corresponds with a transition from middle-school to high school in many education systems. This transition can be particularly difficult for children, especially those in vulnerable groups, not only in terms of navigating new academic challenges but also social ones (Kim & Leve, Citation2011). It is also a time in which health-risking behaviors first manifest (Reyna & Farley, Citation2006). By showing that parenting practices are associated with later decision-making tendencies, the current study provides insights into underlying mechanisms that may link parenting to the development of problem behaviors (Barber et al., Citation2005; Dishion et al., Citation1991; Patterson & Stouthamer-Loeber, Citation1984). Recognition of such antecedents may lead to the development of interventions that target ways to reduce risk behaviors, as well as highlight ways that parents can aid in improving their children’s decision skills that may promote long-term social, financial, and health outcomes.

Authors contributions

All authors contributed to the design of the study. J. Weller, L. Michaels, A. Parker, and M. Reynolds conducted the literature review. M. Reynolds and L. Kirisci developed the dataset from the existing longitudinal study. J. Weller and L. Kirisci conducted the statistical analyses. J.A. Weller wrote the first draft of the manuscript and all authors contributed to and have approved the final manuscript. Early analyses of these data were conducted by L. Michaels as part of an undergraduate honors thesis.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (72.7 KB)

Supplemental Material

Download PDF (72.6 KB)

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Steve Knopf, University of Pittsburgh, for his work with data administration.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available through upon reasonable request to Joshua Weller (email: [email protected]).

Additional information

Funding

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support from the Center for Education and Drug Abuse Research (CEDAR), and especially Steve Knopf and Ralph Tarter. This research was supported in part by the National Institute on Drug Abuse [P50 DA05605, R01 DA033322]. NIDA had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication. The views expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of these funders.

Notes

1 Because we created a composite variable for parenting practices, we considered the possibility that such a composite could mask discordant parenting across children. To address this point, we calculated an intraclass correlation for the parenting variables used as a measure of profile similarity. This variable did not significantly correlate with DMC subscale scores. Further, we conducted an OLS regression to test whether it might potentially impact the associations between our study variables and DMC. These results yielded no substantive differences from the results reported, and thus not discussed further.

2 We did not include another task, Resistance to Sunk Costs, into this measure, as it only included two items that did not correlate with the other scales (Parker et al., Citation2018).

3 This version has been superseded by DSM-V; however, the diagnostic criteria for SUD + has not substantively changed in any way that would impact the results of this study.

4 The neighborhood disadvantage index adds the prevalence of poverty and of mother-only households and subtracts the prevalence of home ownership and college educated residents in the respondent's Census tract. For more information, see Ross and Mirowsky (Citation2001).

5 We also explored the possibility of interaction effects between parenting variables and both neighborhood disadvantage and parental SUD in a parallel model. No interaction effects were found, all p > .45. Thus, we do not further consider this issue.

6 The expected value of a choice is expressed by the equation, EV = ∑ Probabilityi *Outcomei. Thus, for a choice with a 50% chance to win 100, otherwise win 0; the EV of the choice = 50.

