6,126
Views
55
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Psychological determinants of financial buffer saving: the influence of financial risk tolerance and regulatory focus

, ORCID Icon &
Pages 1076-1093 | Received 15 Jun 2015, Accepted 23 Nov 2015, Published online: 26 Feb 2016

Abstract

With governments redistributing more responsibilities unto citizens, individuals have an increasing need for financial resources acting as a buffer against life’s setbacks and unexpected expenditures. The purpose of this study was to examine psychological determinants of saving for a financial buffer, for which a theoretical model was formulated based on the theory of planned behaviour with three new, domain-specific psychological constructs: financial risk tolerance, regulatory focus and perceived saving barriers. Data were collected with an online questionnaire that utilised convenience and snowball sampling to target both students and working individuals (N = 272). Regression analyses offered support for the proposed model, showing that participants’ financial risk tolerance (i.e. an individual’s attitude towards financial risk taking) was significantly associated with their subjective financial knowledge and regulatory focus. Furthermore, perceived financial self-efficacy and financial risk tolerance both predicted participants’ intention to save for a financial buffer. In turn, perceived financial self-efficacy and saving intention predicted self-reported saving behaviour. Importantly, perceived saving barriers mediated the relationship between saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour. In line with the proposed model, results also showed that a specific attitude-based construct (financial risk tolerance) is a considerably better predictor of saving intention than general measures of attitude towards saving. This study is also the first to demonstrate that regulatory focus influences financial risk tolerance. Implications of these findings for stimulating saving behaviour are discussed.

Introduction

A recent movement towards individual responsibility for managing risks, which increasingly encourages individuals to take self-protective measures against calamities (Veldheer et al. Citation2012), has been observed in countries such as Australia and Britain (Hamilton Citation2012) and the Netherlands (Veldheer et al. Citation2012). This shift, inspired by both ideological and monetary reasons (Veldheer et al. Citation2012), is visible in areas ranging from flood management (Kievik and Gutteling Citation2011) to crime prevention (van Steden, van Caem, and Boutellier Citation2011). While people are capable of taking protective measures (see Martin, Bender, and Raish Citation2007), and such investments can reduce damage by calamities considerably (Kreibich et al. Citation2005), an essential prerequisite remains an individual’s financial resources.

Unsurprisingly, governmental risk-mitigation strategies have already been criticised for increasing individual risks during economic downturns (see Chan Citation2006). One particular critique is that incomes have dropped in recent years while individual responsibilities have increased (Chan Citation2006), reducing individuals’ financial resources. But resources like a financial buffer serve as a personal risk management strategy against financial setbacks, which range from unemployment and unexpected medical costs to necessary expenditures on a home or vehicle (Babiarz and Robb Citation2013). Furthermore, a financial buffer also provides psychological benefits: individuals with less than $500 in emergency savings, compared to those who have more than this, are more likely to frequently worry, lose sleep, suffer from health problems and have a lower work productivity (Brobeck Citation2008). Conversely, positive financial behaviours (including saving) predict improved subjective well-being (Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012).

However, many individuals have insufficient financial resources, with even 15% of Dutch (NIBUD Citation2012) and 22% of non-retired American households (Fisher and Anong Citation2012) having no savings at all. Accordingly, 25% of Americans report they cannot come up with $2000 in 30 days, while an additional 19% would only be able to after selling or pawning possessions or taking payday loans (Lusardi, Schneider, and Tufano Citation2011). These findings are alarming, especially given the unknown timing of many expenditures related to adverse events (e.g. job loss, medical costs) (Babiarz and Robb Citation2013). This questions how self-reliant individuals can be when financial resources are scarce or simply not available.

This study aims to explain self-reported saving behaviour for a financial buffer with a model derived from the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB), extended with constructs we consider particularly relevant to the domain of financial buffer saving: financial risk tolerance and regulatory focus. The model may help to inform and guide individual risk management strategies to improve saving for a financial buffer.

Theory

The Theory of Planned Behaviour

The TPB postulates that behaviours over which people have incomplete volitional control are predictable with measures like intention, perceived behavioural control (PBC) and attitude (Ajzen Citation1991, Citation2002; Armitage and Conner Citation2001; Conner and Armitage Citation1998). In short, it assumes first that behavioural, normative and control beliefs predict the extent to which a more favourable attitude, positive subjective norm towards a specific behaviour, and a greater PBC over the behaviour exist in the individual, respectively (Ajzen Citation1991, Citation2002; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). In turn, a more favourable attitude, positive subjective norm and greater PBC predict a stronger intention to perform the behaviour in question (Ajzen Citation1991). Finally, the behavioural intention is seen as a key predictor of actual behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991, Citation2002; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). The TPB has been validated in a wide range of domains (Ajzen Citation1991; Armitage and Conner Citation2001; Conner and Armitage Citation1998), including financial behaviours like budget keeping (Kidwell and Turrisi Citation2004), retirement saving (Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010), saving habits (Loibl, Kraybill, and DeMay Citation2011), negative financial behaviours like not paying bills (Xiao et al. Citation2011) and even future financial behaviours such as saving (Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012). However, it has not yet been applied to financial buffer saving. To generate a TPB-based model for that behaviour, we added several novel constructs (like financial risk tolerance and regulatory focus) to the existing framework, which resulting in the following model we elaborate on below (Figure ).

Figure 1. Proposed model.

Figure 1. Proposed model.

Financial risk tolerance as a specific operationalisation of attitude

The TPB asserts that attitude towards a behaviour influences the intention to perform the behaviour and reflects an individual’s global evaluation of the behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). TPB-based research into financial behaviour, like budget keeping (Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; Kidwell and Turrisi Citation2004), saving (Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010) and positive financial behaviours (Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012), indeed show that attitude influences intention. But the magnitude of this influence also varies considerably, even with the same financial behaviour (e.g. budget keeping: β = .10, Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; β = .44, Kidwell and Turrisi Citation2004). There are several possible explanations for this. First, a broad attitude might not be a good predictor of behaviour. Attitude can, after all, measure general knowledge instead of predicting a particular, personal intention (Ajzen et al. Citation2011). Second, risks are approached differently per domain (Nicholson et al. Citation2005; Weber, Blais, and Betz Citation2002), with for example investment risk propensity unrelated to recreational risk propensity but correlating positively with social risk propensity (Markiewicz and Weber Citation2013), implying that attitude towards risk affects different domain-specific intentions.

