1,655
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Extreme weather events and pro-environmental behavior: evidence from a climate change vulnerable country

ORCID Icon &

ABSTRACT

Experiencing an extreme weather event and its consequences might make the risks associated with climate change more tangible, easier to evaluate, and more salient. Consequently, those experiences might translate into the adoption of pro-environmental behaviours. Understanding this relationship is fundamental for the successful design of policies aimed towards promoting the adoption of climate change adaptation and mitigation measures. This work contributes to the literature by showing that there is in fact a positive relation between experiencing an extreme weather event and willingness to take pro-environmental actions. The prevailing available evidence is for developed countries. Our empirical analysis is based on a nationally representative sample of households from Mexico, a developing country that is highly vulnerable to the effects of extreme weather events.

JEL CODES:

I. Introduction

The frequency and intensity of extreme weather events (EWE), such as droughts, heatwaves, and hurricanes, are expected to increase because of climate change (Seneviratne, Nicholls, and Easterling et al. Citation2012). These events can have severe economic (Botzen, Deschenes, and Sanders Citation2019) and ecological consequences (Nelson et al. Citation2013). The risks and impacts of these events are unevenly distributed across the planet; local contexts determine who is affected, how, and for how long (Olsson et al. Citation2014). Individuals in developing countries, especially the poor and disenfranchised, face the highest risk of being negatively affected (Olsson et al. Citation2014).

If they are to reduce the threats of climate change, households and individuals will need to engage in adaptive actions and support and implement mitigation strategies (Van Valkengoed and Steg Citation2019). One of the multiple factors that explain individuals’ willingness to adopt these behaviours is their perception about climate change and the risks it entails (Grothmann and Patt Citation2005). Individuals’ exposure to EWE can be particularly important in terms of explaining perception of climate change (Demski et al. Citation2017; Ogunbode, Doran, and Böhm Citation2020). Individuals can use a simple observable outcome, the EWE, as a substitute for a broader understanding of climate change (Gärtner and Schoen Citation2021). In this way, personal experience of an EWE can act as an attribute substitute for climate change, making the risks associated with it more tangible, easier to evaluate and more salient (Broomell, Budescu, and Por Citation2015; Gärtner and Schoen Citation2021).

Although the link between EWE experiences and climate change beliefs has been thoroughly studied, there is a need for more research in developing countries, especially those that are highly vulnerable to climate change; most of the available analyses refer to developed countries and there is almost no research for either Africa or Latin America (Broomell, Budescu, and Por Citation2015; Van Valkengoed and Steg Citation2019). We contribute to this literature by analysing the case of Mexico, a country that due to its location, geographic characteristics, low levels of adaptive capacity, and high levels of poverty is highly vulnerable to the negative effects of EWE (Arceo-Gómez, Hernández-Cortés, and López-Feldman Citation2020). We look at the relationship between direct personal experience with an EWE and willingness to adopt a series of pro-environmental actions related to water, energy and waste management.

II. Materials and methods

We use data from the Household and Environmental Module (MOHOMA) of Mexico’s National Income and Expenditure Survey (ENH). The MOHOMA was designed and implemented by Mexico’s National Institute of Statistics, Geography and Informatics to be representative of the whole country. The questionnaire was applied during 2017 and captures information for 2016 (INEGI Citation2017). In addition to basic individual and household characteristics, the questionnaire captures information related to households’ experience with EWE during 2016; this is our variable of interest. Thirteen percentage of the households in our sample responded that they had experienced an EWE during the previous year; of those, 95% said that they had been directly affected in a negative way (e.g. dwelling damaged, loss of job, health afflictions) by that event. Additionally, questionnaire respondents were asked about their willingness to adopt, in an unspecified future date, different pro-environmental actions related to water, energy and waste. With this information, the count and binary variables that act as dependent variables in the empirical analysis were created (see ).

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the variables included in the statistical analyses.