References

  • Arnett, J. J. (1992). Reckless behavior in adolescence: A developmental perspective. Developmental Review, 12(4), 339–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(92)90013-R
  • Arnett, J. J. (2015). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. Oxford University Press.
  • Bacchini, D., Miranda, M. C., & Affuso, G. (2011). Effects of parental monitoring and exposure to community violence on antisocial behavior and anxiety/depression among adolescents. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26(2), 269–292. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260510362879
  • Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 3296–3319. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131780
  • Barber, B. K., Stolz, H. E., & Olsen, J. A. (2005). Parental support, psychological control, and behavioral control: Assessing relevance across time, method, and culture. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70(4), 1–137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2005.00365.x
  • Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., Welte, J. W., Farrell, M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2006). Effects of parental monitoring and peer deviance on substance use and delinquency. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68(4), 1084–1104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00315.x
  • Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75(1), 43–88.
  • Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting styles on adolescent competence and substance use. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 11(1), 56–95. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431691111004
  • Bavolar, J. (2013). Validation of the adult decision-making competence in Slovak students. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(3), 386–392. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500006057
  • Bean, R. A., Bush, K. R., McKenry, P. C., & Wilson, S. M. (2003). The impact of parental support, behavioral control, and psychological control on the academic achievement and self-esteem of African American and European American adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18(5), 523–541. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558403255070
  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S., & Whipple, N. (2010). From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development, 81(1), 326–339. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558403255070
  • Billen, E., Garofalo, C., Weller, J. A., Kirisci, L., Reynolds, M., Tarter, R. E., & Bogaerts, S. (2022). Bidirectional associations between self-regulation and deviance from adolescence to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 34(1), 335–344. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000656
  • Bogenschneider, K., Wu, M. Y., Raffaelli, M., & Tsay, J. C. (1998). Parent influences on adolescent peer orientation and substance use: The interface of parenting practices and values. Child Development, 69(6), 1672–1688. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06184.x
  • Borawski, E., Ievers-Landis, C., Lovegreen, L., & Trapl, E. (2003). Parental monitoring, negotiated unsupervised time, and parental trust: The Role of perceived parenting practices in adolescent health risk behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 33(2), 60–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(03)00100-9
  • Brody, G. H. (2003). Parental monitoring: Action and reaction. In A. C. Crouter & A. Booth (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics: The neglected side of family relationships (pp. 163–169) Erlbaum.
  • Brody, G. H., & Ge, X. (2001). Linking parenting processes and self-regulation to psychological functioning and alcohol use during early adolescence. Journal of Family Psychology, 15(1), 82–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.15.1.82
  • Brown, B., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting practices and peer group affiliation in adolescence. Child Development, 64(2), 467–482. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131263
  • Bruine de Bruin, W., Fischhoff, B., & Parker, A. (2007). Individual differences in adult decision-making competence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(5), 938–956. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.5.938
  • Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 125(3), 367–383. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367
  • Camerer, C. F. (1992). Recent tests of generalizations of expected utility theory. In W. Edwards (Ed.), Utility theories: Measurements and applications (pp. 207–251). Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Ceballo, R., Ramirez, C., Hearn, K. D., & Maltese, K. L. (2003). Community violence and children’s psychological well-being: Does parental monitoring Matter? Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 32(4), 586–592. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3204_11
  • Ceschi, A., Demerouti, E., Sartori, R., & Weller, J. (2017). Decision-making processes in the workplace: How exhaustion, lack of resources and job demands impair them and affect performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 313. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00313
  • Çevik, Y. D., Doğan, N., Dağhan, G., Mumcu, F. K., Somyürek, S., & Karaman, H. (2019). Validation of pre-adolescent decision-making competence in Turkish students. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(3), 364–372. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500004393
  • Chapple, C. L., & Johnson, K. A. (2007). Gender differences in impulsivity. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5(3), 221–234. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204007301286
  • Cheung, N. W. T., & Cheung, Y. W. (2010). Strain, self-control, and gender differences in delinquency among Chinese adolescents: Extending general strain theory. Sociological Perspectives, 53(3), 321–345. https://doi.org/10.1525/sop.2010.53.3.321
  • Commendador, K. (2010). The relationship between maternal parenting style, female adolescent decision making, and contraceptive use. Journal of the American Academy of Nurse Practitioners, 23(10), 561–572. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-7599.2011.00635.x
