Publication Cover
Neurocase
Behavior, Cognition and Neuroscience
Volume 27, 2021 - Issue 3
2,849
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Visual field improvement in neglect after virtual reality intervention: a single-case study

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 308-318 | Received 10 May 2021, Accepted 27 Jun 2021, Published online: 19 Jul 2021

ABSTRACT

A patient suffering from visuo-spatial neglect was investigated as a special interest case during a study on the effectiveness of “restorative approaches” after visual field loss. This patient trained with our newly developed Virtual Reality (VR) system “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” for 254 days. Perimetric results show a visual field expansion of 48.8% (left eye) and 36.8% (right eye) translating to an improvement of approximately 5.5° to 10.5° of visual angle. Further, subjective self-report shows improvements of up to 317% in visual field functionality. Our results indicate that patients suffering from visuo-spatial neglect could benefit from a VR-based restorative intervention.

Introduction

Scientific background

Worldwide over 13 million new cases of stroke are reported annually, with stroke being the second major reason for disability (Lindsay et al., Citation2019). Approximately 50% of people suffering from stroke report visual problems, ranging from central visual issues to eye movement and perceptual disorders (Rowe et al., Citation2019). Besides visual field defects stemming from lesions in early/primary cortical areas, a widespread clinical disorder after stroke or trauma is visuospatial neglect stemming from lesions in higher cortical areas (Berti et al., Citation2015). Neglect describes a multicomponent complex disease with neuropsychological issues in the patients’ processing of spatial information, resulting in decreased awareness for the spatial side opposite to the lesioned cerebral area (e.g., Bisiach, Citation1996; Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021). Literature distinguishes between egocentric and allocentric neglect, with egocentric neglect being the more common form and left-sided neglect occurring twice as frequent than right-sided neglect (Bowen et al., Citation1999; Demeyere & Gillebert, Citation2019). Patients suffering from egocentric neglect “[…] fail to attend to the contralesional side of space (with reference to their own body midline) [while] allocentric neglect is where [patients] fail to attend to the contralesional side of an object in focus.” (Demeyere & Gillebert, Citation2019, p. 491). Studies show prevalence of neglect in older stroke patients (>60 years) ranging from 15% to 33.5% and 50% of stroke patients of all ages suffer left-sided neglect (Chen et al., Citation2015; Linden et al., Citation2005; Ten Brink et al., Citation2016). Visual field assessment is usually conducted with kinetic perimetry (e.g., “Goldmann Perimetry”) and/or static perimetry (e.g., “Humphrey Field Analyzer”) (Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021; Kolling et al., Citation2012).

Neuropsychological rehabilitation

There are two main approaches for patients suffering from visual defects. “Visual Field Training” describes a methodology that helps patients to learn strategies to compensate (thus also known as “Compensatory therapies”) for visual field defects by improvement of saccadic search patterns and increasing attention to blind areas. Conversely, “Visual Field Recovery” aims to reactivate/restitute lesioned areas (in the visual cortex) by repeated light stimulation which – supposedly – leads to neuronal reconnection (thus also known as “Restitution therapies”). While visual field defects originating from stroke or trauma are classified in the scientific literature as “perception deficit,” visuospatial neglect is widely described as “attention deficit” (e.g., Halligan et al., Citation2003; Lunven & Bartolomeo, Citation2017). Despite these differences in neurological location and neuropsychological functionality, similar rehabilitation routines are conducted in visual field defects and visuospatial neglect with both based on methodologies of Visual Field Training (e.g., Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021; Umarova et al., Citation2011). While Visual Field Training currently represents the “gold standard” in clinical rehabilitation, the true effect sizes of Visual Field Recovery are subject to discussion in the scientific community for years (e.g., Bouwmeester et al., Citation2007; Frolov et al., Citation2017). However, researchers agree that the need for visual rehabilitation and new approaches will further increase in the near future. Trauzettel-Klosinski (Citation2011) states that “suitable rehabilitative measures chosen after the thorough diagnostic evaluation of a visual impairment and analysis of its effects can usually restore reading ability, improve orientation, and thereby enhance the patient’s independence and quality of life. As the demand for visual rehabilitation is increasing, steps will need to be taken to make it more widely available.” (p. 871)

