7,772
Views
28
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Extrapolating understanding of food risk perceptions to emerging food safety cases

, &
Pages 996-1018 | Received 02 May 2015, Accepted 12 Dec 2016, Published online: 31 Jan 2017

Abstract

Important determinants of risk perceptions associated with foods are the extent to which the potential hazards are perceived to have technological or naturally occurring origins, together with the acute vs. chronic dimension in which the potential hazard is presented (acute or chronic). This study presents a case study analysis based on an extensive literature review examining how these hazard characteristics affect people’s risk and benefit perceptions, and associated attitudes and behaviors. The cases include E. coli incidences (outbreaks linked to fresh spinach and fenugreek sprouts), contamination of fish by environmental pollutants, (organochlorine contaminants in farmed salmon), radioactive contamination of food following a nuclear accident (the Fukushima accident in Japan), and GM salmon destined for the human food chain. The analysis of the cases over the acute vs. chronic dimension suggests that longitudinal quantification of the relationship between risk perceptions and impacts is important for both acute and chronic food safety, but this has infrequently been applied to chronic hazards. Technologies applied to food production tend to potentially be associated with higher levels of risk perception, linked to perceptions that the risk is unnatural. However, for some risks (e.g. those involving biological irreversibility), moral or ethical concerns may be more important determinants of consumer responses than risk or benefit perceptions. (Lack of) trust has been highlighted in all of the cases suggesting transparent and honest risk–benefit communications following the occurrence of a food safety incident. Implications for optimizing associated risk communication strategies, additional research linking risk perception, and other quantitative measures, including comparisons in time and space, are suggested.

1. Introduction

It has been established that people’s responses to different risks are affected by how they perceive potential hazard characteristics. Furthermore people’s risk perceptions do not always align with technical risk estimates provided by experts (Fischhoff et al. Citation1978; Slovic Citation2000). People’s risk perceptions are often reasonable but can also militate against adoption of self protective behaviors, implying that effective risk communication may be required (Fischhoff and Kadvany Citation2011). In the context of public health, effective risk communication aims to provide laypeople with the information they need to make informed, independent judgments (Morgan et al. Citation2002). Food safety is of particular interest in this context, as there is some evidence suggesting that food risks are perceived differently from non-food risks (FAO/WHO, Citationforthcoming). This is because complete avoidance of food risks is not possible, and because food has cultural, symbolic, familial, and religious connotations which must be taken into account when developing risk messages (Frewer et al. Citation2015). In addition, food may simultaneously be associated with risks, such as inclusion of contaminants, and benefits, such as nutritional advantages (Cohen et al. Citation2005; Hoekstra et al. Citation2013a, Citation2013b; van der Voet, de Mul, and van Klaveren Citation2007) suggesting that both risk and benefit perceptions associated with foods need to be considered when developing risk communication strategies (van Dijk, Fischer, and Frewer Citation2011; Hooper Citation2006; Saba and Messina Citation2003).

Some types of determinants of risk perceptions seem to be specifically important in shaping people’s responses to food risks such as the extent to which the potential hazards are perceived to have ‘technological’ or ‘naturally occurring’ origins (Frewer et al. Citation2013b; Rozin et al. Citation2004; Siegrist Citation2008). The application of technologies to food production may be perceived as hazardous. Failing to take account of this negative starting point, and subsequent negligence of the needs and priorities of consumers during the process of technology development and implementation, has resulted in societal rejection of potentially useful emerging food technologies such as genetically modified (GM) foods (Frewer et al. Citation2011; Raley et al. Citation2016). In contrast, consumers’ low levels of risk perceptions associated with naturally occurring food hazards has increased their tendency to adopt potentially risk behaviors, for example in relation to food preparation practices (Nauta et al. Citation2008).