Those explanations suggest that behaviour is better measured with a specific, instead of a general, attitude. One possible specific measure of attitude is financial risk tolerance, which is defined as the willingness to engage in financial behaviours with uncertain outcomes that have an identifiable negative outcome, and thereby indicates the amount of financial uncertainty someone is willing to accept (Grable Citation2000; Grable and Lytton Citation1999, Citation2003; Grable, Lytton, and O’Neill Citation2004; Grable et al. Citation2009). While financial risk tolerance seems unexamined in relation to saving intention, it does predict actual risk taking and risk avoiding behaviour (Gilliam, Chatterjee, and Grable Citation2010; Grable, Britt, and Webb Citation2008). We included this construct in our model to see whether it can predict saving intention with the assumption that individuals less tolerant of financial risks exhibit a stronger saving intention.

Subjective financial knowledge

We further extend the TPB with subjective financial knowledge (i.e. the perception of one’s financial knowledge) as a predictor of financial risk tolerance. While objective (i.e. accurate) financial knowledge relates to more saving (Babiarz and Robb Citation2013; Mahdzan and Tabiani Citation2013), it only explains modest amounts of variance in self-reported financial behaviours (Lusardi Citation2008; Lusardi and Mitchell Citation2005). Furthermore, knowledge about behaviour has little to do with actually performing it (Ajzen et al. Citation2011). Subjective knowledge, however, has a greater impact on behaviour (Lusardi, Keller, and Keller Citation2009; Wang Citation2009; Xiao et al. Citation2011). Since several TPB-based studies found that subjective financial knowledge predicts financial attitude (Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010; Kidwell and Turrisi Citation2004; Xiao et al. Citation2011), our model assumes that individuals with more subjective financial knowledge have a greater tolerance of financial risks.

Situational economic trust

While it seems that situational trust (i.e. trust in a specific situation or action; Viljanen Citation2005) is unresearched in relation to financial behaviours, there is hardly any economic decision that does not involve some degree of trust (Olsen Citation2012). Furthermore, several situational factors affect saving, ranging from recessions (Crossley, Low, and O’Dea Citation2013) and financial crises (O’Neill and Xiao Citation2012) to the risk of divorce (González and Özcan Citation2013; Pericoli and Ventura Citation2011), health deterioration (Macé Citation2012) and the unemployment of relatives (Tokuoka Citation2013). Generally speaking, income uncertainty has a positive impact on saving behaviour (Alessie and Teppa Citation2009; Fisher Citation2010). We therefore added a measure of economic trust to the model, with the assumption that high situational trust (i.e. displaying optimism about one’s financial situation) predicts a greater financial risk tolerance.

Regulatory focus

Regulatory focus theory distinguishes between two motivational states: promotion and prevention focus (Halamish et al. Citation2008). Promotion-focused individuals are concerned with advancement, growth, potential gains and accomplishment, while prevention-focused individuals concern themselves with safety, potential losses, impediments to goal achievement and responsibility (Crowe and Higgins Citation1997; Freitas et al. Citation2002; Halamish et al. Citation2008; Higgins et al. Citation2001; Lockwood, Jordan, and Kunda Citation2002). In the context of financial behaviour, regulatory focus moderates the asymmetry people typically experience between financial losses and gains (i.e. losses are experienced stronger than gains of the same magnitude; Tversky and Kahneman Citation1992). That is, individuals with a prevention focus experience this asymmetry stronger than those with a promotion focus (Halamish et al. Citation2008; Idson, Liberman, and Higgins Citation2000). To examine whether regulatory focus influences financial risk tolerance and saving intention, we added this construct to the model with the assumptions that prevention-focused individuals display a lower tolerance for financial risks and exhibit a stronger saving intention.

Subjective saving norms

Subjective norms refer to an individual’s perceptions of general social pressure to perform, or not perform, a specific behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). Empirically, social norms influence financial behaviours like retirement saving (Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010) and money management (Cude et al. Citation2006). Furthermore, positive parental norms predict students’ intention to perform financial behaviours like saving (Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012; Xiao et al. Citation2011) and positive subjective norms also predict a stronger retirement saving intention (Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012). As such, subjective saving norms were added to the model as a predictor of saving intention: positive subjective saving norms are expected to predict a stronger saving intention.

Perceived financial self-efficacy

The TPB’s PBC is the individual’s perception of the extent to which performing the behaviour is easy or difficult (Ajzen Citation1991). This concept can be considered interchangeable with self-efficacy (see Ajzen Citation1991; Conner and Armitage Citation1998), with this latter having a similar correlation strength as PBC has with intention (Armitage and Conner Citation2001) while being more clearly defined (see Ajzen Citation2002; Armitage and Conner Citation2001; Conner and Armitage Citation1998), making self-efficacy the preferred measure of PBC (Armitage and Conner Citation2001).

Self-efficacy is an individual’s perception of one’s ability to perform a certain behaviour in dealing with a threat or challenge (Bandura Citation1977), and, when applied to the financial domain, the amount of control and ability one feels when dealing with money issues (Dietz, Carrozza, and Neal Ritchey Citation2003). Empirically, stimulating self-efficacy with an easy-to-follow saving plan increases saving behaviour considerably (Lusardi, Keller, and Keller Citation2009) and, in terms of TPB research, financial self-efficacy is a positive predictor of saving (Xiao et al. Citation2011). The model therefore assumes that perceived financial self-efficacy influences saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour: individuals with a high financial self-efficacy are expected to have both a stronger saving intention and to report more saving behaviour.

Saving intention, saving barriers and saving behaviour

Intention is a central factor in the TPB and assumed to directly influence behaviour due to its indication of how much effort people are willing to exert to perform a specific behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). Ergo, the stronger the intention to engage in behaviour, the more likely its performance should be (Ajzen Citation1991). But for behaviours not under complete volitional control, the availability of resources and opportunities (e.g. time, money) is likely to influence the ability to perform the behaviour as well (Ajzen Citation1991). Consequently, TPB’s intention alone explains little variance in self-reported saving behaviour (e.g. R2 = .08; Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012), while economic variables, like disposable income, explain much more (e.g. R2 = .48 with self-reported recurring saving; Lunt and Livingstone Citation1991).