Controversy

Visual Field Recovery is based on the concept of “visual border stimulation,” the activation of “residual vision capacities,” with various proprietary developments from different research groups and companies. What unites all the concepts, however, is the hypothesis that – by stimulating the “border area” between the intact and the damaged visual area – neurons in the topographically corresponding area of the visual cortex can be re-activated (e.g., Sabel et al., Citation2013). Sabel et al. (Citation1997) reported that patients sometimes detect visual stimuli in the border to the intact visual field. The authors further suggest that 15% of remaining, functionally intact neurons are sufficient to stimulate activity in the affected area. This could, in turn, lead to plastic cerebral changes and result in a partial restitution of the visual field (Rosa et al., Citation2013).

Some studies reported considerable training effects with border stimulation programs indicating neuroplasticity in the visual cortex (Marshall et al., Citation2010; Mueller et al., Citation2007, Citation2003; Poggel et al., Citation2004; Sabel et al., Citation2005, Citation2004; Zihl & von Cranon, Citation1985). Mueller et al. (Citation2007), for example, reported that their training restored up to 17.2% of the formerly blind visual field of their patients. Marshall et al. (Citation2010) reported an average improvement rate of 12.5%.

Other studies, by contrast, did not find (significant) effects (Balliet et al., Citation1985; Glisson, Citation2006; Horton, Citation2005; Reinhard, Citation2005) and hence the effect of vision restoration is still controversially discussed (e.g., Pollock et al., Citation2019; Sabel & Trauzettel-Klosinski, Citation2005). Frolov et al. (Citation2017), for example, attest that there are “remaining nagging questions as to the validity of [published data] and the clinical benefit” (p. 40) and Kerkhoff et al. (Citation2021) states that “the overall evidence is unclear and thus restorative visual field training cannot generally be recommended […]” (p. 196). Horton (Citation2005) assumed in his review that those studies which found beneficial effects of Visual Field Recovery reported overly optimistic outcomes. The reason for this skepticism is the imprecision of the diagnostic instruments which are traditionally used to assess the extent of the visual field loss (e.g., Goldmann Perimetry and/or Humphrey Field Analyzer). Thus, traditional diagnostics systems have major limitations in reliably assessing the potential effects of Visual Field Recovery: i) they lack a stringent fixation control; ii) they allow the patient to (unconsciously) resort to compensation strategies (Schreiber et al., Citation2006); and iii) – in case of manual perimetry – are prone to a certain degree of subjectivity of the examiner.

Against this background, we developed an eye tracking based-perimetric methodology for highly accurate pre-, post-, and follow-up assessments, i.e., “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA) (Leitner, Hutzler et al., Citation2021). The EFA is based on static automated perimetry while additionally taking individual eye movements (with the help of an eye-tracker) in real time into account and compensating for them. The EFA provides a standard error of measurement (SEM) of 0.44° of visual angle. Based on participants’ individual results, the EFA provides disattenuated correlation (validity) with established perimetric instruments of 1.00. Results from patients suffering from cortical lesions imply that the EFA is applicable for clinical use and pre-post-assessment enabling the finding of small changes (>0.5°) in patients’ visual fields.