Further complexity is provided by the acute vs. chronic context in which the potential hazard is presented (Glik Citation2007). Presenting a naturally occurring risk in an acute or ‘crisis’ context may increase risk perceptions (Pidgeon, Kasperson, and Slovic Citation2003). Examples include foodborne outbreaks that may be difficult to predict in terms of which microbial hazard will occur when, and affect whom. In the case of chronically occurring food hazards, (e.g. radioactive contamination of food), more information regarding the potential for varied impacts across differentially vulnerable populations may become available as a consequence of the ongoing risk assessment process (WHO/FAO, Citationforthcoming). The acute vs. chronic context of the hazard may differentially influence people’s perceptions of risks, and hence their behaviors. In order to understand the potential impacts of both acute and chronic food safety incidents on both public health and economic functioning of the food chain, it is important to quantify the relationships between food risk perceptions and impacts. However, new metrics may be needed to assess this relationship (Dreyer et al. Citation2010), which can be formally included in the risk assessment phase of the risk analysis process.

This paper presents case study analyses based on an extensive literature review examining how food hazard characteristics affect people’s risk and benefit perceptions, and associated attitudes and behaviors. Two ‘axes’ frame the analysis. The first relates to the ‘risk origin’ (technological or natural). The second relates to the ‘acute vs. chronic dimension’ of the food hazard. Therefore, the following hypotheses will be tested through case study analysis:

H1: Consumers perceive the application of technologies to food production as hazardous.

H2: Consumers’ risk perceptions of naturally occurring food hazards may have negative impacts regarding optimizing nutrition.

H3: Acute vs. chronic hazards differentially influence people’s risk perceptions and behaviors due to the temporal context

2. Methodology

Use of case studies to understand a specific research question is a research strategy that focuses on understanding the dynamics present within a single or multiple settings (Eisenhardt Citation1989). In the comparative analysis of cases that follows, four food-related cases were presented. In each case, it is arguable that there is potential for risk perceptions associated with the food hazard to lead consumers to behave in a way contrary to their own, and societal interests, where societal is understood to refer to a ‘common good.’ Thus, it should be possible to derive generic, as well as situation specific, conclusions regarding risk perception and food choice. The cases selected have been subject to considerable attention among different stakeholders (e.g. the scientific community in general, regulatory agencies, media, and representatives of civic society such as non-governmental organizations). The cases include E. coli incidences, (outbreaks linked to fresh spinach and fenugreek sprouts), contamination of fish by environmental pollutants, (organochlorine contaminants in farmed salmon), radioactive contamination of food following a nuclear accident (the accident at the Fukushima Dai-ichi nuclear power plant), and genetically modified (GM) salmon destined for the human food chain.

Classification of the cases according to risk origin and acute vs. chronic dimension is provided in Table .

Table 1. Classification of the cases according to risk origin (technological vs. natural) and acute vs. chronic dimension.

Each case is described with a brief explanation about the risk issue, information about the factors linked to increased consumer risk perceptions, the impact of the incident, a chronological overview from consumers’ perspective, and where feasible, research reporting consumers’ attitudes and behavior during and/or after the incidents, and, if applicable, discussion of the additional metrics needed to quantify the relationship between risk perceptions and impacts.

The number of English language media reports that were published about each case are provided in Table . Although not having access to media reports in other languages (e.g. German, Japanese) is a limitation to reflect the complete picture on the information that those consumers received, Table may still be a good proxy reflecting societal interests or social amplification of risk effect.

Table 2. Overview of English language media reporting on the four cases in this paper.

3. Case studies

3.1. E. coli incidences

Some strains of E. coli bacteria (e.g. Shiga Toxin producing E. coli (STEC) such as E. coli 0157:H7, E. coli O104:H4) are pathogenic, resulting in diarrhea or serious conditions (e.g. hemolytic uremic syndrome (HUS)) that can be fatal (UK Food Standards Agency Citation2013). STEC are among the most reported and monitored food pathogens in the EU and US because they frequently cause sporadic cases of illnesses and large foodborne outbreaks in these countries (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Citation2013; European Food Safety Authority Citation2013). When communicated in a ‘crisis’ context, E. coli outbreaks have the potential to generate high level of public concern (UK Food Standards Agency Citation2013).