Two psychological mechanisms, both seemingly not yet addressed in the literature, might explain this discrepancy. First, saving barriers, such as economic conditions (see Fisher Citation2010; Lunt and Livingstone Citation1991) and the complexity of saving (e.g. tax-deferred retirement saving; see Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012), might govern the relationship between intention and saving behaviour. Even though people with more income are also likely to save more (e.g. Hershey et al. Citation2008; Lunt and Livingstone Citation1991; Lusardi Citation2008), perceived barriers, like a felt shortage of knowledge or a perceived insufficient income, do limit saving behaviour (Lusardi, Keller, and Keller Citation2009). As such, we expect saving barriers to have a mediating influence on the relationship between saving intention and saving behaviour: individuals who perceive stronger saving barriers are expected to have more difficulty translating intention into behaviour. And with intention assumed to capture the motivational factors influencing behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991), we expect that individuals with weaker intentions perceive more subjective barriers to saving which in turn predicts weaker saving behaviour.

Second, the low variance explained by saving intention could be caused by conflicting intentions towards saving. Interestingly, the TPB does not differentiate between intentions but assumes a single, presumably aggregated, intention (see Ajzen Citation1991; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). To address this possible limitation, we expand saving intention by also taking potentially conflicting intentions into account. The model assumes that such measure of saving intention predicts both perceived saving barriers and self-reported saving behaviour: individuals with a stronger saving intention are expected to perceive less saving barriers and to report more saving behaviour.

Method

Data were collected with a SurveyMonkey online questionnaire and analysed with RStudio (.98.493) and R (3.0.2). Participants were recruited through the University of Twente students participants pool and working individuals through email (snowball sampling) and online message boards (convenience sampling). This sampling approach was chosen to increase heterogeneity by including people with a potentially unclear financial situation (i.e. students) together with those who had a more stable, and potentially full-time, income. The students were given research credits for participating while other participants were included in a raffle of seven gift certificates from a Dutch online retailer worth 20 euros each. Before starting the questionnaire, participants were presented with an accompanying letter that included the research description and privacy assurances. By randomising statements and page order, we prevented order-effect bias (Perreault Citation1975), though the page with demographic variables was always included at the end.

Participants

A total of 340 participants partook in the questionnaire, of which 272 (80%) completed it. Since excluding participants a posteriori would increase the odds of non-random, biased exclusion, no incomplete questionnaires were removed. As a result, the sample size for each construct varies.

The majority of participants was female (60%) while 69% had attended higher education at some point (college or university; see Table ). On average the participants were young (M = 26.81, SD = 9.51, n = 272), ranging 15 to 73 years. Accordingly, half of participants were students although a large minority (40%) were employed. In terms of monthly net income, 22% reported no income, 33% less than 1500 euros, 31% between 1500 and 3000 euros, and a small group (6%) reported more than 3000 euros.

Table 1. Demographic statistics of the participants (N = 272).

Questionnaire

Participants’ level of agreement with statements was assessed with a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (fully disagree) to 5 (fully agree), and statements were formulated income-neutral to prevent biasFootnote1. Construct scales were refined with both exploratory and confirmatory principal components analysis (oblique rotation) followed by evaluating internal consistency.

Financial risk tolerance was measured with adapted statements from Grable and Lytton’s (Citation1999) Financial Risk Tolerance Scale. The 10 items were combined such that high scores signalled a high tolerance for financial risks while low scores displayed a financial risk intolerance (Cronbach’s α = .82, M = 2.25, SD = .74, n = 287).

Subjective financial knowledge assessed subjective estimates of knowledge and practical knowledge statements adapted from Flynn and Goldsmith (Citation1999) and Xiao et al. (Citation2011). The 14 statements were combined such that high scores indicated more financial knowledge (Cronbach’s α = .91; M = 3.07, SD = .89, n = 290).

The seven situational economic trust statements were derived from Statistics Netherlands (CBS (Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek) Citationn.d.) to measure trust in one’s current and future economic situation. The scores were computed such that high scores indicated a higher amount of situational economic trust (Cronbach’s α = .77; M = 3.23, SD = .79, n = 296).

Participants’ regulatory focus was measured with 9 statements from the General Regulatory Focus Measure (Lockwood, Jordan, and Kunda Citation2002) and divided into two subscales: promotion focus (Cronbach’s α = .65, M = 3.60, SD = .69, n = 291) and prevention focus (Cronbach’s α = .66, M = 2.98, SD = .75, n = 291). Both were calculated such that higher scores denoted a stronger focus.

Subjective saving norms were measured with statements adapted from Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman (Citation2010). The six items were combined such that high scores signalled a stronger perceived saving norm (Cronbach’s α = .81; M = 3.53, SD = .69, n = 289).

Perceived financial self-efficacy was measured with eight items adapted from Danes and Haberman (Citation2007), Dietz, Carrozza, and Neal Ritchey (Citation2003), and Shim, Serido, and Tang (Citation2012), and aggregated such that high scores indicated high perceived financial self-efficacy (Cronbach’s α = .84; M = 3.80, SD = .75, n = 295).

To address the possibility of conflicting intentions, saving intention (13 items) consisted out of a general saving intention (adapted from Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; Kidwell and Turrisi Citation2004; Xiao et al. Citation2011) and intention-based statements derived from instrumental and stimulating risk taking. Instrumental risk taking is thoughtfully taking financial risks to achieve relatively distant goals (Rogers, Viding, and Chamorro-Premuzic Citation2013; Zaleskiewicz Citation2001) while stimulating risk taking is risk taking due to the liking of risks with a strong emotional excitement (Vong Citation2007; Zaleskiewicz Citation2001) combined with poor risk and reward estimates (Rogers, Viding, and Chamorro-Premuzic Citation2013). Empirically, instrumental risk taking is associated with more self-reported savings, while stimulating risk taking correlates negatively with saving (Rogers, Viding, and Chamorro-Premuzic Citation2013). The saving intention scale was constructed such that high scores indicated a stronger intention to save for a financial buffer (Cronbach’s α = .76; M = 3.77, SD = .55, n = 287).

Perceived barriers to saving included statements about informational and financial barriers that were adapted from Lunt and Livingstone (Citation1991), Lusardi, Keller, and Keller (Citation2009), and Madern and Van Gaalen (Citation2011). The seven statements were combined with high values indicating more perceived obstacles (Cronbach’s α = .80; M = 2.21, SD = .78, n = 301).

The five self-reported saving behaviour statements were adapted from Davis and Hustvedt (Citation2012) and Shim, Serido, and Tang (Citation2012). This scale was constructed such that high scores indicated a higher amount of self-reported saving behaviour (Cronbach’s α = .84; M = 3.88, SD = 1.02, n = 303).