  • Cutrín, O., Maneiro, L., Sobral, J., & Gómez-Fraguela, J. A. (2019). Longitudinal validation of a new measure to assess parental knowledge a d its sources in Spanish adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(5), 1220–1235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01366-z
  • Cutrín, O., Maneiro, L., Chowdhury, Y., Kulis, S. S., Marsiglia, F. F., & Gómez Fraguela, J. A. (2022). Longitudinal associations between parental support and parental knowledge on behavioral and emotional problems in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 51(6), 1169–1180. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-021-01559-0
  • Dahl, R. (2004). Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1308.001
  • Dawes, R. M., & Hastie, R. (2001). Rational choice in an uncertain world: The psychology of judgment and decision making. Sage Publications.
  • Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological Inquiry, 8(3), 161–175. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0803_1
  • del Missier, F., Mantyla, T., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2012). Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 331–351. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.731
  • Dishion, T. J., Capaldi, D. M., & Yoerger, K. (1999). Middle childhood antecedents to progressions in male adolescent substance use: An ecological analysis of risk and protection. Journal of Adolescent Research, 14(2), 175–205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0743558499142003
  • Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., Stoolmiller, M., & Skinner, M. L. (1991). Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 172–180. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.1.172
  • Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychological Bulletin, 51(4), 380–417. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0053870
  • El-Sheikh, M., & Flanagan, E. (2001). Parental problem drinking and children’s adjustment: Family conflict and parental depression as mediators and moderators of risk. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(5), 417–432. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010447503252
  • Faherty, A. N., Lowe, K., & Arnett, J. J. (2020). Mind games: Parental psychological control and emerging adults’ adjustment. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(2), 695–714. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407519877240
  • Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A Review. Child Development Perspectives, 8(4), 258–264. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12095
  • Feldman, S. S., & Wentzel, K. R. (1990). The relationship between parenting styles, sons’ self-restraint, and peer relations in early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 10(4), 439–454. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431690104002
  • Finkenauer, C., Engels, R., & Baumeister, R. (2005). Parenting behaviour and adolescent behavioural and emotional problems: The role of self-control. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(1), 58–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250444000333
  • Finucane, M. L., & Lees, N. B. (2005). Decision-making competence of older adults: Models and methods (National Council workshop on decision making by older adults, Vol. 46).
  • Fischhoff, B., & Beyth-Marom, R. (1983). Hypothesis evaluation from a Bayesian perspective. Psychological Review, 90(3), 239–260. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.3.239
  • Fischhoff, B. (1991). Value elicitation: Is there anything in there? American Psychologist, 46(8), 835–847. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.8.835
  • Garbarino, J. (1999). Lost boys: Why our sons turn violent and how we can save them. Free Press.
  • Garofalo, C., Weller, J. A., Kirisci, L., & Reynolds, M. D. (2021). Elaborating on the longitudinal measurement invariance and construct validity of the triarchic psychopathy scales from the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment, 33(9), 890–903. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001023
  • Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., & Apostoleris, N. H. (1997). Predictors of parent involvement in children’s schooling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 538–548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.538
  • Grolnick, W. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). Issues and challenges in studying parental control: Toward a new conceptualization. Child Development Perspectives, 3(3), 165–170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00099.x
  • Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford University Press.
  • Hallquist, M. N., Hipwell, A. E., & Stepp, S. D. (2015). Poor self-control and harsh punishment in childhood prospectively predict borderline personality symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 549–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000058
  • Hoeve, M., Dubas, J. S., Eichelsheim, V. I., Van der Laan, P. H., Smeenk, W., & Gerris, J. R. (2009). The relationship between parenting and delinquency: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(6), 749–775. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9310-8
  • Houtepen, J. A. B. M., Sijtsema, J. J., Klimstra, T. A., Van der Lem, R., & Bogaerts, S. (2019). Loosening the reins or tightening them? Complex relationships between parenting, effortful control, and adolescent psychopathology. Child & Youth Care Forum, 48(1), 127–145. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10566-018-9477-7
  • Jacobson, D., Parker, A. M., Spetzler, C., Bruine de Bruin, W., Hollenbeck, K., Heckerman, D., & Fischhoff, B. (2012). Improved learning in U.S. history and decision competence with decision-focused curriculum. PLoS One. 7(9), e45775. PMCID: PMC3448680 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045775
  • Jessor, R. (1987). Problem‐behavior theory, psychosocial development, and adolescent problem drinking. Addiction, 82(4), 331–342. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.1987.tb01490.x
  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39(4), 341–350. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341
  • Karavasilis, L., Doyle, A. B., & Markiewicz, D. (2003). Associations between parenting style and attachment to mother in middle childhood and adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(2), 153–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/0165025024400015
