1,208
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Psychological factors predicting self-reported and observed aggression in male forensic psychiatric inpatients

ORCID Icon &

Abstract

The present study examined the psychological determinants of self-reported and observed aggression in male violent forensic psychiatric inpatients. Baseline data came from 232 inpatients referred to a cognitive–behavioral treatment program. Linear regression models were used to assess the relationship between the patients’ psychological characteristics and aggressive behavior. Self-reported aggression was studied with cross-sectional data, while the inpatients’ observed aggression in the ward was investigated with prospective data. The main factors contributing to the prediction of self-reported aggression were state anger, antisocial lifestyle and agreeableness, while the main factors contributing to the prediction of observed aggression were trait anger and agreeableness. The findings support the focus of treatment programs for forensic psychiatric inpatients on anger management.

Introduction

Violent forensic psychiatric patients often display aggressive behavior in the ward where they have been admitted (Greer et al., Citation2020). During the past decades, extensive research has been done on the individual, clinical and situational factors of aggressive behavior among forensic psychiatric inpatients (Steinert, Citation2002). However, research into the influence of psychological factors on this behavior is relatively scarce. At the same time, knowledge of these factors is essential for putting together effective programs that lead to a lasting reduction in aggressive behavior during their hospital stay but also after discharge when they are in an environment where aggressive behavior is often allowed or encouraged. The present study focuses on the relationship of aggression with psychological characteristics of 232 male Dutch forensic psychiatric inpatients who were obliged to participate in a cognitive–behavioral treatment program for the reduction of anger and aggression (Hornsveld & Kraaimaat, Citation2019).

Traits and behaviors

Relations between personality traits, for instance, the NEO Five-Factor Model (Costa & McCrae, Citation1992), and aggressive behavior have been studied frequently in offender and nonoffender populations. In a study by Jones et al. (Citation2011), neuroticism, agreeableness and conscientiousness appeared significantly related to aggressive behavior. Extraversion and openness also manifested significant relations with aggression, but the effect sizes were small. Hornsveld et al. (Citation2008) found that Dutch forensic psychiatric inpatients (all males) scored significantly higher on neuroticism and significantly lower on agreeableness than nonclinical adults. Also, the role of neuroticism and agreeableness in relational or intimate partner aggression has been demonstrated in several studies with college or undergraduate students (e.g. Hines & Saudino, Citation2008).

Veenstra et al. (Citation2008) concluded from the literature that individuals prone to aggression are characterized by relatively high trait anger, a negatively toned emotion. They tend to perceive situations as hostile and are less capable of controlling their hostile thoughts and feelings. However, anger is neither necessary nor sufficient for aggression or violence, but it impels aggression, particularly when its intensity overrides regulatory control mechanisms. While the experience of anger creates a readiness for aggression, it may be otherwise directed, suppressed or reconstituted (Novaco, Citation2017). Although several theoretical explanations have been published on the relationship between anger and aggression (e.g. Wilkowski & Robinson, Citation2010), studies investigating such a connection are limited and are primarily performed in nonclinical populations (e.g. Bogdan et al., Citation2016; Zhang & Chan, Citation2016). Studies linking anger management treatment and reduced recidivism have indirectly demonstrated the link between anger and aggression. Many studies have demonstrated that cognitive–behavioral treatment programs that comprise, among others, anger management training resulted in lower violence recidivism (e.g. Cortoni et al., Citation2006; Dowden & Andrews, Citation2000; Joliffe & Farrington, Citation2007; Polaschek et al., Citation2004; Polaschek et al., Citation2016).

Hostility, the inclination to attribute negative intentions to others (Berkovitz, Citation1993), can also lead to anger and aggressive behavior (e.g. Matthews & Norris, Citation2002). For instance, Lim, Day and Casey (Citation2011) found that violent offenders reported significantly higher trait anger levels and an increased tendency for hostile attributions than their nonviolent counterparts. Also, a meta-analytic review by Orobio de Castro et al. (Citation2002) demonstrated a significant association between hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior in children. According to the integrative cognitive model (ICM) by Wilkowski and Robinson (Citation2010), a hostile interpretation of an (alleged) conflict situation can activate trait anger, which in turn leads to state anger or reactive aggression.

Studies have addressed the relation of psychopathy to criminality (Blackburn & Coid, Citation1998), different forms of aggression (Cima & Raine, Citation2009; Cornell et al., Citation1996; Woodworth & Porter, Citation2002), violence in the community (Vitacco et al, Citation2005) and institutional aggression (Guy et al., Citation2005; Hildebrand et al. Citation2004; Hill et al., Citation2004). Walters (Citation2003) studied the relation between Hare’s original two Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (PCL–R) factors (‘callous remorseless use of others’ and ‘chronically unstable and antisocial lifestyle’; Hare, Citation1991) and verbal infractions or physical aggression. The original Factor 2 appeared to have a moderately positive correlation with verbal infractions or physical aggression, whereas the original Factor 1 showed less robust associations. Guy et al. (Citation2005) refined this analysis and found less evidence for divergent relationships between the two original PCL–R factors and various aggressive and violent behavior types. All relevant research indicates that the original Factor 2, which primarily refers to socially deviant behavior, is a good predictor of problem behaviors such as aggressive behavior and violent recidivism. Conversely, relations between the original Factor 1 and these forms of problem behavior are often modest or absent (e.g. Zwets et al., Citation2015).