Findings

The model was tested with hierarchical linear regression analyses, all of which adhered to the assumptions of linear regression analysis (see Field, Miles, and Field Citation2012). These three regression analyses, each predicting one of the model’s key variables (financial risk tolerance, saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour), coupled with the mediation analysis generated the following results:

Regression Analyses

Situational economic trust, subjective financial knowledge and regulatory focus were assumed to predict financial risk tolerance. The regression model, see Table , confirmed that both subjective financial knowledge (β = .40, p < .001) and prevention focus (β = −.18, p < .01) were significant predictors of financial risk tolerance (R2 = .25); unexpectedly, however, situational economic trust and participants’ promotion focus were not. Thus, as expected, more subjective financial knowledge predicted a higher tolerance for financial risks while participants who indicated a stronger prevention focus reported lower levels of financial risk tolerance.

Table 2. Results of the hierarchical regression analyses for the dependent variables financial risk tolerance, saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour, respectively.

The model also proposed that financial risk tolerance, regulatory focus, subjective saving norms and perceived financial self-efficacy would forecast saving intention. Indeed, financial risk tolerance (β = −.56, p < .001) and perceived financial self-efficacy (β = .50, p < .001) significantly predicted saving intention (R2 = .46). Therefore less tolerance for financial risks and more perceived financial self-efficacy both independently predicted a stronger intention to save for a financial buffer. Contrary to expectations, neither regulatory focus (promotion and prevention focus) nor subjective saving norms predicted saving intention (see Table ).

The model further assumed that self-reported saving behaviour was predicted by saving intention and perceived financial self-efficacy. This was indeed the case: both a higher saving intention (β = .36, p < .001) and more perceived financial self-efficacy (β = .51, p < .001) were predictive of more self-reported saving behaviour (R2 = .52).

Additional regression analyses explored how demographic variables impacted key variables. For financial risk tolerance, women were less tolerant of financial risks than men (β = −.32, p < .001). In addition, participants who earned more than 3000 euros displayed a higher financial risk tolerance than those who earned less than 1500 euros (β = .85, p < .001). Furthermore, homeowners were more tolerant of financial risks than tenants (β = .18, p < .05), while participants who lived with their parents (as opposed to living with a partner) were less tolerant of financial risks (β = −.19, p < .05).

Saving intention was also affected by demographic variables: women reported higher saving intentions than men (β = .18, p < .01), and participants living with their parents reported more saving intention than those living with a partner (β = .26, p < .05). Additionally, employees reported more saving intention than students (β = .47, p < .001), and unemployed participants also expressed a greater intention to save than students (β = .97, p < .05).

Self-reported saving behaviour was influenced by one demographic variable: employees reported more saving behaviour than students (β = .46, p < .001), which suggests that the influence of demographics on reported saving behaviour is minor.

To see if demographic variables impacted the proposed model, we performed a stepwise regression analysis that added all demographic variables to the model that predicted self-reported saving behaviour with saving intention and financial self-efficacy (R2 = .52). This modestly increased explained variance (R2 = .59; F(14, 255) = 2.28, p < .01) with just one demographic variable significant: homeowners reported more saving behaviour than renters (β = .15, p < .05). Likewise, regressing all demographic variables on self-reported saving behaviour explained considerably less variance (R2 = .26) than predicting saving behaviour with saving intention and financial self-efficacy.

The impact of perceived saving barriers

We hypothesised that the positive relation between saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour is explained by perceived saving barriers. That is, a higher saving intention would predict seeing less barriers, which in turn would predict more self-reported saving behaviour. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to assess each component of this proposed mediation model. First, it was found that saving intention was positively associated with self-reported saving behaviour (B = 1.00, t(278) = 10.60, p < .001). Saving intention was also negatively related to perceived saving barriers (B = −.43, t(278) = −5.30, p < .001). Lastly, results indicated that the mediator, perceived saving barriers, was negatively associated with saving behaviour (B = −.65, t(278) = −11.22, p < .001).

Because both the a-path and b-path were significant, the presumed mediation effect was tested using the bootstrapping method with bias-corrected confidence intervals (Preacher and Hayes Citation2008). Per Preacher and Hayes (Citation2008) recommendation, a 95% confidence interval of the indirect effect with 10,000 bootstrap resamples was obtained. The bootstrapped unstandardised indirect effect was .28 (bias-corrected CI = .15 to .42), confirming saving obstacles’ mediating effect on the relation between saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour. Furthermore, results indicated that the direct effect of saving intention on self-reported saving behaviour lessened when controlling for saving obstacles (B = .72, t(277) = 8.76, p < .001), thereby suggesting partial mediation. The standardised beta coefficients are displayed in Figure .

Figure 2. Proposed model with standardised beta values resulting from the regression analyses and mediation analysis.

Note: * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001. Insignificant relations are highlighted with a dotted line.
Figure 2. Proposed model with standardised beta values resulting from the regression analyses and mediation analysis.

Discussion

This study aimed to identify potential determinants of financial buffer saving. For this, the TPB-based model assumes that financial risk tolerance is predicted by situational economic trust, subjective financial knowledge and regulatory focus. Results indicated that more subjective financial knowledge positively predicted financial risk tolerance. This is a new finding, although other studies related knowledge to an increased willingness to take financial risks (e.g. Grable and Joo Citation2004), presumably through its impact on confidence (Wang Citation2009). Ajzen et al.’s (Citation2011) critique of general knowledge notwithstanding, subjective knowledge seems valuable in predicting attitude towards financial buffer saving.

Regulatory focus also predicted financial risk tolerance, albeit only a stronger prevention focus was associated with less financial risk tolerance. While in line with the literature (Crowe and Higgins Citation1997; Freitas et al. Citation2002; Halamish et al. Citation2008; Lockwood, Jordan, and Kunda Citation2002), participants’ promotion focus unexpectedly did not influence financial risk tolerance. This can suggest that, when it comes to financial buffer saving, promotion-focused individuals seek opportunities for accomplishments in other domains.

Contrary to expectations, situational economic trust also did not influence financial risk tolerance. While other studies found that both personal (e.g. González and Özcan Citation2013; Macé Citation2012) and macro-level circumstances (e.g. O’Neill and Xiao Citation2012) influence financial behaviour, this finding suggests that an individual’s opinion of their financial situation might not influence financial behaviour through attitude.