Virtual reality

Recently, various research groups began developing and studying perimetric and therapeutic procedures for patients suffering from visual field defects based on virtual reality and mobile concepts. The shared vision is to use so-called “Head Mounted (Virtual Reality) Devices” (VR) and/or mobile applications (based on tablets or cell phones) as valid and reliable tools for diagnostic and therapeutic purposes (e.g., Goh et al., Citation2018; Leitner, Guetlin et al., Citation2021; Matsumoto et al., Citation2016; Wroblewski et al., Citation2014). The goal is to ease and improve both diagnostics and intervention for healthcare professionals and patients alike. Different studies imply that VR and mobile visual field assessment tools (e.g., implemented on iPads) are indeed an alternative for established, stationary tools. Anderson et al. (Citation2017) showed that “Detecting rapid visual field progression may be improved using a home-monitoring strategy, even when compliance is imperfect.” (p. 1735). For example, Schulz et al. (Citation2017) state that iPad-based threshold perimetry shows “[…] reasonable sensitivity/specificity […]” (p. 346) and Nesaratnam et al. (Citation2017) envisage that “[…] such a test having a role in assessing bed-bound patients in hospital where access to formal visual field testing is difficult, or indeed in rapid testing of visual fields at the bedside […]” (p. 1). This approach is particularly helpful for stroke patients who are bedridden or immobile (Spofforth et al., Citation2017). Sand et al. (Citation2012) additionally emphasize that the focus in daily neurological practice and rehabilitation is mainly on motor symptoms and that visual impairments must receive more consideration in order to improve patients’ independence and quality of life. Because VR or tablets offer various protocols for connectivity (e.g., 5 G, 4 G, 3 G, WIFI, Bluetooth, NFC, etc.), this hardware can be implemented effortlessly in telemedical routines. In this context, Johnson et al. (Citation2017) showed that mobile perimetric applications produce valid and reliable results in areas with limited medical care. This progress represents a major opportunity for people in developing countries or medically poorly served areas to receive orthoptic and neuropsychological care and rehabilitation, prevent degenerative illnesses and monitor therapeutic progress. In general, VR constitutes an evolution that will continue to develop in stroke and orthoptic research but also in various other academic fields such as in traumatic brain injury, Parkinson disease, or autism over the next few years (e.g., Canning et al., Citation2020; Cortés-Pérez et al., Citation2020; Maggio et al., Citation2019; Qazi & Raza, Citation2020; Spreij et al., Citation2020).

In this light, we developed a portable VR solution for neuropsychological rehabilitation to both investigate and enable the effectiveness of therapeutic interventions on visual field defects (Leitner, Guetlin et al., Citation2021). Results on the reliability and validity of the device indicate that the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) is highly accurate in displaying visual stimuli in predefined areas of the users’ visual field, which is a fundamental requirement for the potential effects of rehabilitation approaches such as Visual Field Recovery. Specifically, SVFT provides an overall accuracy of .990 (sensitivity of .982 and specificity of .994). Concurrently, feedback from patients further indicates that neurorehabilitative training with VR is practical, easy, and comfortable to perform (in contrast to setups based on classical “Head-Chin Rests” solutions).

Aim

In our current research project, we found indications that patients suffering from neglect might significantly benefit from Visual Field Recovery when training with SVFT. Due to the scope of our research project, we originally excluded patients suffering from visual neglect from our experimental design and concentrated solely on patients suffering from clearly definable defects in early visual cortex (e.g., infarct of the arteria cerebri posterior). However, we later decided to include one patient (i.e., “Patient 7”) – who suffers from visual neglect after trauma (see below for details) – as a designated special interest case. We hypothesized that the highly focused and frequently repeated training situation in VR – due to its closed design covering both eyes and, in this way, leading to intense confrontation with the training task – could potentially improve attentional mechanisms in neglect patients (Azouvi et al., Citation2017; Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021; Kerkhoff & Schenk, Citation2012). In the following case report, we present our findings.

Methods

Patient involvement, privacy, and ethical considerations

Patient 7 was not directly involved in the design of this study. Her ID number is not known to anyone outside the research group. Written informed consent (to report all amnestic, clinical, and diagnostic details in this manuscript) was obtained from Patient 7. The neuropsychological intervention, the ophthalmologic assessment, and the concept of the study was approved by the ethics committee of the University of Salzburg. The present study is guided by the fundamental principles of respect for the individual, the right to self-determination, and the right to make informed decisions, i.e., informed consent, as noted in the Declaration of Helsinki. The study is registered in the ICMJE-approved registry “German Clinical Trials Register” (DRKS00025205).

Patient characteristics

Patient 7 is a 40 to 45 years old woman who suffered traumatic brain injury from an accident in the mid of 2018 with extensive posterior medial infarct and posterior infarct on the right with midline shift (ICD: I63.4) and posterior medial infarct on the left and right cerebellar infarct in the AICA flow area resulting in a visual field defect (quadrantanopia lower right and lower left) with egocentric visual neglect symptomatic to the lower left side.