Social anxiety was found to play a role in relational aggression (e.g. Batanova & Loukas, Citation2011) and forensic psychiatric outpatients’ anger (Kraaimaat & Hornsveld, Citation2022). Also, there is evidence that a subgroup of patients with a social anxiety disorder shows aggressive behavior (Kashdan & McKnight, Citation2010). Therefore, emotion regulation (discrimination of social anxiety and anger) and prosocial skills training are essential components of treatment programs aimed at reducing anger and aggression (e.g. Cortoni et al., Citation2006). Daffern et al. (Citation2007) incorporated the several individual factors of aggressive behavior into functional analysis, a method that correlates antecedents, individual characteristics, emotional responses and consequences (Haynes & O’Brien, Citation2000). Hornsveld, Kraaimaat, Nunes, et al. (Citation2019) also applied this functional analysis method, which was used to elucidate relevant individual factors such as personality traits and problem behaviors to explain reactive and proactive aggressive behavior in Dutch forensic psychiatric patients.

The present study

The two-fold aim of the present study with male forensic psychiatric inpatients was to explore the relevant psychological determinants of self-reported aggression cross-sectionally and observed aggression in the ward prospectively. We studied a relatively large sample of violent forensic psychiatric patients since there is a relative absence of studies on violent offender subjects with generalized and overlearned aggressive behavior.

Method

Participants

In the Netherlands, offenders who have committed a serious violent crime that is punishable with a maximum imprisonment of more than four years (e.g. murder, manslaughter, aggravated assault or rape) can be detained under a hospital order (‘TBS order’). Based on an extensive psychiatric and/or psychological evaluation at a specialized assessment center of the Ministry of Justice and Security, it concerns offenders who are judged to have diminished responsibility for the offense they committed (Van Marle, Citation2000). TBS involves involuntary admission to a specialized maximum-security forensic psychiatric hospital with obligatory treatment programs that should decrease recidivism risk to an ‘acceptable level for society’.

The study was performed on 232 male TBS patients at four Dutch forensic psychiatric institutions with a mean age of 33.6 years (SD = 7.98, range = 19–57), and 69% of the patients were from Dutch descent. The patients were classified as having an antisocial personality disorder or a psychotic disorder in remission combined with an antisocial personality disorder (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fifth Edition, DSM–5; American Psychiatric Association, Citation2013). Condition of the remitted psychotic patients has been stabilized to the extent that their antisocial personality disorder was most prominent. In addition, all patients had a sufficient command of the Dutch language in speech and writing. The study was approved by the Regional Ethics Committee, CMO of Rotterdam, the Netherlands.

Setting

All patients stayed in high-security wards with a maximum of 11 and 7 patients. The patient–staff ratio was approximately 1:1.8. All patients receive general education and occupational training. Pharmacotherapy was applied to patients with a psychotic disorder and personality-disordered patients for whom it was indicated and who did not refuse medication. No specific data are available on whether and, if so, in what form individual participants received medication, since nonmedical practitioners in the participating institutions do not have access to the files of psychiatrists. Psychiatrists are part of the multidisciplinary treatment teams and in this way inform nonmedical practitioners about their medication policy of the participants.

Approximately four months after admission, the psychiatric and psychological evaluations are carried out, and an individual treatment plan is established. Four to 12 weeks after the psychiatric and psychological evaluations and before the individual indicated therapy, the patients’ aggression in the ward was assessed using observation scales.

Measures

The Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL–R; Hare, Citation1991; Dutch version: Vertommenet al., Citation2002) was employed for measuring psychopathy. The checklist consists of 20 items, which have to be scored by certified examiners based on a file study and an interview using a 3-point scale with does not apply (0), applies to some extent (1) and applies (2). Vertommen et al. (Citation2002) found support for the PCL–R’s reliability and validity in the Dutch version. In addition, they confirmed Hare’s two-factor structure: ‘Callous and remorseless use of others‘ (e.g. ‘Lack of remorse or guilt’) and ‘Chronically unstable and antisocial lifestyle’ (e.g. ‘Poor behavioral controls’). Also, Hildebrand et al. (Citation2002) reported that the internal consistency of the Dutch version is high and that the interrater variability of two separate factor scores and the total score varies from good to excellent. Internal reliability coefficients Cronbach α of Factor 1 and Factor 2 obtained in the present sample was .69 and .71.