The second part of the model assumes that saving intention is predicted by financial risk tolerance, regulatory focus, perceived financial self-efficacy and subjective saving norms. Financial risk tolerance indeed predicted saving intention: intolerance for financial risks was associated with a stronger saving intention while a high financial risk tolerance forecasted less saving intention. Furthermore, this TPB-derived relationship predicts intention better (β = −.56, p < .001) than other TPB-based studies that used general attitude (e.g. β = .10, Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; β = .25, Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010). This suggests that specific and fine-grained measures of attitude are a feasible line of inquiry for TPB-based studies.

Prevention and promotion focus did not influence saving intention, however. This is surprising since, theoretically speaking (e.g. Crowe and Higgins Citation1997; Higgins et al. Citation2001), especially prevention focus seems a prime candidate for influencing saving intention for a financial buffer. Even though we used the General Regulatory Focus Measure (Lockwood, Jordan, and Kunda Citation2002), our scales achieved a lower internal consistency, possibly due to translating them into Dutch.

As expected, perceived financial self-efficacy predicted saving intention: the higher an individual’s financial self-efficacy, the stronger their intention to save money. Individuals who feel capable of dealing with financial matters can thus be expected to display strong saving intentions, while those who feel powerless with financial decisions have a commensurate lower intention to save money. This finding corroborates other studies which showed that financial self-efficacy influences saving intention and financial behaviour (Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; Lusardi, Keller, and Keller Citation2009; Xiao et al. Citation2011).

Interestingly, we do not observe that subjective saving norms predict saving intention, contradicting other studies (Croy, Gerrans, and Speelman Citation2010; Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012; Xiao et al. Citation2011). There are two potential explanations for this. First, high educational attainment is associated with more financial knowledge and individuals with high financial knowledge are considerably less likely to consult their social environment about financial matters (van Rooij, Lusardi, and Alessie Citation2011). Since highly educated participants were overrepresented in our sample, this might have lessened the impact of social norms. Second, findings from Kidwell and Turrisi (Citation2004) suggest that, when people experience a high level of control over a given behaviour, they experience a stronger effect of subjective norms, with the reverse true for people with low perceived control. After all, with a high perceived control over a given behaviour, it is hard to justify not performing the behaviour desired by one’s environment. But with a majority of participants unemployed, they might have experienced little control over saving money, in turn reducing the impact of social norms.Footnote2

The third part of the model postulates that self-reported saving behaviour is predicted by perceived financial self-efficacy and saving intention, with the influence of this latter mediated by perceived barriers to saving. Results confirmed these assumptions. A higher perceived financial self-efficacy predicted more self-reported saving behaviour, suggesting that individuals who feel capable of dealing with money issues also succeed better in saving money, a finding corroborated by other studies (Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012; Xiao et al. Citation2011).

Saving intention also predicted self-reported saving behaviour such that a stronger intention was associated with more self-reported saving. This not only suggests that participants succeeded reasonably well in translating intentions into behaviour, but with intention assumed to reflect motivation (Ajzen Citation1991), participants were also motivated to save. Furthermore, our measures of intention and perceived financial self-efficacy seem valuable with high amount of variance explained in self-reported saving behaviour (R2 = .52) compared to other studies (e.g. R2 = .21, Davis and Hustvedt Citation2012).

Perceived saving barriers also affected self-reported saving behaviour: the more obstacles participants perceived, the less saving behaviour reported. Saving intention, in turn, influenced perceived saving barriers: the stronger the intention, the fewer barriers seen. As it turns out, perceived saving barriers partially mediated the relationship between saving intention and self-reported saving behaviour: individuals with weaker saving intentions perceived more saving barriers and this predicted weaker saving behaviour in turn. This new finding confirms studies which showed that actual (Lunt and Livingstone Citation1991) and perceived (Lusardi, Keller, and Keller Citation2009) barriers are related to saving behaviour, and supports the TPB’s assertion that the amount of volitional control influences the extent to which intentions are translated into behaviour (Ajzen Citation1991).

Three variables in our model are prime candidates for interventions. The first is saving intention, which predicted self-reported saving behaviour and lead to less perceived barriers, in turn further stimulating saving behaviour. The second is financial self-efficacy, which influenced saving intention and had a direct impact on self-reported saving behaviour. And the third (perceived saving barriers) affected how well saving intentions are translated into behaviour. These findings suggest that financial buffer saving can be stimulated with actionable information that increases financial self-efficacy (i.e. no financial jargon and small, manageable steps). Lusardi and others (2009), for instance, designed an easy-to-follow seven-step plan to increase supplemental retirement saving. Of the participants that received their brochure, 27.6% (n = 83) participated as opposed to 7.3% of the control group (n = 210). Such interventions can also address perceived saving barriers revolving around informational deficiencies and misconceptions (e.g. saving plans are difficult or require a certain amount of income). Since prevention focus predicted less financial risk tolerance, which in turn predicted more saving intention, an intervention may also be framed as a basic necessity instead of an opportunity for accomplishment. This also makes people take action sooner (see Freitas et al. Citation2002).

In aggregate, our model not only confirms meta-analytical findings which showed that the TPB predicts a wide range of behaviours (Armitage and Conner Citation2001; Conner and Armitage Citation1998), it also explains considerably more variance in self-reported behaviour (R2 = .52) than the TPB on average does (R2 = .32; Armitage and Conner Citation2001). And despite demographic variables predicting emergency savings in earlier research (Babiarz and Robb Citation2013), our demographic variables explained a modest amount of variance (R2 = .26) compared to the model with just two psychological variables. Furthermore, seemingly important demographic variables like income and employment did not affect self-reported financial buffer saving. Taken together these findings suggest that adapting the TPB to domain-specific attitudes and behaviour is a viable approach, also since that addresses behaviour in a manner that cannot be deduced from demographic variables alone.

Limitations of the current study include a self-selection bias due to self-recruiting participants, although the sample was more heterogeneous than other TPB-based studies into financial behaviour (cf. Shim, Serido, and Tang Citation2012; Xiao et al. Citation2011). Furthermore, we did not examine actual behaviours but used the less privacy-invasive measure of self-reported behaviour. Suggestions for further research include relating a financial buffer size to a participant’s income category. That allows for examining how the relative size of a buffer affects constructs like financial self-efficacy, perceived saving barriers and saving intention. We did not to do this in this current study since we expected an insufficient sample size for categorising participants in income groups. Additional research can also examine other risk-mitigating financial products besides a saving account (e.g. insurance), whose relative popularity might depend on an individual’s regulatory focus or financial risk tolerance.