From a neuropsychological perspective, Patient 7 is conscious, oriented in time, place, and situation. She is unobtrusive in her general perception and attention, her concentration capacity is undisturbed, there are no memory or retentive disorders, and she shows no signs of anosognosia. Her speech production is fluent and well articulated, her speech comprehension is undisturbed and requests from healthcare professionals are executed promptly and adequately. There are no signs of meningismus, her pupillomotor functionality is undisturbed and pupils are bilaterally isochor and normal light reactive. Patient 7 shows a slightly saccadic gaze sequence, but no gaze paresis or indications of double vision with age adequate accommodation. Patient 7 showed significant difficulties in the “Clock Drawing Test”. Drawing from 12 to 6 o’clock was uneventful; however, the patient did not know what to do next and stopped the test. After some further questioning, the patient first drew 8, 4, and 3 in the left half of the clock face. After repeated questions and assistance, the patient was able to complete the clock. In a second task, the (horizontally performed) “Line Bisection Test,” Patient 7 showed no significant abnormalities.

Design, assessment, and rehabilitation

Patient 7 was told that the effectiveness of the therapeutic approach on her condition is unknown. Further, no outcomes regarding perimetric results were communicated during the study but fully discussed after the final assessment date.

The inner 10° of visual field of Patient 7 was diagnosed monocularly in detail with the help of the EFA on July 14th, 2020 at the “Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience” of the University of Salzburg. Patient 7’s vision was corrected for acuity during all visual field tests. Based on these diagnostic results, the SVFT was configured to present bright light stimuli onto her visual border area, which is between the intact and defect visual field of Patient 7 (see ). In a total training duration of 254 days, the visual field of Patient 7 was assessed in regular intervals (M = 51 days (SD = 17)) with the help of the EFA (6 assessments in total). When significant changes in the visual field (> 3°) after each assessment were found, the training stimuli were rearranged accordingly.

Figure 1. Illustration of placement of 38 training stimuli (yellow-dotted circles) along the border area of the intact (green dots) and defect (red dots) inner 10° of the binocular visual field of Patient 7, as assessed with the help of the “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA). The illustration shows training stimuli arrangement after the first visual field assessment with the EFA on July 14th, 2020

Figure 1. Illustration of placement of 38 training stimuli (yellow-dotted circles) along the border area of the intact (green dots) and defect (red dots) inner 10° of the binocular visual field of Patient 7, as assessed with the help of the “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA). The illustration shows training stimuli arrangement after the first visual field assessment with the EFA on July 14th, 2020

Training stimuli had a size of 3° and a luminance of 1,000 cd/m2, appearing for 750 ms in randomized order on a dark background. The presentation of each stimulus was delayed for 2000 ms (plus a randomized duration of up to 1500 ms) to give Patient 7 time to react to the displayed stimulus via remote control. This is necessary as the SVFT runs a “stimulus-location adaptation process” during training sessions which is based on user feedback by clicking (or not) the remote control during the delay after every displayed stimulus. If the user detects the stimulus peripherally, its location is moved for 0.8° toward a custom reference point within the central area of the defect visual area. This allows the system to automatically adapt to an improving visual field or small inaccuracies from minor gaze deviations from the fixation cross. On the contrary, if the stimulus is not detected, it is moved away from the reference point for 0.4° toward the center of the patient’s intact visual field, balancing out the therapeutic process in case of inaccuracies, misclicks, or loss of already gained training progress. Thus, this behavioral procedure produces a steady approximation of the “real” border across the intact and defect visual field and successively compensates for potential minor gaze inaccuracies of the patient during the training session procedure (Leitner, Guetlin et al., Citation2021). After 30 minutes of training, the SVFT ends automatically.

Patient 7 was instructed to train twice a day for 6 days a week and extensively informed about the procedure and methodology of Visual Field Recovery and regarding the functionality of the SVFT on the first assessment date (for details see Leitner, Guetlin et al., Citation2021). At the end of the first appointment, the SVFT was handed over to Patient 7 and an appointment was made for the first interim measurement. The study called for Patient 7 to come into our laboratory at the University of Salzburg approximately every 2 months for visual field testing with the EFA. However, this plan could not be fully adhered to due to the COVID-19 pandemic, which is why some appointments had to be shifted back-and-forth by a few weeks over the next months. Nonetheless, this led to a mean duration between assessment appointments of 51 days (SD = 17).