  • Keren, G., & de Bruin, W. B. (2003). On the assessment of decision quality: Considerations regarding utility, conflict and accountability. In D. Hardman and L. Macchi (Eds.), Thinking: Psychological perspectives on reasoning, judgment and decision making. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/047001332X.ch16
  • Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Burk, W. J. (2010). A reinterpretation of parental monitoring in longitudinal perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(1), 39–64. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00623.x
  • Kim, H. K., & Leve, L. D. (2011). Substance use and delinquency among middle school girls in foster care: A three-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(6), 740–750. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025949
  • Kincaid, C., Jones, D. J., Cuellar, J., & Gonzalez, M. (2011). Psychological control associated with youth adjustment and risky behavior in African American single mother families. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 20(1), 102–110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-010-9383-6
  • Kwan, M., Cairney, J., Faulkner, G., & Pullenayegum, E. (2012). Physical activity and other health-risk behaviors during the transition into early adulthood: A Longitudinal cohort study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 42(1), 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2011.08.026
  • Lansford, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Fontaine, R. G., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2014). Peer rejection, affiliation with deviant peers, delinquency, and risky sexual behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43(10), 1742–1751. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-014-0175-y
  • Lansford, J. E., Godwin, J., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D., Bornstein, M. H., Chang, L., Chen, B.-B., Deater-Deckard, K., Di Giunta, L., Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Steinberg, L., Tapanya, S., Alampay, L. P., Uribe Tirado, L. M., & Zelli, A. (2018). Longitudinal associations between parenting and youth adjustment in twelve cultural groups: Cultural normativeness of parenting as a moderator. Developmental Psychology, 54(2), 362–377. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000416
  • Lanza, H. I., Huang, D. Y., Murphy, D. A., & Hser, Y. I. (2013). A latent class analysis of maternal responsiveness and autonomy-granting in early adolescence: Prediction to later adolescent sexual risk-taking. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 33(3), 404–428. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431612445794
  • Latendresse, S. J., Rose, R. J., Viken, R. J., Pulkkinen, L., Kaprio, J., & Dick, D. M. (2008). Parenting mechanisms in links between parents’ and adolescents’ alcohol use behaviors. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 32(2), 322–330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00583.x
  • Lau, K. S., Marsee, M. A., Lapré, G. E., & Halmos, M. B. (2016). Does parental relational aggression interact with parental psychological control in the prediction of youth relational aggression? Deviant Behavior, 37(8), 904–916. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2016.1156981
  • Lauriola, M., & Weller, J. (2018). Beyond daredevils: Risk taking from a temperament perspective. In M. Raue, E. Lermer, & B. Streicher (Eds.), Psychological aspects of risk and risk analysis: Theory, models, and applications. Springer.
  • Leckman, J., Sholomaskas, D., & Thompson, W. (1982). Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: A methodological study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39(8), 879–883. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1982.04290080001001
  • Lekes, N., Gingras, I., Philippe, F., Koestner, R., & Fang, J. (2010). Parental autonomy-support, intrinsic life goals, and well-being among adolescents in China and North America. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(8), 858–869. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9451-7
  • Levin, I. P., & Gaeth, G. J. (1988). How consumers are affected by the framing of attribute information before and after consuming the product. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), 374–378. https://doi.org/10.1086/209174
  • Li, J. B., Willems, Y. E., Stok, F. M., Deković, M., Bartels, M., & Finkenauer, C. (2019). Parenting and self-control across early to late adolescence: A three-level meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(6), 967–1005. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619863046
  • Liga, F., Ingoglia, S., Inguglia, C., Lo Coco, A., Lo Cricchio, M. G., Musso, P., Cheah, C., Rose, L., & Gutow, M. R. (2017). Associations among psychologically controlling parenting, autonomy, relatedness, and problem behaviors during emerging adulthood. The Journal of Psychology, 151(4), 393–415. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2017.1305323
  • Loeber, R. (1989). Values and attitudes towards delinquent behavior. Pittsburgh Youth Study. Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, PA.
  • Lucassen, N., Kok, R., Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Hofman, A., Verhulst, F. C., Lambregtse‐Van den Berg, M. P., & Tiemeier, H. (2015). Executive functions in early childhood: The role of maternal and paternal parenting practices. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 489–505. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12112
  • Luyckx, K., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Goossens, L., & Berzonsky, M. D. (2007). Parental psychological control and dimensions of identity formation in emerging adulthood. Journal of Family Psychology, 21(3), 546–550. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.21.3.546
  • Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E., & Zhao, J. (2013). Poverty impedes cognitive function. Science, 341(6149), 976–980. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1238041
  • Manzeske, D. P., & Dopkins Stright, A. (2009). Parenting styles and emotion regulation: The role of behavioral and psychological control during young adulthood. Journal of Adult Development, 16(4), 223–229. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10804-009-9068-9
  • Mata, F., Miranda, D. M., Sallum, I., Bechara, A., & Malloy-Diniz, L. F. (2013). Do general intellectual functioning and socioeconomic status account for performance on the Children’s Gambling Task? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 68. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00068