This study showed that psychological constructs play an important role with financial buffer saving, although this aspect of self-reliance has not received much research attention nor governmental interest, despite the latter shifting increasingly more (financial) responsibility unto its citizens (e.g. Veldheer et al. Citation2012). A greater focus on stimulating adequate financial buffers by considering the psychological constructs set forth in the current model is therefore needed. That not only makes individuals more self-reliant and independent, it also enables citizens to collectively reduce the financial and personal impact of calamities.

Notes

1. The questionnaire is available from the corresponding author upon request.

2. Indeed, employed participants reported significantly higher saving norms (M = 3.68, SD = .62, n = 116) than jobless participants (M = 3.42, SD = .69, n = 156; t(261) = 3.33, p < .001).

References

  • Ajzen, Icek. 1991. “The Theory of Planned Behavior.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50 (2): 179–211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.
  • Ajzen, Icek. 2002. “Perceived Behavioral Control, Self-Efficacy, Locus of Control, and the Theory of Planned Behavior.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 32 (4): 665–683. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb00236.x.
  • Ajzen, Icek, Nicholas Joyce, Sana Sheikh, and Nicole Gilbert Cote. 2011. “Knowledge and the Prediction of Behavior: The Role of Information Accuracy in the Theory of Planned Behavior.” Basic and Applied Social Psychology 33 (2): 101–117. doi:10.1080/01973533.2011.568834.
  • Alessie, Rob, and Federica Teppa. 2009. “Saving and Habit Formation: Evidence from Dutch Panel Data.” Empirical Economics 38 (2): 385–407. doi:10.1007/s00181-009-0272-z.
  • Armitage, Christopher J., and Mark Conner. 2001. “Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A Meta-Analytic Review.” British Journal of Social Psychology 40 (4): 471–499. doi:10.1348/014466601164939.
  • Babiarz, Patryk, and Cliff A. Robb. 2013. “Financial Literacy and Emergency Saving.” Journal of Family and Economic Issues 35 (1): 40–50. doi:10.1007/s10834-013-9369-9.
  • Bandura, Albert. 1977. “Self-Efficacy: Toward a Unifying Theory of Behavioral Change.” Psychological Review 84 (2): 191–215. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191.
  • Brobeck, Stephen. 2008. “Understanding the Emergency Savings Needs of Low-and Moderate-Income Households: A Survey-Based Analysis of Impacts, Causes, and Remedies.” Consumer Federation of America. http://www.csrees.usda.gov/nea/economics/pdfs/understandingTheEmergencySavingsNeedsOfLow_102908.pdf.
  • CBS (Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek). n.d. “CBS StatLine Database.” Accessed February 12, 2014. http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb/publication/?DM=SLNL&PA=7388PCR&D1=26-32,40-58,73-105,112-117&D2=465,469,473,477,482,486,490,494,499,503,507,511,516,520&HDR=T&STB=G1&VW=T
  • Chan, Raymond K. H. 2006. “Risk and Its Management in Post-Financial Crisis Hong Kong.” Social Policy and Administration 40 (2): 215–229. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9515.2006.00485.x.
  • Conner, Mark, and Christopher J. Armitage. 1998. “Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review and Avenues for Further Research.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 28 (15): 1429–1464. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01685.x.
  • Crossley, Thomas F., Hamish Low, and Cormac O’Dea. 2013. “Household Consumption through Recent Recessions.” Fiscal Studies 34 (2): 203–229. doi:10.1111/j.1475-5890.2013.12003.x.
  • Crowe, Ellen, and E. Tory Higgins. 1997. “Regulatory Focus and Strategic Inclinations: Promotion and Prevention in Decision-Making.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 69 (2): 117–132. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.2675.
  • Croy, Gerry, Paul Gerrans, and Craig Speelman. 2010. “The Role and Relevance of Domain Knowledge, Perceptions of Planning Importance, and Risk Tolerance in Predicting Savings Intentions.” Journal of Economic Psychology 31 (6): 860–871. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2010.06.002.
  • Cude, Brenda J., Frances C. Lawrence, Angela C. Lyons, Kaci Metzger, Emily LeJeune, Loren Marks, and Krisanna Machtmes. 2006. “College Students and Financial Literacy: What They Know and What We Need to Learn.” In Eastern Family Economics and Resource Management Association, 102–109. http://www.cgsnet.org/ckfinder/userfiles/files/College_Students_and_Financial_Literacy.pdf.
  • Danes, Sharon M., and Heather R. Haberman. 2007. “Teen Financial Knowledge, Self-Efficacy, and Behavior: A Gendered View.” Financial Counseling and Planning 18 (2): 48–60. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/Delivery.cfm?abstractid=2228406.
  • Davis, Kimberlee, and Gwendolyn Hustvedt. 2012. “It’s a Matter of Control: Saving for Retirement.” International Review of Social Sciences and Humanities 3 (2): 248–261. http://irssh.com/yahoo_site_admin/assets/docs/25_IRSSH-354-V3N2.202201509.pdf.
  • Dietz, Bernadette E., Mark Carrozza, and P. Neal Ritchey. 2003. “Does Financial Self-Efficacy Explain Gender Differences in Retirement Saving Strategies?” Journal of Women & Aging 15 (4): 83–96. doi:10.1300/J074v15n04.
  • Field, Andy, Jeremy Miles, and Zoë Field. 2012. Discovering Statistics Using R. London: Sage.
  • Fisher, Patti J. 2010. “Gender Differences in Personal Saving Behaviors.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 21 (1): 14–24. http://6aa7f5c4a9901a3e1a1682793cd11f5a6b732d29.gripelements.com/pdf/volume_21_issue_1/pattiejfisher.pdf.
  • Fisher, Patti J., and Sophia T. Anong. 2012. “Relationship of Saving Motives to Saving Habits.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 23 (1): 63–79. https://www.afcpe.org/assets/pdf/v23_j4.pdf.