Zwets et al. (Citation2015) studied the four-factor structure of the PCL–R (Hare, Citation2003) and found in a comparable sample of 411 Dutch forensic psychiatric inpatients that the internal consistency of the PCL–R total score was good with Cronbach’s α coefficients of .83 for both groups of patients. For the group of patients with a personality disorder, the internal consistency of the Interpersonal, Lifestyle and Antisocial factors was acceptable (αs of .71, .71 and .70, respectively) whereas the internal consistency of the Affective factor was somewhat lower (.67). The internal consistency of the group of patients with a psychotic disorder was acceptable for the Affective and Lifestyle factors (αs of .76 and .77, respectively) and lower for the Interpersonal and Antisocial factors (αs .60 and .69, respectively). Mean inter-item correlations (MICs) were also calculated as this descriptive statistic is a true indicator of item homogeneity. The mean inter-item correlations indicated acceptable homogeneity for all factors in patients with a personality disorder (Interpersonal = .39, Affective = .34, Lifestyle = .33, Antisocial = .31) and in patients with a psychotic disorder (Interpersonal = .29, Affective = .45, Lifestyle = .40, Antisocial = .30). For the total PCL–R scale, mean inter-item correlations were acceptable for both groups (.20 for both groups) as MICs of .20 or above indicated acceptable homogeneity (Zwets et al., Citation2015).

The NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, Citation1992; Dutch version: Hoekstra et al., Citation1996) has 60 items and measures the Big Five personality domains Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. Participants score items on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from entirely disagree (1) to entirely agree (5). Internal consistency and test–retest reliability of the Dutch NEO-FFI scales were good in nonclinical adults’ samples (Hoekstra et al., Citation1996). In the present study, only the domains Neuroticism, Agreeableness and Conscientiousness were used. Cronbach’s α coefficients obtained in the present study are .79, .60 and .74.

The Adapted Version of Rosenzweig’s (1978) Picture-Frustration Study (PFS–AV; Hornsveld & Kraaimaat, Citation2022a; Hornsveld et al., Citation2007a) was used to measure hostility. The PFS–AV asks participants to write down their reactions to 12 cartoon-like pictures. Respondents are instructed to examine the pictures’ situation and write the first appropriate reply that enters their minds in the blank text box. Answers are scored by an experienced and independent research assistant (psychologist) on a 7-point scale, ranging from not at all hostile (1) to extremely hostile (7). In a sample of 231 forensic psychiatric patients, the internal consistency (α = .76), test–retest reliability (r = .67) and interrater reliability (r = .77) of the PFS–AV were moderate to good. Furthermore, evidence was found for the convergent validity of the test as scores correlated with indices of agreeableness and aggressive behavior (Hornsveld et al., Citation2007a). Measurement invariance of the PFS–AV was found to be satisfactory and shows that the instrument is appropriate for investigating hostility across populations (Hornsveld & Kraaimaat, Citation2022b). In the present sample, the obtained internal reliability coefficient α was .85.

The Inventory of Interpersonal Situations (IIS; Kraaimaat, Citation2020; Van Dam-Baggen & Kraaimaat, Citation1990, Citation1999) is a Dutch self-report questionnaire with two scales. One scale indicates social discomfort/anxiety; the other scale concerns the frequency of performance of social responses (i.e. social skills). Each scale consists of the same 35 items formulated as responses to specific social situations. On the Discomfort/Anxiety scale, items are scored from no tension (1) to very much tension (5); on the Frequency scale, the identical items are scored from I never do (1) to I always do (5). The reliability and validity of the IIS have been investigated in several adult psychiatric and nonpsychiatric samples. The scales of Discomfort/Anxiety and Frequency showed stability over time. Cronbach’s αs revealed a high internal consistency on both scales, while the conceptual structure was shown to be relatively invariant across socially anxious and nonsocially anxious groups. The IIS scales demonstrated high predictive validity for overt behavior in social situations (Van Dam-Baggen & Kraaimaat, Citation1999). Cronbach’s α coefficients obtained in the present sample were .94 and .91.

The Trait Anger subscale of the Dutch version of Spielberger’s (Citation1980) StateTrait Anger Scale (ZAV; Van der Ploeg et al., Citation1982) was used to measure the general disposition to anger. Participants rate each item how they generally feel using a 4-point Likert scale: almost never (1), sometimes (2), often (3) and almost always (4). In a group of 150 Dutch male university students, Van der Ploeg et al. (Citation1982) found that the trait anger scales’ internal consistency (α coefficient) was .78, and test–retest reliability of .78 was documented in a subgroup of 70 students. The convergent validity of the trait anger scale also proved to be satisfactory. The present sample obtained an internal Cronbach α coefficient of .92.

The NAS Part A of the Novaco Anger Scale–Provocation Inventory (NAS–PI; Novaco, Citation2003; Dutch version: Hornsveld et al., Citation2011) was used to measure state anger and concerned the self-reported responses relating to cognitive, arousal and behavioral components of anger in 48 anger-eliciting situations. The items are scored on a 3-point Likert-type scale: never true (1), sometimes true (2) and always true (3). In a sample of 194 Dutch violent forensic psychiatric outpatients (all males), for the NAS total score the internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) was found to be .95, and the test–retest reliability in a subgroup of 90 outpatients was .80 (Hornsveld et al. Citation2011). The internal reliability coefficient α obtained in the present study was .94.