  • McGraw, A. P., Mellers, B. A., & Ritov, I. (2004). The affective costs of overconfidence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(4), 281–295. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.472
  • Miller, S., Loeber, R., & Hipwell, A. (2008). Peer deviance, parenting and disruptive behavior among young girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(2), 139–152. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9265-1
  • Moilanen, K. L., & Manuel, M. L. (2017). Parenting, self-regulation and social competence with peers and romantic partners. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 49, 46–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.02.003
  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2017). Mplus user’s guide (8th ed.). Muthén & Muthén.
  • Nelson, D. A., & Crick, N. R. (2002). Parental psychological control: Implications for childhood physical and relational aggression. In B. K. Barber (Ed.), Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children and adolescents (pp. 168–189). APA.
  • O’Connor, T. G. (2002). Annotation: The ‘effects’ of parenting reconsidered: Findings, challenges, and applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 555–572. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00046
  • Parker, A., Bruine de Bruin, W., & Fischhoff, B. (2015). Negative decision outcomes are more common among people with lower decision-making competence: An item-level analysis of the Decision Outcome Inventory (DOI). Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 363. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00363
  • Parker, A. M., Bruine de Bruin, W., Fischhoff, B., & Weller, J. (2018). Robustness of decision‐making competence: Evidence from two measures and an 11‐year longitudinal study. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 31(3), 380–391. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2059
  • Parker, A. M., & Fischhoff, B. (2005). Decision-making competence: External validation through and individual-differences approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.481
  • Parker, A. M., & Weller, J. A. (2015). Greater decision-making competence is associated with greater expected-value sensitivity, but not overall risk taking: An examination of concurrent validity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 717. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00717
  • Parkes, A., Henderson, M., Wight, D., & Nixon, C. (2011). Is parenting associated with teenangers’ early sexual risk-taking, autonomy and relationship with sexual partners? Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health, 43(1), 30–40. https://doi.org/10.1363/4303011
  • Patterson, G. R., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1984). The correlation of family management practices and delinquency. Child Development, 55(4), 1299–1307. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129999
  • Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. Cambridge University Press.
  • Pilgrim, C. C., Schulenberg, J. E., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Johnston, L. D. (2006). Mediators and moderators of parental involvement on substance use: A national study of adolescents. Prevention Science, 7(1), 75–89. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-005-0019-9
  • Pinquart, M. (2017). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 873–932. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000295
  • Raiffa, H. (1968). Decision analysis: Introductory lectures on choices under uncertainty. Addison-Wesley.
  • Reyna, V., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(1), 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1529-1006.2006.00026.x
  • Ridenour, T. A., Tarter, R. E., Reynolds, M., Mezzich, A., Kirisci, L., & Vanyukov, M. (2009). Neurobehavior disinhibition, parental substance use disorder, neighborhood quality and development of cannabis use disorder in boys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 102(1–3), 71–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2009.01.009
  • Roche, K., Mekos, D., Alexander, C., Astone, N., Bandeen-Roche, K., & Ensminger, M. (2005). Parenting influences on early sex initiation among adolescents: How neighborhood matters. Journal of Family Issues, 26(1), 32–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X04265943
  • Rodríguez-Meirinhos, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Oliva, A., Brenning, K., & Antolín-Suárez, L. (2020). When is parental monitoring effective? A person-centered analysis of the role of autonomy-supportive and psychologically controlling parenting in referred and non-referred adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49(1), 352–368. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01151-7
  • Romer, D., Reyna, V. F., & Satterthwaite, T. D. (2017). Beyond stereotypes of adolescent risk taking: Placing the adolescent brain in developmental context. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.07.007
  • Ross, C. E., & Mirowsky, J. (2001). Neighborhood disadvantage, disorder, and health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 42(3), 258–276. https://doi.org/10.2307/3090214
  • Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. (1970). Replicability of factors in Children’s Report of Parent Behavior (CRPBI). The Journal of Psychology, 76(2), 239–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1970.9916845
  • Shigeto, A., Grzywacz, J. G., & Cui, M. (2019). Parental control for Latinas and non-Latinas: Implications for emerging adult mental health. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 28(4), 1071–1083. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01345-4
  • Sidze, E., & Defo, B. (2013). Effects of parenting practices on sexual risk-taking among young people in Cameroon. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-616
  • Siebenbruner, J., Englund, M. M., Egeland, B., & Hudson, K. (2006). Developmental antecedents of late adolescence substance use patterns. Development and Psychopathology, 18(02), 551–571. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579406060287
  • Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Chen, Y. F., Brody, G. H., & Lin, K. H. (2007). Identifying the psychological factors that mediate the association between parenting practices and delinquency. Criminology, 45(3), 481–517. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.2007.00086.x
  • Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the concept of parental psychological control: Proposing new insights on the basis of self-determination theory. Developmental Review, 30(1), 74–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.11.001
  • Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Luyten, P. (2010). Towards a domain-specific approach to the study of parental psychological control: Distinguishing between dependency-oriented and achievement-oriented psychological control. Journal of Personality, 78(1), 217–256. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00614.x
  • Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Luyckx, K., & Goossens, L. (2006). Parenting and adolescent problem behaviors: An integrated model with adolescent self-disclosure and perceived parental knowledge as intervening variables. Developmental Psychology, 42(2), 305–318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.305
  • Stanovich, K. E. (2016). The comprehensive assessment of rational thinking. Educational Psychologist, 51(1), 23–34. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1125787
  • Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002
  • Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87(2), 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245
  • Strough, J., Parker, A. M., & Bruine de Bruin, W. (2015). Understanding life-span developmental changes in decision-making competence. In T. Hess, J. Strough, & C. Löckenhoff (Eds.), Aging and decision making: Empirical and applied perspectives (pp. 235–257). Elsevier Academic Press.
  • Tarter, R. E., & Vanyukov, M. M. (2001). Introduction: Theoretical and operational framework for research into the etiology of substance use disorders. Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance Abuse, 10(4), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1300/J029v10n04_01
  • Tarter, R. E., Kirisci, L., Mezzich, A., Cornelius, J. R., Pajer, K., Vanyukov, M., Gardner, W., Blackson, T., & Clark, D. (2003). Neurobehavior disinhibition in childhood predisposes boys to substance use disorder by young adulthood: Direct and mediated etiologic pathways. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(6), 1078–1085. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078
  • Toplak, M. E., & Flora, D. B. (2021). Resistance to cognitive biases: Longitudinal trajectories and associations with cognitive abilities and academic achievement across development. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 34(3), 344–358. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.2214
  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Rational thinking and cognitive sophistication: Development, cognitive abilities, and thinking dispositions. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 1037–1048. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034910
  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. A., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(2), 246–260. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.246
  • Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Matos, L. (2005). Examining the motivational impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal framing and internally controlling versus autonomy-supportive communication style upon early adolescents’ academic achievement. Child Development, 76(2), 483–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00858.x
  • Vansteenkiste, M., Zhou, M. M., Lens, W., & Soenens, B. (2005). Experiences of autonomy and control among Chinese learners: Vitalizing or immobilizing? Journal of Educational Psychology, 97(3), 468–483. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.468
  • Vlek, C. (1984). What constitutes ‘A good decision’? Acta Psychologica, 56(1–3), 5–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(84)90004-0
  • Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton University Press.
  • Warren, C., Kann, L., Small, M., Santelli, J., Collins, J., & Kolbe, L. (1997). Age of initiating selected health-risk behaviors among high school students in the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 21(4), 225–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1054-139X(97)00112-2
  • Weller, J. A., Ceschi, A., & Randolph, C. (2015). Decision-making competence predicts domain-specific risk attitudes. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(540), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00540
  • Weller, J. A., Leve, L. D., Kim, H. K., Bhimji, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2015). Plasticity of risky decision making among maltreated adolescents: Evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Development and Psychopathology, 27(2), 535–551. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000140
  • Weller, J. A., Levin, I., Rose, J., & Bossard, E. (2012). Assessment of decision-making competence in preadolescence. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25(4), 414–426. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.744
  • Weller, J. A., Levin, I. P., Shiv, B., & Bechara, A. (2007). Neural correlates of adaptive decision making for risky gains and losses. Psychological Science, 18(11), 958–964. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02009.x
  • Weller, J. A., Moholy, M., Bossard, E., & Levin, I. (2015). Preadolescent decision-making competence predicts interpersonal strengths and difficulties: A 2-year prospective study. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(1), 76–88. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.1822
  • Weller, J. A., Parker, A., Reynolds, M., & Kirisci, L. (2021). Developmental trajectory classes in psychological dysregulation predict later decision-making competence. Addictive Behaviors, 112, 106650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106650
  • Wilder, S. (2014). Effects of parental involvement on academic achievement: A meta-synthesis. Educational Review, 66(3), 377–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131911.2013.780009
  • Zhai, Z. W., Kirisci, L., Tarter, R. E., & Ridenour, T. A. (2014). Psychological dysregulation during adolescence mediates the association of parent-child attachment in childhood and substance use disorder in adulthood. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 40(1), 67–74. https://doi.org/10.3109/00952990.2013.848876