  • Flynn, Leisa Reinecke, and Ronald E. Goldsmith. 1999. “A Short, Reliable Measure of Subjective Knowledge.” Journal of Business Research 46 (1): 57–66. doi:10.1016/S0148-2963(98)00057-5.
  • Freitas, Antonio L., Nira Liberman, Peter Salovey, and E. Tory Higgins. 2002. “When to Begin? Regulatory Focus and Initiating Goal Pursuit.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 (1): 121–130. doi:10.1177/0146167202281011.
  • Gilliam, John, Swarn Chatterjee, and John Grable. 2010. “Measuring the Perception of Financial Risk Tolerance: A Tale of Two Measures.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 21 (2): 30–43. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2127431.
  • González, Libertad, and Berkay Özcan. 2013. “The Risk of Divorce and Household Saving Behavior.” The Journal of Human Resources 48 (2): 404–434. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1300337.
  • Grable, John E. 2000. “Financial Risk Tolerance and Additional Factors That Affect Risk Taking in Everyday Money Matters.” Journal of Business and Psychology 14 (4): 625–630. doi:10.1023/A:1022994314982.
  • Grable, John E., Sonya L. Britt, and Farrell J. Webb. 2008. “Environmental and Biopsychosocial Profiling as a Means for Describing Financial Risk-Taking Behavior.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 19 (2): 3–18. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2226466.
  • Grable, John E., and So-Hyun Joo. 2004. “Environmental and Biopsychosocial Factors Associated with Financial Risk Tolerance.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 15 (1): 73–82. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2260471.
  • Grable, John, and Ruth H. Lytton. 1999. “Financial Risk Tolerance Revisited: The Development of a Risk Assessment Instrument.” Financial Services Review 8 (3): 163–181. doi:10.1016/S1057-0810(99)00041-4.
  • Grable, John E., and Ruth H. Lytton. 2003. “The Development of a Risk Assessment Instrument: A Follow-up Study.” Financial Services Review 12 (3): 257–274. http://www2.stetson.edu/fsr/abstracts/vol_12_num3_p257.pdf.
  • Grable, John, Ruth Lytton, and Barbara O’Neill. 2004. “Projection Bias and Financial Risk Tolerance.” The Journal of Behavioral Finance 5 (3): 142–147. doi:10.1207/s15427579jpfm0503_2.
  • Grable, John, Michael Roszkowski, So-Hyun Joo, Barbara O’Neill, and Ruth H. Lytton. 2009. “A Test of the Relationship between Self-Classified Financial Risk-Tolerance and Investment Risk-Taking Behaviour.” International Journal of Risk Assessment and Management 12 (2/3/4): 396–419. doi:10.1504/IJRAM.2009.025929.
  • Halamish, Vered, Nira Liberman, E. Tory Higgins, and Lorraine Chen Idson. 2008. “Regulatory Focus Effects on Discounting over Uncertainty for Losses Vs. Gains.” Journal of Economic Psychology 29 (5): 654–666. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.002.
  • Hamilton, Myra. 2012. “The ‘New Social Contract’ and the Individualisation of Risk in Policy.” Journal of Risk Research 17 (4) ( November 5): 453–467. doi:10.1080/13669877.2012.726250.
  • Hershey, Douglas A., Joy M. Jacobs-Lawson, John J. McArdle, and Fumiaki Hamagami. 2008. “Psychological Foundations of Financial Planning for Retirement.” Journal of Adult Development 14 (1–2): 26–36. doi:10.1007/s10804-007-9028-1.
  • Higgins, E. Tory, Ronald S. Friedman, Robert E. Harlow, Lorraine Chen Idson, Ozlem N. Ayduk, and Amy Taylor. 2001. “Achievement Orientations from Subjective Histories of Success: Promotion Pride versus Prevention Pride.” European Journal of Social Psychology 31 (1): 3–23. doi:10.1002/ejsp.27.
  • Idson, Lorraine Chen, Nira Liberman, and E. Tory Higgins. 2000. “Distinguishing Gains from Nonlosses and Losses from Nongains: A Regulatory Focus Perspective on Hedonic Intensity.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 36: 252–274. doi:10.1006/jesp.1999.1402.
  • Kidwell, Blair, and Robert Turrisi. 2004. “An Examination of College Student Money Management Tendencies.” Journal of Economic Psychology 25 (5): 601–616. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(03)00073-4.
  • Kievik, Milou, and Jan M. Gutteling. 2011. “Yes, We Can: Motivate Dutch Citizens to Engage in Self-Protective Behavior with Regard to Flood Risks.” Natural Hazards 59 (3): 1475–1490. doi:10.1007/s11069-011-9845-1.
  • Kreibich, H., A. H. Thieken, Th Petrow, M. Müller, and B. Merz. 2005. “Flood Loss Reduction of Private Households due to Building Precautionary Measures – Lessons Learned from the Elbe Flood in August 2002.” Natural Hazards and Earth System Science 5 (1): 117–126. doi:10.5194/nhess-5-117-2005.
  • Lockwood, Penelope, Christian H. Jordan, and Ziva Kunda. 2002. “Motivation by Positive or Negative Role Models: Regulatory Focus Determines Who Will Best Inspire Us.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83 (4): 854–864. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.83.4.854.
  • Loibl, Cäzilia, David S. Kraybill, and Sara Wackler DeMay. 2011. “Accounting for the Role of Habit in Regular Saving.” Journal of Economic Psychology 32 (4): 581–592. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.04.004.
  • Lunt, Peter K., and Sonia M. Livingstone. 1991. “Psychological, Social and Economic Determinants of Saving: Comparing Recurrent and Total Savings.” Journal of Economic Psychology 12 (4): 621–641. doi:10.1016/0167-4870(91)90003-C.
  • Lusardi, Annamaria. 2008. “Household Saving Behavior: The Role of Financial Literacy, Information, and Financial Education Programs.” NBER Paper No. 13824. http://www.nber.org/papers/w13824.
  • Lusardi, Annamaria, Punam Anand Keller, and Adam M. Keller. 2009. “New Ways to Make People save: A Social Marketing Approach.” NBER Paper No. 14715. http://www.nber.org/papers/w14715.
  • Lusardi, Annamaria, and Olivia S. Mitchell. 2005. “Implications for Retirement Wellbeing.” http://www.dnb.nl/binaries/WorkingPaper78_tcm46-146735.pdf.
  • Lusardi, Annamaria, Daniel J. Schneider, and Peter Tufano. 2011. “Financially Fragile Households: Evidence and Implications.” NBER Paper No. 17072. http://www.nber.org/papers/w17072.