The Aggression Questionnaire is derived from Buss and Perry’s Aggression Questionnaire with 29 items (AQ; Buss & Perry, Citation1992; Dutch version: Meesters et al., Citation1996), which spread among four subscales, namely Physical Aggression, Verbal Aggression, Anger and Hostility. Respondents answer the items using a 5-point scale ranging from not at all like me (1) to completely like me (5). In a group of 138 Dutch TBS patients (all males), Hornsveld et al. (Citation2009) found for the AQ total an internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) of .83 and for the four subscales an internal consistency of .72 .34, .57 and .81 successively. Only Physical Aggression and Verbal Aggression subscales were used in the present study. The scores on these two subscales were added together. The correlation between both scales was .61. The internal coefficient α of the combined scale in the present study was .82.

The Observation Scale for Aggressive Behavior (OSAB; Hornsveld et al, Citation2007b) measures behavior in the ward. The scale comprises 40 items spread over the subscales Irritation/Anger, Anxiety/Gloominess, Aggressive Behavior, Social Behavior, Antecedent and Sanction. The staff scores the behavior of inpatients in the preceding week on a 4-point scale with no (1), seldom (2), occasionally (3) and frequently (4). The psychometric qualities of the OSAB were studied in a sample of 220 Dutch violent forensic psychiatric inpatients. Results showed that the internal consistency of the subscales varied from .63 to .93, and interrater reliability correlations were between .49 and .81, whereas test–retest reliability ranged between .48 and .79 (Hornsveld et al., Citation2007b). In the present study, only the 10-item subscale Aggressive Behavior was used. The internal consistency α of this subscale obtained in the present study was .79.

Procedure

The questionnaires, in paper-and-pencil format, were taken individually from the patients. Then, after 4–12 weeks, before the patients started with their cognitive–behavioral treatment program for their aggression, the staff members completed the observation scale (OSAB).

Statistics

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) Version 25 and AMOS 26. Descriptive statistics were used to examine the scores on the questionnaires and scales. Two-tailed significance testing and α levels of .05 and .01 were used for all analyses. First, Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to examine the relationship between all variables in the study. Next, linear regression analyses were calculated with those independent variables significantly associated with the dependent variables. A linear regression analysis (forward procedureFootnote1) was performed with age, PCL–R Factor 2 antisocial lifestyle, the NEO-FFI personality domains Neuroticism and Agreeableness, state anger (NAS–PI) and trait anger (STAS) as independent variables and self-reported aggression (AQ) as the dependent variable. In addition, a linear regression analysis (forward procedure) was performed with the NEO-FFI personality domains Neuroticism and Agreeableness, state anger (NAS–PI), trait anger (STAS) and self-reported aggression (AQ) as independent variables and observed aggression (OSAB) as the dependent variable. Multicollinearity was measured by tolerance (Allison, Citation2003). Finally, to explore the indirect effects of the predicting variables, indices of mediation for all obtained regression models were calculated using AMOS 26 (Preacher & Hayes, Citation2008).

Results

Distributions of all variables were investigated and considered normal as skewness and kurtosis were within |3| (Tabachnick & Fiddell, Citation2007). In , the mean, standard deviation and the number of participants are presented for the measurements obtained from the participants. As shown in , the internal consistency coefficients of the measurements used indicated a modest (Agreeableness, PCL–R factors) to good and excellent internal reliability. Concerning the OSAB, no incidence of aggression was observed in 13% of the patients (scores <11) during the one week of observation. Due to missing values, the SPSS pairwise procedure was used to calculate Pearson correlation coefficients and perform regression analyses with the dataset of 232 inpatients.

Table 1. Characteristics of participants.

In , correlation coefficients are presented between all variables. State anger (NAS–PI), trait anger (STAS), antisocial lifestyle (PCL–R Factor 2) and neuroticism (NEO-FFI) appeared to be positive, and age and agreeableness (NEO-FFI) were negatively related to self-reported aggression (AQ). In addition, trait anger (STAS), state anger (NAS–PI), self-reported aggression (AQ) and neuroticism (NEO-FFI) were positively related and agreeableness (NEO-FFI) negatively related to observed aggression (OSAB).

Table 2. Correlations between the psychological factors, self-reported and observed aggression, and age.

Predictors of self-reported aggression (AQ)

A linear regression analysis (forward method) was performed with the AQ scale as the dependent variable. Only those independent variables significantly related to the dependent variable were introduced. A summary of the results is presented in .

Table 3. Regression analysis results: predicting self-reported aggression (AQ).

The analysis resulted in three models explaining 32–39% of the variance of self-reported aggression. Predictors in the last model were state anger (NAS–PI) followed by antisocial lifestyle (PCL–R Factor 2) and agreeableness (NEO-FFI). The explained variance of 39% and Cohen’s (Citation1988) f 2 = .65 are indicative of a large effect. As can be seen, by the standard coefficient B, relatively high scores for state anger and antisocial lifestyle and a low score of agreeableness were associated with high scores of self-reported aggression.