Consumption of foods and food categories associated with the outbreak tends to decline during the course of the outbreak (Mazzocchi Citation2006; McCullough, Marsh, and Huffaker Citation2013; Oniki Citation2006). These effects are large enough to quantify the ‘acute’ impacts of a food safety incident by examination of product-specific sales data (Hooper Citation2006; Saba and Messina Citation2003). However, risk perceptions may have negative unintended consequences, such as not consuming the product or substitutes for a prolonged period after the crisis ends, in spite of their health benefits (Cuite et al., Citationforthcoming). Such ‘stigmatization’ of foods may therefore have negative long-term health effects (Gregory, Slovic, and Flynn Citation1996). Against this, failure to communicate risks effectively may have negative consequences for health if people fail to adopt self-protective behaviors. For example, (SteelFisher et al. Citation2013) asked participants from different age groups about their self-protective behaviors with regard to a hypothetical foodborne outbreak and found that they were very likely to adopt a range of recommended protective behaviors (although this is less common among older than younger participants). However, consumer surveys that were conducted during/following actual outbreaks in the US have shown that 41% of consumers, who were aware of the recall, did not look for the recalled food in their homes (Hallman, Cuite, and Hooker Citation2009), and that only 35% stopped eating that food (Blendon et al. Citation2010)

E. coli incidences may also occur sporadically where the risk is more likely to be presented in a ‘chronic’ context, and in these cases risks may be perceived as relatively ‘lower.’ In these cases, people are much less motivated to change behaviors, including self-protective ones (Fischer, Frewer, and Nauta Citation2006). Underreporting of the illness has been reported as a significant problem (Kaptan and Fischhoff Citation2011) due to being unsure of the cause, not knowing who to contact, believing reporting would not be helpful, and being too ill (Arendt et al. Citation2013). It is therefore relevant to compare perceptions of E. coli incidences across ‘acute’ and ‘chronic’ contexts.

Acute E. coli 0157:H7 outbreak linked to fresh spinach. In September 2006, E. coli 0157:H7 infections associated with fresh spinach affected over 200 people in 26 North American states. More than 100 of these cases were hospitalized, and 31 developed a form of kidney failure (HUS) that resulted in three deaths (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Citation2006; Gelting et al. Citation2011). The source of the outbreak was identified as the processing and packaging plant of Natural Selection Foods, LLC in San Juan Bautista, CA. The precise means by which the bacteria spread to the spinach remained unknown, but US public health agencies were able to make predictions based on field work (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2007).

Following the outbreak, the lettuce safety initiative that had been launched in 2006 was expanded to include spinach (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2013b). This initiative aimed to reduce public health risks by focusing on the product, agents and areas of greatest concern and to alert consumers early and respond rapidly in the event of an outbreak.

During the outbreak, US Food and Drug Administration’s (FDA) first advice to consumers was not to eat bagged fresh spinach (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2006c). This was updated the next day to not to eat fresh spinach and fresh spinach containing products (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2006b). The advice was updated again to confirm that spinach grown in non-implicated areas was safe to consume (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2006a). One consequence of FDA’s communication was that around 18% of American consumers surveyed reported that they had stopped buying other bagged vegetables (Cuite et al., Citationforthcoming). Bagged spinach expenditures were still 10% down at the end of 2007. In addition, over a period of 68 weeks, retail expenditures decreased 20% for bagged spinach, 1% for unbagged spinach, and 1% for all leafy greens (Arnade, Calvin, and Kuchler Citation2009).