Measures and analysis

Changes in the visual field of Patient 7 are calculated and displayed in two different ways. These are as follows: 1) Percentage change of visual field functionality is based on the ratio of perceived (green dots) and unperceived stimuli (red dots) between the first (0 days) and the last assessment day (254 days), as well between assessment days (0, 64, 105, 182, 217, 254). Areas that were not assessed at the first appointment were not considered for the calculation of indicated percent change – even if these areas were additionally tested in later assessment days due to significant changes in visual field functionality. As a result, potential improvements tend to be underestimated rather than overestimated. 2) Apparently improved regions in the visual field of Patient 7 are marked in the visual field plots with yellow rectangles. Horizontal and vertical changes of these areas are calculated and reported in degree of visual angle (°). A prerequisite for this calculation is a coherent image of the enhancement of larger contiguous – former blind areas – of at least 3°. Areas that do not qualify for calculation but tend to show apparent improvements are reported but no specific calculation for degree of visual angle is carried out.

In addition to the objective perimetric measurement of potential improvements in the visual field functionality of Patient 7 – which was done with the help of the EFA – we were also especially interested in the subjective valuation of Patient 7 regarding her visual capacities. Thus, before every EFA assessment in our lab, Patient 7 was asked to rate the status of her visual field on a continuum subjectively, ranging from “very bad” to “very good.” In order not to influence Patient 7’s subjective self-assessment performed before each EFA assessment; the results of the visual field results were not reported to her between appointments. After the last appointment, the visual field results were communicated to Patient 7 and discussed in detail.

Results

Visual field test results from the EFA after training for approximately 8 months with the SVFT show an expansion of Patient 7’s visual field of 48.8% (OS) and 36.8% (OD). This translates to a horizontal expansion of approximately 7.25° and vertical expansion of approximately 5.5° of visual angle for the upper left area in OS and to a horizontal expansion of approximately 10.5° and vertical expansion of approximately 5.5° of visual angle for the lower right area in OD (both marked as yellow rectangles in ). Improvements marked by the lower right rectangle in OS are too widespread and diffuse to be translated into degree of visual angle, however reporting of overall improvement in this area appears worth noticing. In this context, also the unsystematic improvements in the center of the defect area, which are (naturally) not yet locatable during baseline assessment, are remarkable. Because the EFA repeats undetected stimuli – after displaying a random stimulus that is not recorded – the probability of improvements by random patient’s reaction is highly unlikely. Also, the EFA documents if patients are “trigger happy”, which is not the case with this patient (also see ). Dots in dark red represent stimuli not detected the first time but detected the second time. These stimuli usually occur near “visual border areas,” situated between defect and functional visual field areasand are also observable in the visual field results of Patient 7.

Figure 2. Illustration of visual field tests (inner 10°) – conducted with our perimetric “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA) – after 217 days of therapy with our “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) of Patient 7 suffering from quadrantanopia and visual neglect symptomatic. Red dots represent stimuli not detected, green dots represent stimuli detected during perimetric assessment. One stimulus stands for approximately 0.75° of visual angle. Note the obvious improvements in stimulus detection – marked by the yellow rectangles – at both sides of the respective visual field defects. Data analysis (by total numbers of undetected stimuli based on baseline assessment) shows total visual field enlargement of 48.8% (OS) and 36.8% (OD) and gradual improvement especially during the first 64 days of therapy (also see )

Figure 2. Illustration of visual field tests (inner 10°) – conducted with our perimetric “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA) – after 217 days of therapy with our “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) of Patient 7 suffering from quadrantanopia and visual neglect symptomatic. Red dots represent stimuli not detected, green dots represent stimuli detected during perimetric assessment. One stimulus stands for approximately 0.75° of visual angle. Note the obvious improvements in stimulus detection – marked by the yellow rectangles – at both sides of the respective visual field defects. Data analysis (by total numbers of undetected stimuli based on baseline assessment) shows total visual field enlargement of 48.8% (OS) and 36.8% (OD) and gradual improvement especially during the first 64 days of therapy (also see Figure 3)