There was no evidence of collinearity because tolerance values (.87, .99 and .87) were all above .20 (Martin & Bridsmon, Citation2012). Exploration of mediated effects on self-reported aggression using AMOS 26 revealed insignificant indices of mediation (Preacher & Hayes, Citation2008) for antisocial lifestyle (standardized indirect effect = .00) and agreeableness (standardized indirect effect = .00).

Predictors of observed aggression (OSAB)

A linear regression analysis (forward method) was performed with the OSAB scale as the dependent variable. Only those independent variables significantly related to the dependent variable were introduced. A summary of the results is presented in .

Table 4. Regression analysis results: predicting observed aggression (OSAB).

The analysis resulted, respectively, in two models explaining nine to about 13% of the variance of observed aggression. Predictors of the last model were trait anger (STAS) and agreeableness (NEO-FFI). The explained variance of about 13% and Cohen’s (Citation1988) f 2 = .15 indicate a medium effect. As can be seen, by the standard coefficient B, a relatively high trait anger score and a low agreeableness score were associated with high scores of observed aggression. No evidence was revealed of collinearity because tolerance values (.90 and .90) were all above .20 (Martin & Bridsmon, Citation2012). Exploration of a mediated effect on observed aggression using AMOS 26 revealed an insignificant index of mediation (Preacher & Hayes, Citation2008) for agreeableness (standardized indirect effect = .00). Potentially aggression of forensic psychiatric patients at the ward seems to be driven by the individual factors of enduring anger and low sociability.

Discussion

The psychological factors of trait and state anger, agreeableness, neuroticism and antisocial lifestyle were significantly associated with the aggressive behavior of forensic psychiatric inpatients. Specifically, high scores on state anger and antisocial lifestyle and low scores on agreeableness were the main predicting variables for self-reported aggression. High scores on trait anger and low scores on agreeableness were the prospective and main predictors of observed aggression. These individual factors explained 39% of the variance of self-reported and 13% of the variance of observed aggression in the ward. It must be noted that other factors, specific environmental factors, might be at stake concerning observed aggression at the ward. For instance, Daffern et al. (Citation2007) found that the most common function of inpatient aggression was a response to the restrictions and demands of the inpatient setting and provocative actions by co-patients.

Self-reported aggression and observed aggression were significantly associated but to a relatively low degree. The latter finding may be due to various factors such as the use of different measurement methods, in this case, self-reporting versus observation by staff, a cross-sectional versus a prospective design, and different periods and situations such as one week of observed behavior at the ward and patients’ retrospective assessment of aggression in a wide range of past and present situations. The ward’s anger-inducing situations will also be quite different and more restricted from those previously experienced by the patients. A forensic hospital is a highly structured, restricted and controlled environment where aggressive behavior is not tolerated and inhibited (e.g. Van Marle, Citation2000). Outside the hospital, the patients probably lived in an environment where aggressive behavior is more permitted and sometimes even instrumental and strategic. Therefore, the results obtained with the OSAB and the AQ may indicate somewhat different realms that are both informative of the forensic inpatients’ aggression.

Neuroticism

Neuroticism was low but significantly related to self-reported as well as observed aggression. The personality trait of neuroticism indicates a person’s susceptibility to disturbed emotional regulation, as demonstrated by heightened emotional states such as fear, anxiety and aggression (Barlow et al., Citation2014; Ormel et al., Citation2013). As such, neuroticism can be regarded as a higher order construct of specific negative emotions. Due to a disrupted emotion regulation process, forensic psychiatric patients with a high score of neuroticism and trait anger may be inclined to respond in conflict situations with negative emotions such as state anger, resulting in aggression.

Since no instrumental goal was present or assumed in the observed aggressive behavior on the ward, there is no reason to consider the measured aggressive behavior as proactive (Bettencourt et al., Citation2006). In addition, anger was significantly related to self-reported and observed aggression, which is indicative of reactive aggressive behavior. When patients are observed to be angry, it may indicate that their aggressive behavior is not instrumental (Daffern et al., Citation2007). Notwithstanding this, both reactive and proactive components may be involved in self-reported aggression as measured by the combined AQ subscales.

Social anxiety

Social anxiety and lack of adequate social skills turned out not to be associated with aggression. This contrasts with our clinical observations that forensic psychiatric patients often respond more clumsily during role-plays than they indicate in interviews and on self-report measures. An explanation may be that they confuse assertive with aggressive responding. Some support for this assumption was found in an earlier study (Hornsveld et al., Citation2008) where forensic psychiatric inpatients reported, compared with a norm group, significantly lower social anxiety and more social skills in situations where criticism is given, and higher social anxiety and fewer skills where another person is complimented. Further research with regard to this issue is warranted.

Hostility was negatively associated with agreeableness and positively with state anger but not associated with self-reported and observed aggression. In line with this, Lim et al. (Citation2011) found higher trait levels and an increased tendency for hostile attributions in violent offenders compared to nonviolent counterparts. This suggests that the role of hostile attributions is more or less restricted to eliciting and enhancing the forensic psychiatric patients’ anger. It seems that hostility and anger might reciprocally activate each other.