Acute E. coli O104:H4 outbreak linked to fenugreek sprouts. Over 3800 cases of E. coli O104:H4 infections were reported in Germany between May and June 2011. More than 800 of those developed HUS that resulted in 54 deaths (Frank et al. Citation2011; Werber et al. Citation2012). In addition, several cases were reported in 12 other European countries, as well as the US and Canada (Bloch, Felczykowska, and Nejman-Faleńczyk Citation2012). Fenugreek seeds imported from Egypt were the most likely common link between the outbreak in Germany and a related outbreak in France (European Food Safety Authority Citation2011). As a result, the European Commission temporarily banned the import of fenugreek and certain seeds from Egypt to the European Market on July 6 (European Commission Citation2011). The relative rarity of the bacterial strain, the associated serious health consequences, difficulties in tracing the bacteria back to the food source, and communication failures on the part of authorities resulted in global media attention.

The German public health authorities provided information about the outbreak initially on 24 May 2011, without a reference to the affected crop. Consumers were advised the next day to be careful when eating raw tomatoes, lettuce, and cucumbers, in particular in Northern Germany, as these vegetables were believed to be the potential causes of the outbreak. On 26 May, the German authorities announced that three cucumbers from Spain were identified as the potential cause of the outbreak. On 1 June, however, the Spanish cucumbers were cleared. Finally, on 10 June, consumers were advised not to eat raw sprouts as contaminated sprouts of a Lower Saxony producer were identified as the source of the outbreak. On 23 June, an international investigation concluded that fenugreek sprouts were the common link between the German outbreak and a related outbreak in France (Werber et al. Citation2012). On 5 July, consumers were informed that the outbreak had ended but were still advised not to eat raw sprouts.

Consumer advisories during the outbreak have been criticized in terms of implicating a broad scope of unaffected produce as potential sources (cucumbers, tomatoes, and lettuce while the actual source was fenugreek sprouts). Furthermore, foreign production (Spain) was implicated in the outbreak, whereas in reality the problem had domestic origins, thus misleading consumers (Poudelet Citation2012; WHO Citation2011). As a consequence, consumer demand for a range of fresh produce, in particular, produce grown locally and imported from Spain declined considerably (Anderson Citation2011; Exner Citation2011). For example, German institutional kitchens (e.g. Stuttgart’s youth hostels) stopped serving fresh salad. As well as having potentially negative impacts on the nutritional quality of diets, the negative economic impacts on Spanish producers were severe, with concomitant impacts on the broader local communities (Anderson Citation2011).

In contrast to acute outbreaks, chronic E. coli incidents affecting only one or very few people occur relatively frequently (Tariq, Haagsma, and Havelaar Citation2011). These incidents tend not to receive much media attention and are not the focus of acute risk communication, although the consequences for affected individuals can be as severe as those who were infected during an outbreak. Optimistic bias (where people do not perceive they are personally vulnerable to a specific food risk) appears to militate against the adoption of safe domestic food hygiene practices associated with the prevention of foodborne illness (Miles and Scaife Citation2003; Redmond and Griffith Citation2004; Verbeke et al. Citation2007). In addition, engaging in safe food preparation practices may be considered as too difficult or otherwise costly or inconvenient as the risks of public health problems linked to microbial contamination of food are perceived to be low (Fischer, Frewer, and Nauta Citation2006).

3.2. Contamination of fish by environmental pollutants

Seafood, in particular fish, is an important supplier of omega-3 fatty acids, and a significant source of protein, vitamins, and minerals that are essential to maintain good health. Research has suggested that fish consumption may contribute to prevention of certain illnesses such as cardiovascular disease (Kris-Etherton, Harris, and Appel Citation2003), and cancer (Norat et al. Citation2005), and is beneficial to fetal neurodevelopment (Nesheim and Yaktine Citation2007). Increased fish consumption is frequently targeted as a public health nutrition goal (Ruxton et al. Citation2004). However, fish is also associated with environmental contaminants such as methylmercury and organochlorine compounds (e.g. PCB). Methylmercury might have adverse effects on developing fetuses (Nesheim and Yaktine Citation2007), while PCB’s might adversely affect liver, kidney, and central nervous system (Sirot, Leblanc, and Margaritis Citation2012). Vulnerabilities to risk also vary across the population (for example, pregnant women and immuno-compromised individuals are more at risk from negative effects), and it is important to examine risk–benefit perceptions across different population groups (van Dijk et al. Citation2012).