Figure 3. Illustration of test statistics of “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA) of the left eye (OS) and the right eye (OD) of Patient 7 after 0, 64, 105, 182, 217 and 254 days of neuropsychological training with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) (x-axis) in absolute numbers of presented test stimuli (left y-axis). The ratio between true positive and true negative stimuli shows improvements in visual functions (yellow line) due to repeated neuropsychological training with the SVFT (right y-axis)

Figure 3. Illustration of test statistics of “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA) of the left eye (OS) and the right eye (OD) of Patient 7 after 0, 64, 105, 182, 217 and 254 days of neuropsychological training with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) (x-axis) in absolute numbers of presented test stimuli (left y-axis). The ratio between true positive and true negative stimuli shows improvements in visual functions (yellow line) due to repeated neuropsychological training with the SVFT (right y-axis)

The behavior of Patient 7 during visual field assessment – represented in the EFA’s test statistics – shows no abnormalities, with false-negative and false-positive rates within usual and similar ranges comparable to standard perimetric analysis of Patient 7 with the Humphrey Field Analyzer (see ).

Analysis of the SVFT training log shows that Patient 7 trained consistently at least twice for 30 minutes per day over the entire period. Specifically, Patient 7 performed 136 training sessions within the first 64 days, 88 training sessions within the following 41 days, 171 training sessions within the following 77 days, 104 training sessions within the following 35 days and 111 training sessions within the last 37 days. This results in an average of 2.1 training sessions per day for the first period, 2.1 training sessions per day for the second period, 2.2 training sessions per day for the third period and 3.0 training sessions per day for the fourth and fifth period. This translates to a total mean of 2.5 training sessions per day with the SVFT for the entire period of 254 days. Additional results show that the perceptual improvements of Patient 7 – because of the training with the SVFT – are not only objectively measurable via the EFA but also reported by the patient herself (see ). These results further indicate that Patient 7 feels that she is benefiting from the rehabilitation program and that her visual (-attentional) capabilities improve. We interpret the subjective worsening at the last test time point as a reaction to the preceding announcement of the research team to temporarily stop the therapeutic intervention to evaluate the data of the study and to plan further steps. According to her own statements after assessment, the patient would have liked to continue training with the device.

Figure 4. Illustration of subjective self-report (left y-axis) of visual field improvement of Patient 7 after 0, 64, 105, 182, 217 and 254 days of training (x-axis) with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT). Results show improvements in self-evaluation (on a continuum (right y-axis) ranging from “very bad” (0%) to “very good” (100%)) of visual field functionality and confirms objective data from repeated perimetric assessment with the “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA)

Figure 4. Illustration of subjective self-report (left y-axis) of visual field improvement of Patient 7 after 0, 64, 105, 182, 217 and 254 days of training (x-axis) with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT). Results show improvements in self-evaluation (on a continuum (right y-axis) ranging from “very bad” (0%) to “very good” (100%)) of visual field functionality and confirms objective data from repeated perimetric assessment with the “Eye Tracking Based Visual Field Analysis” (EFA)

illustrates visual field assessment of Patient 7 with the Humphrey Field Analyzer and Goldmann Perimeter before and after neuropsychological training with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT). Initial results indicate a broad visual field defect below the horizontal line. Test statistics (SITA-Fast) from the Humphrey Field Analyzer show 10% false-positive errors, 11% false negative errors, 1 fixation loss and an assessment duration of 6 minutes for the right eye. For the left eye there are 0% false-positive errors, 0% false negative errors, 0 fixation losses and an assessment duration of 5 minutes 28 seconds. After training with the SVFT, results from the Humphrey Field Analyzer indicate no substantial changes in visual field functionality. Test statistics (SITA-Fast) show 3% false-positive errors, 10% false negative errors, 0 fixation losses and an assessment duration of 5 minutes 24 seconds for the right eye. For the left eye there are 0% false-positive errors, 0% false negative errors, 0 fixation losses and an assessment duration of 6 minutes 1 second. However, results from Goldmann perimetry indicate – following assessment results from the EFA (see ) – that visual field functionality of Patient 7 increased considerably after neuropsychological training with the SVFT. Especially, wide areas in the lower left quadrant indicate improvement. In addition, lower areas within the inner 10° of visual field appear to have improved for around 5° of visual angle. During both visual field-testing sessions with the Goldmann Perimeter, Patient 7 was calm, concentrated and acted consistently.