Limitations

The present investigation has several limitations. First, the results show that trait and state anger are the main predictors of what appears to be primarily reactive aggression. In future studies, a specially designed instrument for self-reported proactive aggression may be used, for example, the Reactive Proactive Questionnaire (RPQ; Raine et al., Citation2006). Second, though self-report is a primary source for assessing the patient’s thoughts, emotions and action tendencies, it has several limitations. Even though the scores on the self-report questionnaires showed a consistent picture, we should keep in mind that forensic psychiatric patients’ responses may be influenced by their desire to make a positive impression and limited insight into their behavior (Hornsveld, Kraaimaat, Nijman, et al., Citation2019). Third, the correlations of agreeableness with related constructs support the construct validity of this measure. However, the low internal reliability (α = .60) warrants further investigation of underlying facets of this broad domain and their correlations with the measures of anger and aggression used in the present study.

Our impression is that the primarily low-educated patients did not know the meaning of some words and only asked for clarification when administered in a few cases. Last, as only male patients were enrolled in the study, it is unknown to what extent the findings generalize to female forensic psychiatric patients. Primarily, females are known to use more covert and indirect social and verbal forms of aggression (Björkvist, Citation2018).

Further research and consequences for treatment

As the determinants examined in this study are rather general, further research on the population of violent forensic psychiatric patients is needed into the specific processes involved in emotion dysregulation, such as attention, perception and information processing (e.g. Andrews & Bonta, Citation2010; Wilkowski & Robinson, Citation2010).

In conclusion, forensic psychiatric inpatients’ aggression was associated with the dynamic and psychological factors of trait and state anger, agreeableness, neuroticism and antisocial lifestyle. Furthermore, high anger and low agreeableness were efficient in predicting high self-reported and observed aggression levels. Our results support using therapy modules aimed at anger management, prosocial attitudes and prosocial skills in forensic psychiatric patients (e.g. Goldstein et al., Citation1998; Hornsveld & Kraaimaat, Citation2019).

Ethical standards

Declaration of conflicts of interest

Ruud H.J. Hornsveld has declared no conflicts of interest

Floris W. Kraaimaat has declared no conflicts of interest

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the Regional Ethics Committee, CMO of Rotterdam, the Netherlands, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study