  • Macé, Serge. 2012. “Saving and Perceived Health Risks.” Review of Economics of the Household ( September 28). doi:10.1007/s11150-012-9160-y.
  • Madern, Tamara, and Corinne Van Gaalen. 2011. “Bindingstechnieken: Een Onderzoek Naar Methoden Om Verleidingen van Geld Uitgeven Te Weerstaan [Binding Techniques: A Search for Methods to Resist Temptations for Spending Money].” http://www.nibud.nl/fileadmin/user_upload/Documenten/PDF/onderzoeken/2011/Bindingstechnieken_dec.2011.pdf.
  • Mahdzan, Nural Shahnaz, and Saleh Tabiani. 2013. “The Impact of Financial Literacy on Individual Saving: An Exploratory Study in the Malaysian Context.” Transformations in Business & Economics 12 (1): 41–55. http://www.transformations.khf.vu.lt/28/se28.pdf.
  • Markiewicz, Łukasz, and Elke U. Weber. 2013. “DOSPERT’s Gambling Risk-Taking Propensity Scale Predicts Excessive Stock Trading.” Journal of Behavioral Finance 14 (1): 65–78. doi:10.1080/15427560.2013.762000.
  • Martin, Ingrid M., Holly Bender, and Carol Raish. 2007. “What Motivates Individuals to Protect Themselves from Risks: The Case of Wildland Fires.” Risk Analysis 27 (4): 887–900. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2007.00930.x.
  • NIBUD. 2012. “40% van de huishoudens heeft te weinig buffer [40% of Households have Insufficient Buffer].” Accessed September 12, 2013. http://www.nibud.nl/over-het-nibud/perskamer/overzicht-persberichten/persbericht/artikel/40-van-de-huishoudens-heeft-te-weinig-buffer.html.
  • Nicholson, Nigel, Emma Soane, and Mark Fenton Fenton-O'Creevy, and Paul Willman. 2005. “Personality and Domain-Specific Risk Taking.” Journal of Risk Research 8 (2): 157–176. doi:10.1080/1366987032000123856.
  • O’Neill, Barbara, and Jing J. Xiao. 2012. “Financial Behaviors before and after the Financial Crisis: Evidence from an Online Survey.” Journal of Financial Counseling and Planning 23 (1): 33–46. http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2220894.
  • Olsen, Robert A. 2012. “Trust: The Underappreciated Investment Risk Attribute.” Journal of Behavioral Finance 13 (4): 308–313. doi:10.1080/15427560.2012.735728.
  • Pericoli, Filippo, and Luigi Ventura. 2011. “Family Dissolution and Precautionary Savings: An Empirical Analysis.” Review of Economics of the Household 10 (4): 573–595. doi:10.1007/s11150-011-9127-4.
  • Perreault, William D. 1975. “Controlling Order-Effect Bias.” The Public Opinion Quarterly 39 (4): 544–551. http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.2307/2748507.10.1086/268251
  • Preacher, Kristopher J., and Andrew F. Hayes. 2008. “Asymptotic and Resampling Strategies for Assessing and Comparing Indirect Effects in Multiple Mediator Models.” Behavior Research Methods 40 (3): 879–891. doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.
  • Rogers, John, Essi Viding, and Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic. 2013. “Instrumental and Disinhibited Financial Risk Taking: Personality and Behavioural Correlates.” Personality and Individual Differences 55 (6): 645–649. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.011.
  • Shim, Soyeon, Joyce Serido, and Chuanyi Tang. 2012. “The Ant and the Grasshopper Revisited: The Present Psychological Benefits of Saving and Future Oriented Financial Behaviors.” Journal of Economic Psychology 33 (1): 155–165. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2011.08.005.
  • Tokuoka, Kiichi. 2013. “Saving Response to Unemployment of a Sibling.” Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 89: 58–75. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2013.03.010.
  • Tversky, Amos, and Daniel Kahneman. 1992. “Advances in Prospect Theory: Cumulative Representation of Uncertainty.” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 5 (4): 297–323. doi:10.1007/BF00122574.
  • van Rooij, Maarten, Annamaria Lusardi, and Rob Alessie. 2011. “Financial Literacy and Stock Market Participation.” Journal of Financial Economics 101 (2): 449–472. doi:10.1016/j.jfineco.2011.03.006.
  • van Steden, Ronald, Barbara van Caem, and Hans Boutellier. 2011. “The ‘Hidden Strength’ of Active Citizenship: The Involvement of Local Residents in Public Safety Projects.” Criminology and Criminal Justice 11 (5): 433–450. doi:10.1177/1748895811414467.
  • Veldheer, Vic, Jedid-Jah Jonker, Lonneke Van Noije, and Cok Vrooman. 2012. “Een Beroep Op de Burger: Minder Verzorgingsstaat, Meer Eigen Verantwoordelijkheid003F [An Appeal to the Citizen: Less Welfare, More Personal Responsibility]” http://www.scp.nl/Publicaties/Alle_publicaties/Publicaties_2012/Een_beroep_op_de_burger.
  • Viljanen, Lea. 2005. “Towards an Ontology of Trust.” In Trust, Privacy, and Security in Digital Business: Second International Conference, TrustBus 2005, Copenhagen, Denmark, August 22–26, 2005. Proceedings, 175–184. Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/11537878_18.
  • Vong, Fanny. 2007. “The Psychology of Risk-Taking in Gambling among Chinese Visitors to Macau.” International Gambling Studies 7 (1): 29–42. doi:10.1080/14459790601157731.
  • Wang, Alex. 2009. “Interplay of Investors’ Financial Knowledge and Risk Taking.” Journal of Behavioral Finance 10 (4): 204–213. doi:10.1080/15427560903369292.
  • Weber, Elke U., Ann-Renée Blais, and Nancy E. Betz. 2002. “A Domain-Specific Risk-Attitude Scale: Measuring Risk Perceptions and Risk Behaviors.” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 15 (4): 263–290. doi:10.1002/bdm.414.
  • Xiao, Jing Jian, Chuanyi Tang, Joyce Serido, and Soyeon Shim. 2011. “Antecedents and Consequences of Risky Credit Behavior among College Students: Application and Extension of the Theory of Planned Behavior.” Journal of Public Policy & Marketing 30 (2): 239–245. doi:10.1509/jppm.30.2.239.
  • Zaleskiewicz, Tomasz. 2001. “Beyond Risk Seeking and Risk Aversion: Personality and the Dual Nature of Economic Risk Taking.” European Journal of Personality 15 (S1): S105–S122. doi:10.1002/per.426.