Figure 5. Illustration of visual field test with Humphrey Field Analyzer and Goldmann Perimetry before and after training with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT). While the results of the Humphrey Field Analyzer do not suggest any significant changes in the visual field of Patient 7, the results of Goldmann Perimetry indicate noticeable improvements. Particularly in areas on the lower left and in lower areas of the inner 10° of visual angle, apparent improvements can be observed

Note: To increase visibility and interpretability of the Goldmann Perimetry plot, the red and blue markers were digitally redrawn. Blue lines represent Goldmann marker size I and red lines size III, both with a relative intensity of 1.00.
Figure 5. Illustration of visual field test with Humphrey Field Analyzer and Goldmann Perimetry before and after training with the “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT). While the results of the Humphrey Field Analyzer do not suggest any significant changes in the visual field of Patient 7, the results of Goldmann Perimetry indicate noticeable improvements. Particularly in areas on the lower left and in lower areas of the inner 10° of visual angle, apparent improvements can be observed

Patient 7 reported mild dizziness that occurred especially at the beginning – after finishing training sessions with our virtual reality device SVFT. After a few weeks, however, this no longer occurred. Patient 7 also reported that the training sometimes made her tired, especially in the evening. Otherwise, Patient 7 reported no unwanted or unpleasant side effects or adverse events. Rather, she related that the compact and flexible design of the training device greatly facilitated the rehabilitation measures, and that the device was comfortable to wear.

Discussion

Reliable and conclusive evidence on the effect of neuropsychological interventions to improve visual field defects is scarce and still subject to debate in the scientific community. In our current research project, we aim to test different clinical interventions for their effectiveness and strive to provide new, evidence-based approaches to established methodologies. To create the basic conditions for this we have developed two new tools in the field of diagnosis as well as in the field of rehabilitation. On the one hand, the perimetric tool “Eye tracking based visual field analysis” (EFA) helps us to obtain reliable and accurate assessment results on the patients' current visual field status as well as regarding potential changes after intervention (Leitner, Hutzler et al., Citation2021). On the other hand, the neuropsychological tool “Salzburg Visual Field Trainer” (SVFT) creates the virtual reality-based foundation for an accurate, controlled, and immersive rehabilitation situation (Leitner, Guetlin et al., Citation2021). We are currently concentrating on questions regarding “restitutive procedures”, as utilized in so-called “Visual Field Recovery” approaches. Since the findings in this context can be described as controversial, we decided to pay strict attention to the neurological background of the respective individual visual field defects when selecting patients. Therefore, we focus mainly on clearly definable “classical” visual field defects that originate in early cortical areas (e.g., stemming from insult of the arteria cerebri posterior) and can be classified as pure “perceptual disorder”. It is our suspicion that earlier studies – besides inaccurate pre-, post-, and follow-up diagnostics, which are due to compensatory eye movements in the course of perimetric examinations – did not elaborate on the exact neurological and neuropsychological background of the visual field defects. Specifically, we suspect that i.a., patients with early and higher cortical lesions were mixed in these studies. In addition, it is often difficult and time-consuming to differentiate between scotomas that occur either due to perceptual issues or attentional issues. This would explain the incoherent picture that emerges from literature and textbooks. To further investigate the results presented in this manuscript, advancing studies with a larger number of patients suffering from visuospatial neglect is currently underway.