Notes

1 The forward procedure means that variables are entered consecutively according to the extent of their correlation.

References

  • Allison, P. D. (2003). Missing data techniques for structural equation modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(4), 545–557. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.112.4.545
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). APA. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
  • Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2010). The psychology of criminal conduct (5th ed.). Matthew Bender & Company.
  • Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Sauer-Zavala, S., Bullis, J. R., & Carl, J. R. (2014). The origins of neuroticism. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 9(5), 481. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614544528
  • Batanova, M., & Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents: Examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective tak. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(11), 1534–1543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-011-9634-x
  • Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  • Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 751–777. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.751
  • Björkvist, K. (2018). Gender differences in aggression. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 39–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.030
  • Blackburn, R., & Coid, J. W. (1998). Psychopathy and the dimensions of personality disorder in violent offenders. Personality and Individual Differences, 25(1), 129–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00027-0
  • Bogdan, S. R., Măirean, C., & Havârneanu, C.-E. (2016). A meta-analysis of the association between anger and aggressive driving. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 42(2), 350–364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2016.05.009
  • Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452–459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452
  • Cima, M., & Raine, A. (2009). Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different subtypes of aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 835–840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031
  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum.
  • Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., & Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(4), 783–790. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.64.4.783
  • Cortoni, F., Nunes, K., & Latendresse, M. (2006). An examination of the effectiveness of the Violence Prevention Program (Research Report, R-178). Correctional Service Canada.
  • Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and the Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
  • Daffern, M. D., Howells, K., & Ogloff, J. R. (2007). What’s the point? Towards a methodology for assessing the function of psychiatric inpatient aggression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.01.011
  • Dowden, C., & Andrews, D. A. (2000). Effective correctional treatment and violent reoffending: A meta-analysis. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminology, 42(4), 449–467. https://doi.org/10.3138/cjcrim.42.4.449
  • Goldstein, A. P., Glick, B., & Gibbs, J. C. (1998). Aggression Replacement Training: A comprehensive intervention for aggressive youth. Research Press.
  • Greer, B., Taylor, R. W., Cella, M., Stott, R., & Wykes, T. (2020). The contribution of dynamic isk factors in predicting aggression: A systematic review including inpatient forensic and non-forensic mental health services. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 53, 101433. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2020.101433
  • Guy, L. S., Edens, J. F., Anthony, C., & Douglas, K. S. (2005). Does psychopathy predict Institutional misconduct among adults? A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(6), 1056–1064. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.6.1056
  • Hare, R. D. (1991). The hare psychopathy checklist-revised. Multi-Health Systems.
  • Hare, R. D. (2003). Hare psychopathy checklist-revised (PCL-R) (2nd ed.). Toronto: Multi-Health Systems. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412959537.n134
  • Haynes, S. N., & O’Brien, W. H. (2000). Principles and practice of behavioral assessment. Plenum.
  • Hildebrand, M., De Ruiter, C., & Nijman, H. (2004). PCL-R psychopathy predicts disruptive behavior among male offenders in a Dutch forensic psychiatric hospital. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19(1), 13–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260503259047
  • Hildebrand, M., De Ruiter, C., De Vogel, C. V., & der Wolf, P. (2002). Reliability and factor structure of the Dutch language version of Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 1(2), 139–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/14999013.2002.10471169
  • Hill, C. D., Neumann, C. S., & Rogers, R. (2004). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Psychopathy Checklist: Screening Version in offenders with Axis I disorders. Psychological Assessment, 16(1), 90–95. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.90
  • Hines, D. A., & Saudino, K. J. (2008). Personality and intimate partner aggression in dating relationships: The role of the “Big Five”. Aggressive Behavior, 34(6), 593–604. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20277
  • Hoekstra, H. A., Ormel, J., & De Fruyt, F. (1996). Handleiding NEO. Persoonlijkheidsvragenlijsten [Manual NEO personality questionnaires]. Swets Test Services.
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2019). Treatment programs for violent offenders and sexually violent offenders. In R. H. J. Hornsveld, F. W. Kraaimaat, L. A. C. L. Gijs, & E. J. Palmer (Eds.), Assessment and obligatory treatment of violent and sexually violent offenders. Integrating research and practice (pp. 125–137). Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27840-3
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2022a). De PFS-AV, een meetinstrument voor vijandigheid [The PFS-AV, an assessment instrument for hostility]. Gedragstherapie [Dutch Journal of Behavior Therapy], 55(1), 56–72.
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2022b). Psychological factors predicting violent prisoninmates’ anger and aggression. Journal of Forensic Psychology Research and Practice. https://doi.org/10.1080/24732850.2022.2048768
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Kraaimaat, F. W., Nijman, H. L. I., Rosa, S. J., Nunes, K. L., & Palmer, E. J. (2019). The use of self-report questionnaires. In R. H. J. Hornsveld, W. Kraaimaat, L. A. C. L. Gijs, & E. J. Palmer (Eds.), Assessment and obligatory treatment of violent and sexually violent offenders: Integrating research and practice (pp. 51–65). Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27840-3
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Kraaimaat, F. W., Nunes, K. L., & Palmer, E. J. (2019). Dynamic risk factors of violent and sexually violent offenders. In R. H. J. Hornsveld, F. W. Kraaimaat, L. A. C. L. Gijs, & E. J. Palmer (Eds.), Assessment and obligatory treatment of violent and sexually violent offenders. Integrating research and practice (pp. 39–50). Springer Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-27840-3
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Muris, P. E. H. M., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2011). The Novaco Anger Scale-Provocation Inventory (1994 version) in Dutch forensic psychiatric patients. Psychological Assessment, 23(4), 937–944. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024018
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Muris, P., Kraaimaat, F. W., & Meesters, C. (2009). The Aggression Questionnaire in Dutch violent forensic psychiatric patients and secondary vocational students. Assessment, 16(2), 181–192. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191108325894
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Nijman, H. L. I., Hollin, C. R., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2007a). An adapted version of the Rosenzweig Picture-Frustration Study (PFS-AV) for the measurement of hostility in violent forensic psychiatric patients. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health: CBMH, 17(1), 45–56. https://doi.org/10.1002/cbm.638