The association between experiencing an EWE and willingness to take pro-environmental actions is analysed by estimating a series of Poisson and probit models. For each one of the three variables that measure the number of actions that an individual is willing to take, we estimate four different specifications of the Poisson model.Footnote1 In the first specification, the only independent variable is the binary variable for experiencing an EWE. For the second specification, we add the following individual and household level variables: age, gender, elementary school, middle school, high school, university, wealth and household size (see for a description of each one of the covariates). For the third specification, the following locality and municipal level variables are added: rural, poverty and density. Finally, for the fourth specification, we add region indicators; this allows for the possibility that our results could be affected by unobserved geographic regional factors. In this way, we try to isolate the effect of experiencing an EWE from other confounding factors. The full, and preferred, model is the following:

Eyij=exp(β0j+β1jexperiencei+Xiδj+regioni)

where yij is the number of actions that individual i is willing to take in one of the j categories (water, energy or waste management), experience is equal to one if the household in which individual i lives was affected by an EWE, Xi is a vector that includes all the individual, household, locality and municipal controls previously mentioned, and regioni are regional indicator variables. To complement this analysis, we estimate the association between experiencing an EWE and the probability that an individual will say that she is willing to pay more for water, energy or waste disposal. To do this, we estimate probit models for the same four specifications explained above.

III. Results

shows the results of estimating Poisson regressions to look at the association between experiencing an EWE and the number of pro-environmental actions that an individual is willing to take. Panel A shows results for pro-environmental actions related to water. In the specification without any covariates (column 1), we find that experiencing an EWE increases the number of water-related pro-environmental actions that an individual is willing to take. The statistical significance of this effect remains very high when individual and household covariates (column 2), municipal and locality covariates (column 3), and region indicators (column 4) are included. Results for actions related to energy are a lot weaker (Panel B). The estimated coefficient is only weakly significant for the specifications that include municipal and locality covariates (column 3), and regional indicators (column 4). Results for waste-related actions (Panel C) are highly statistically significant across all specifications. Although there are no studies directly comparable to ours, the results presented here are in line with those by Demski et al. (Citation2017), who show that experiencing an EWE can have effects on behavioural intentions not directly related to the event.

Table 2. Estimations of the impacts of experiencing an extreme weather event (EWE).

shows the average marginal effect that experiencing an EWE has on the expected value of the different pro-environmental groups of actions; for all the estimations we use the coefficients from the full specification. An individual affected by an EWE during the previous year is willing to take, on average, almost 0.1 more pro-environmental water-related actions than an individual from a household not affected by an EWE. The point estimates for the average marginal effects on waste actions are almost twice as big. Meanwhile, the marginal effects on energy are much smaller in magnitude and only statistically significant at a 10% level.

Figure 1. The effects of experiencing an extreme weather event on the number of pro-environmental measures that an individual is willing to adopt across different categories. Estimates of marginal effects shown with 95% robust confidence intervals. The Poisson models behind each estimation are those described in Panels A to C in .

Figure 1. The effects of experiencing an extreme weather event on the number of pro-environmental measures that an individual is willing to adopt across different categories. Estimates of marginal effects shown with 95% robust confidence intervals. The Poisson models behind each estimation are those described in Panels A to C in Table 2.

The results for the relationship between experiencing an EWE and willingness to pay more for water, energy or waste disposal are shown in Panel D of . The estimations are very robust across the different specifications and show a positive relationship. Average marginal effects, using the full specification, show that individuals affected by an EWE are five percentage points (95% CI: 1.7, 8.3) more likely to say that they are willing to pay more for water, energy or waste disposal, than those not affected by an EWE.

IV. Conclusions

This work contributes to the literature by showing that experiencing an EWE can increase individuals’ willingness to take pro-environmental actions that are not directly related to the nature of the event experienced. Nevertheless, an important limitation of our work is that there is nothing to say about the effect that experiencing an EWE has on actual behaviour. Another limitation is that, with the data available, we cannot discriminate between different hypotheses that can explain the underlying mechanisms behind our results. Finally, identifying causal effects is restricted by the fact that we do not have longitudinal data or data resulting from an experimental setting.

Our analysis is based on a nationally representative sample of households from a developing country that is highly vulnerable to the effects of EWEs. Improving our understanding of the factors that explain pro-environmental decisions in settings like this, can be very relevant for the design of policies aimed towards promoting the adoption of climate change adaptation and mitigation measures. In this sense, the results presented here suggest that targeting some mitigation and adaptation policies on households recently affected by an EWE is something that might be worth exploring. For example, one policy intervention could be to provide affected households with information that links climate change with the occurrence of EWEs similar to the one that they have experienced. This could be complemented by an intervention that provides information about climate change adaptation and locally available mitigation options. Arguably, these interventions could reinforce households’ perception and awareness of climate change, while also modifying their behaviour in terms of adoption of mitigation and adaptation actions. In any case, more research and additional data is needed to better understand the causal relationship between experiencing EWEs and the adoption of pro-environmental actions.