Interpretation

The results of the present case study suggest that patients suffering from neglect could benefit from a virtual reality-based neuropsychological intervention based on restitutive concepts. Interestingly, the Visual Field Recovery intervention we used for Patient 7 was not originally intended for neglect patients as they suffer from lesions in higher cortical regions. The primary understanding of the scientific community and the clinical gold standard here is the use of so-called “compensatory procedures” (e.g., Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021). This means that in the course of training, patients are encouraged to make conscious eye movements into the blind field to improve awareness for this area. In contrast, “restitutive procedures” – as utilized in the SVFT in this study – rely on continuous fixation of a central point, thus forcing decentralized, peripheral awareness of the training stimuli displayed between defect and intact visual field. The background of this approach is – as mentioned above – the hypothesis that these training stimuli reactivate the respective lesioned, topographically equivalent regions in early areas of the visual cortex. To date, we are not aware of any other study that has treated patients with neglect symptoms using an immersive procedure such as we did with a virtual reality device, combined with a restitutive procedure. Conversely, current literature reviews show mainly studies concentrating on visual scanning or pursuit trainings, neck-muscle vibration therapy, prism adaptation, visuo-motor feedback or other forms of attention training combined with magnetic or current stimulation (Kerkhoff et al., Citation2021; Pollock et al., Citation2019)

We interpret the results of Patient 7 after intervention as improvements in both the right lower quadrant and the left lower quadrant. Based on perimetric analysis and changes after therapy, we hypothesize that Patient 7 suffers from a quadrant anopia to the right with primarily leftward (but also partially rightward) neglect components. Results from Goldmann Perimetry after intervention seem to support this hypothesis. Further, these results from the Goldmann Perimeter additionally hold another interesting component as they indicate that areas outside the actual training area of the SVFT seem to have improved during therapy. Because these areas are consistent with the training direction of the presented stimuli in the SVFT, we hypothesize that these ameliorations may represent an extension of Patient 7’s generally improved perceptual ability to the (lower) left – ranging even into outer areas of her visual field.

Regarding the results from the Humphrey Field Analyzer, indicating no considerable changes in the visual field of Patient 7 we hypothesize that the diagnostics algorithm, the overall accuracy and results illustration of this device are too imprecise and coarsely. Additionally, some areas of improvement – as indicated by results from the Goldmann Perimeter – lie outside the assessment area of the Humphrey Field Analyzer (>30°). As the Goldmann Perimeter is still considered as the “gold standard” in perimetric assessment, we interpret the results from this device as confirmation of the results found with the EFA.

Limitations

Our findings are limiting due to the nature of a case study, but concurrently indicate the potential of a virtual reality-based neuropsychological intervention method that is based on a restitutive concept. Thus, at this point no generalized statements about the therapeutic effect size of our presented virtual reality training are valid. However, our findings warrant further and closer investigation (with a larger sample size) of the potential effective factors of the neuropsychological intervention that contributed to Patient 7 benefitting from an improved visual field both objectively and subjectively.

Another limitation of the present work relates to the functional improvement of patient 7’s daily life during/after therapy with the SVFT. In discussions after each assessment appointment, the patient’s general life situation and her coping with the visual field defect were discussed. However, Patient 7 did not report any conspicuous improvements in her daily life, although she consistently rated her visual impairment as improving over the training period. Thus, the present study does not provide structured and objectified data on the practical implications of Patient 7’s life situation during/after training with the SVFT. This area will also be further investigated and analyzed in continuing studies with larger numbers of participants.

Conclusion

Our findings suggest that a virtual reality supported neuropsychological intervention based on restitutive concepts for patients suffering from visual neglect could improve visual field functionality and related visual perception after stroke or trauma.

Ethics approval

The following study was reviewed and approved by the ethics commission of the University of Salzburg (Reference No. 39/2018)

Competing interests

All authors declare that they have no conflict of interest and approve the manuscript.

The sponsor or funding organization had no role in the design or conduct of this research.

Contribution

All authors contributed equally to this work.

Acknowledgments

The authors sincerely thank Anja-Maria Ladek and Lydia Hell from the University Hospital Salzburg for Ophthalmology and Optometry for their medical and orthoptic support and Sarah Schuster for proof-reading and critical comment on the work.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

Data is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Additional information

Funding

The present study is based on the funded project “Advanced Perimetry for the Evaluation of Neuroplasticity in the Visual Cortex” by the Austrian Science Fund (FWF) (Reference No. P31299)

References