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Nijman, H. L. I., Hollin, C. R., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2007b). Development of the Observation Scale for Aggressive Behavior (OSAB) for Dutch forensic psychiatric inpatients with an antisocial personality disorder. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 30(6), 480–491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2007.09.009
  • Hornsveld, R. H. J., Nijman, H. L. I., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (2008). Aggression Control Therapy for Violent forensic psychiatric patients: First results. Psychology, Crime & Law, 14(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160701340569
  • Joliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2007). A systematic review of the national and international evidence on the effectiveness of interventions with violent offenders. Ministry of Justice.
  • Jones, S. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2011). Personality, antisocial behavior, and aggression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Criminal Justice, 39(4), 329–337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2011.03.004
  • Kashdan, T. B., & McKnight, P. E. (2010). The darker side of social anxiety: When aggressive impulsivity prevails over shy inhibition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 47–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359280
  • Kraaimaat, F. W. (2020). Manual supplement 2020: Additional norms for the Inventory of Interpersonal Situations (IIS) [Inventarisatielijnst Omgaan met Anderen (IOA)]. Free Publication Nijmegen. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.22.16341.04322
  • Kraaimaat, F. W., & Hornsveld, R. H. J. (2022). Are anger, hostility, and aggression involved in the social anxiety of forensic psychiatric outpatients? Submitted manuscript.
  • Lim, L., Day, A., & Casey, S. (2011). Social cognitive processing in violent male offenders. Psychiatry Psychology and Law Psychology and Law, 18(2), 177–189. https://doi.org/10.1080/13218711003739490
  • Martin, W. E., & Bridsmon, K. D. (2012). Qualitative and statistical research methods: From hypothesis to results. Wiley.
  • Matthews, B. A., & Norris, F. H. (2002). When is believing “seeing”? Hostile attribution bias As a function of self-reported aggression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(1), 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb01418.x
  • Meesters, C., Muris, P., Bosma, H., Schouten, E., & Beuving, S. (1996). Psychometric evaluation of the Dutch version of the Aggression Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(10), 839–843. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(96)00065-4
  • Novaco, R. W. (2003). The Novaco Anger scale and provocation inventory: Manual. Western Psychological Services.
  • Novaco, R. W. (2017). Cognitive-behavioral factors and anger in the occurrence of aggression and violence. In P. Sturmey (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of violence and aggression, chapter 26. Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119057574.whbva026
  • Ormel, J., Bastiaansen, A., Riese, H., Bos, E. H., Servaas, M., Ellenbogen, M., Rosmalen, G. J. M., & Aleman, A. (2013). The biological and psychological basis of neuroticism: Current status and future directions. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(1), 59–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.09.004
  • Orobio de Castro, B., Veerman, J. W., Koops, W., Bosch, J. D., & Monshouwer, H. J. (2002). Hostile attribution of intent and aggressive behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 73(3), 916–934. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00447
  • Polaschek, D. L. L., Collie, R. M., & Walkey, F. H. (2004). Criminal attitudes to violence: Development and preliminary validation of a scale for male prisoners. Aggressive Behavior, 30(6), 484–503. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20081
  • Polaschek, D. L. L., Yesberg, J. A., Bell, R. K., Casey, A. R., & Dickson, S. R. (2016). Intensive psychological treatment of high-risk violent offenders: Outcomes and pre-release mechanisms. Psychology, Crime & Law, 22(4), 344–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2015.1109088
  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879
  • Raine, A., Dodge, K., Loeber, R., Gatzke-Kopp, L., Lynam, D., Reynolds, C., Stouthamer-Loeber, M., & Liu, J. (2006). The Reactive-Proactive Aggression Questionnaire: Differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggressive Behavior, 32(2), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20115
  • Spielberger, C. D. (1980). Preliminary manual for the State-Trait Anger Scale (STAS). The University of South Florida.
  • Steinert, T. (2002). Prediction of inpatient violence. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 106s412, 133–141. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0447.106.s412.29.x
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fiddell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.
  • Van Dam-Baggen, C. M. J., & Kraaimaat, F. W. (1990). Inventarisatielijst Omgaan met Anderen, IOA. Herziene Handleiding deel 2 [Inventory of interpersonal situations. IIS. Revised manual, vol. 2]. Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Van Dam-Baggen, R., & Kraaimaat, F. (1999). Assessing social anxiety: The Inventory of Interpersonal Situations (IIS). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 15(1), 25–38. https://doi.org/10.1027//1015-5759.15.1.25
  • Van der Ploeg, H. M., Defares, P. B., & Spielberger, C. D. (1982). Handleiding bij de Zelf-Analyse Vragenlijst, ZAV [Manual for the self-analysis questionnaire, ZAV]. Swets & Zeitlinger.
  • Van Marle, H. J. C. (2000). Forensic services in the Netherlands. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 23(5–6), 515–531. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2527(00)00049-2
  • Veenstra, L., Bushman, B. J., & Koole, S. L. (2018). The facts on the furious: A brief review of the psychology of trait anger. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19, 98–103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.014
  • Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2008). Prosocial and antisocial behavior in preadolescence: Teachers’ and parents’ perceptions of the behavior of girls and boys. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32(3), 243–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025408089274
  • Vertommen, H., Verheul, R., Ruiter, C. d., & Hildebrand, M. (2002). De herziene versie van Hare’s Psychopathie Checklist (PCL-R) [The revised version of Hare’s Psychopathy Checklist (PCL-R)]. Swets Test Publishers.
  • Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., & Jackson, R. L. (2005). Testing a four-factor model of psychopathy and its association with ethnicity, gender, intelligence, and violence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(3), 466–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.466
  • Walters, G. D. (2003). Predicting criminal justice outcomes with the Psychopathy Checklist And Lifestyle Criminality Screening Form: A meta-analytic comparison. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 21(1), 89–102. https://doi.org/10.1002/bsl.519
  • Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). The anatomy of anger: An integrative cognitive Model of trait anger and reactive aggression. Journal of Personality, 78(1), 9–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00607.x
  • Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(3), 436–445. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.436
  • Zhang, T., & Chan, A. H. S. (2016). The association between driving anger and driving outcomes: A meta-analysis of evidence from the past twenty years. Accident; Analysis and Prevention, 90(1), 50–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2016.02.009
  • Zwets, A. J., Hornsveld, R. H. J., Neumann, C., Muris, P., & Van Marle, H. J. C. (2015). The four-factor model of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised: Validation in a Dutch forensic inpatient sample. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 39(1), 13–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2015.01.016