The source of contamination with methylmercury or organochloride in fish may be perceived as technological or at least unnatural in origin. Both negative and positive consequences of changes in fish consumption may be perceived to be delayed, as health impacts (both toxicity effects and positive effects of omega-3 consumption) are long term. Communicating risks and benefits of fish consumption presents a challenge for experts to target the information to the appropriate audience and to help differentially vulnerable consumers make informed decisions to optimize their own health protection (Engelberth et al. Citation2013; Verbeke et al. Citation2008).

There is some evidence to suggest that risk communication in this regard is successful. Studies assessing the effect of risk communication messages (e.g. FDA mercury advisories) on awareness and behavior of vulnerable consumer groups demonstrate that they are generally aware of the risks, and follow the recommendations related to amount and what type of fish to consume, however, particular attention is needed for less educated, less knowledgeable, and low-income groups (e.g. Driscoll, Sorensen, and Deerhake Citation2012; Lando, Fein, and Choinière Citation2012; Shimshack, Ward, and Beatty Citation2007; Teisl et al. Citation2011).

Organochlorine contaminants in farmed Atlantic salmon. An article published in the 9 January 2004, issue of Science reported that farmed Atlantic salmon (particularly from Scotland and the Faroe Islands) contained higher levels of organochlorine contaminants than wild Pacific salmon. The authors suggested that consumption of this particular fish should be limited to less than one and a half portions per month and concluded that consumption of farmed Atlantic salmon may pose risks that limit the beneficial effects of fish consumption. In response to this article, the UK Food Standards Agency immediately issued a press release, pointing out that the levels of dioxins and PCB’s found in this study were in line with those previously found by the UK Food Standards Agency (FSA), and are within safety levels set by the World Health Organization (UK Food Standards Agency Citation2004a). On 9 January, the FSA issued a more detailed response highlighting that there is no reason to avoid eating Scottish farmed salmon or any other salmon (UK Food Standards Agency Citation2004b). Later, in 2004, an inter-committee subgroup consisting of experts from the British Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition and Committee on Toxicity issued a report suggesting that the UK population should be encouraged to increase its oily fish consumption to one portion a week to confer significant public health benefits without appreciable risk from the contaminants in fish (Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition Citation2004). The Science article and subsequent responses from the Scottish Salmon Industry, and FSA received substantial media attention in the UK (BBC News Citation2004; Highfield Citation2004).

In a cross-national European study conducted with 206 participants from Germany, Greece, Norway, and UK (van Kleef et al. Citation2009), participants were interviewed about recent food safety incidents in their home countries. Fifty-two participants from the UK were interviewed about their opinions regarding contaminated farmed Atlantic salmon incident. The results suggest that UK participants were generally confused about the conflicting information provided by the media reporting the Science article and FSA’s response. They required more information about how guidelines on contaminants are developed and reviewed and wanted to be updated about follow-up activities such as investigations. They also reported that they do not trust the salmon industry because in their view the industry is more concerned about economic motivations than the safety of fish. Lack of trust in the farmed salmon industry was also found in more recent studies (Schlag and Ystgaard Citation2013), but was reported as not predicting consumption choices for salmon (Hall and Amberg Citation2013). In studies on other food safety incidents (e.g. BSE), consumption behaviors have been found to be affected by lack of trust (Pieniak et al. Citation2008; Rosati and Saba Citation2004).

Chronic contamination of salmon resulted in reduced levels of trust in food industry (van Kleef et al. Citation2009; Schlag and Ystgaard Citation2013). However, in order to quantify the effects of this increased risk perception and reduced trust on fish consumption, additional research is needed on people’s attitude toward nuclear power. As part of this, it is important to segregate risk perceptions associated with farmed salmon from those associated with fish in general, in order to establish the extent to which risk perceptions have generalized to other fish or seafood species. Thus, without further analysis, the long-term public health impacts of this chronic food safety incident are unknown.