Acknowledgments

We thank Danae Hernández-Cortés, Isael Fierros, participants at the 16th annual meeting of the Environment for Development Initiative, and an anonymous reviewer, for their valuable comments and suggestions.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Additional information

Funding

The author(s) reported there is no funding associated with the work featured in this article.

Notes

1 We use a quasi-maximum likelihood Poisson estimator, which has the advantage of being consistent regardless of the form of the variance (Wooldridge Citation2010). As a robustness check we estimated binomial models to consider the fact that our count data is conditional on an upper bound. The results of that model are qualitatively the same as those presented here; the marginal effects are almost indistinguishable across the different estimations. Finally, since our data does not show overdispersion, estimating negative binomial models leads to the same results as estimating Poisson models.

References

  • Arceo-Gómez, E. O., D. Hernández-Cortés, and A. López-Feldman. 2020. “Droughts and Rural Households’ Wellbeing: Evidence from Mexico.” Climatic Change 162 (3): 1197–1212. doi:10.1007/s10584-020-02869-1.
  • Botzen, W., O. Deschenes, and M. Sanders. 2019. “The Economic Impacts of Natural Disasters: A Review of Models and Empirical Studies.” Review of Environmental Economics and Policy 13 (2): 167–188. doi:10.1093/reep/rez004.
  • Broomell, S. B., D. V. Budescu, and H. H. Por. 2015. “Personal Experience with Climate Change Predicts Intentions to Act.” Global Environmental Change 32: 67–73. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2015.03.001.
  • Demski, C., S. Capstick, N. Pidgeon, R. G. Sposato, and A. Spence. 2017. “Experience of Extreme Weather Affects Climate Change Mitigation and Adaptation Responses.” Climatic Change 140 (2): 149–164. doi:10.1007/s10584-016-1837-4.
  • Gärtner, L., and H. Schoen. 2021. “Experiencing Climate Change: Revisiting the Role of Local Weather in Affecting Climate Change Awareness and Related Policy Preferences.” Climatic Change 167 (3): 1–20. doi:10.1007/s10584-021-03176-z.
  • Grothmann, T., and A. Patt. 2005. “Adaptive Capacity and Human Cognition: The Process of Individual Adaptation to Climate Change.” Global Environmental Change 15 (3): 199–213. doi:10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2005.01.002.
  • INEGI. 2017. Módulo de Hogares y Medio Ambiente. Accessed April 2019. https://www.inegi.org.mx/programas/mohoma/2017/
  • Nelson, E. J., P. Kareiva, M. Ruckelshaus, K. Arkema, G. Geller, E. Girvetz, D. Goodrich, et al. 2013. ”Climate Change’s Impact on Key Ecosystem Services and the Human Well-Being They Support in the US.” Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 11: 483–893. doi:https://doi.org/10.1890/120312.
  • Ogunbode, C. A., R. Doran, and G. Böhm. 2020. “Individual and Local Flooding Experiences are Differentially Associated with Subjective Attribution and Climate Change Concern.” Climatic Change 162 (4): 2243–2255. doi:10.1007/s10584-020-02793-4.
  • Olsson, L., M. Opondo, P. Tschakert, A. Agrawal, S. H. Eriksen, S. Ma, L. N. Perch, et al. 2014. ”Livelihoods and Poverty.” In Climate Change 2014. Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability. Part A: Global and Sectoral Aspects. Contribution of Working Group II to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, edited by C. B. Field, V. R. Barros, and D. J. Dokken, 793–832. Cambridge, UK and New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Seneviratne, S.I., N. Nicholls, D. Easterling, Goodess, C.M., Kanae, S., Kossin, J., Luo, Y., et al. 2012. ”Changes in Climate Extremes and Their Impacts on the Natural Physical Environment.” In A Special Report of Working Groups I and II of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) Managing the Risks of Extreme Events and Disasters to Advance Climate Change Adaptation, edited by C. Field, V. Barros, and T. Stocker, 109–230. Cambridge, UK, and New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
  • Van Valkengoed, A. M., and L. Steg. 2019. “Meta-Analyses of Factors Motivating Climate Change Adaptation Behaviour.” Nature Climate Change 9 (2): 158–163. doi:10.1038/s41558-018-0371-y.
  • Wooldridge, J. 2010. Econometric Analysis of Cross Section and Panel Data. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: MIT Press.