3.3. Radioactive contamination of food following a nuclear accident

Nuclear power has long been perceived as unacceptably risky by some members of the public. Incidents such as the Chernobyl accident have highlighted the potentially negative effects of a nuclear accident to human and environmental health in general (Drottz-Sjöberg and Sjoberg Citation1990; Renn Citation1990), and the human food chain in particular (BBC News Citation2011; Beach Citation1990). In the early 2000’s, nuclear power has been repositioned as a solution to mitigate climate change because it has the potential to contribute to the growing demand for energy without emitting carbon dioxide to the atmosphere (International Atomic Energy Agency Citation2013; Sailor et al. Citation2000; US Department of Energy Citation2005; Whitfield et al. Citation2009), although its adoption in this regard is controversial (Sovacool and Cooper Citation2008). Accordingly, research has suggested people’s attitude toward nuclear power is becoming less negative (Brook Citation2012; Goodfellow, Williams, and Azapagic Citation2011). However, the catastrophic Fukushima nuclear accident that occurred, following a tsunami, in Japan in March 2011 may have had a negative impact upon this trend toward more positive attitudes (Kanda, Tsuji, and Yonehara Citation2012; Poortinga, Aoyagi, and Pidgeon Citation2013).

Although not strictly a ‘technological food production’ related hazard, this case represents the occurrence of an acute food hazard with technological origins, where perceptions (based on those learned from previous examples of similar incidents) are formed rapidly under conditions of uncertainty, and are linked to unintended and uncontrollable effects of technology. In addition, perceptions are shaped by uncertainties associated with the geographic and temporal ‘spread’ of impacts, in particular immediately after the crisis has occurred (Hamada and Ogino Citation2012).

Research shows that most consumers (both in the country of the incident and neighboring countries) avoid purchasing products from affected areas in the aftermath of the incident and this avoidance behavior still continues in the following years although at a lower extent (e.g. Belyakov Citation2015; Hee et al. Citation2014; Sawada, Aizaki, and Sato Citation2014; Turcanu et al. Citation2007). However, avoidance from the food grown in contaminated areas also depends on food availability. For example, in the aftermath of the Chernobyl accident, many people continued consuming contaminated food due to poverty and lack of alternative food supply (Belyakov Citation2015). The accident at the Fukushima Dai-ichi nuclear power plant (NPP). On 11 March 2011, an earthquake of 9.0 magnitude created a powerful tsunami that flooded the Fukushima Dai-ichi NPP in Japan. As the flooding cut off power for cooling and created malfunction of all backup systems, reactors overheated, and a marked amount of radiation was released to the environment, including the Pacific Ocean.

The Japanese authorities evacuated citizens living within 20 km radius and suggested that people living in the radius of 20–30 km of the plant remain indoors. On 17 March , provisional standards for radioactivity in foods were established as radioactive contamination of food was observed in areas far from the NPP. These standards were subsequently revised and lowered in April 2011 (Baba Citation2013).

There were no initial deaths or serious exposures to radiation at the NPP. However, the evacuation resulted in 60 immediate deaths of patients or elderly people in nursing homes and health care facilities due to deterioration of serious medical conditions (González et al. Citation2013). On 16 December , more than 9 months after the accident, the Japanese authorities declared the plant to be stable, although acknowledging that it would take decades to decontaminate the surrounding areas (Buerk Citation2011).

The Japanese Government announced a comprehensive review of its energy policy to emphasize renewable sources. In addition, all NPP’s in Japan have either been closed or had their operations suspended for safety inspections and maintenance. The accident has also affected other countries’ future energy plans. In response to German citizens’ rising concerns about nuclear energy as a result of the Fukushima accident, Germany announced plans to shut down all its nuclear reactors by 2022. Similarly, Switzerland agreed to phase out its five aging power reactors, and Italy decided to exclude nuclear energy from its future energy mix.

Communication efforts by the Japanese authorities to the public during and aftermath of the disaster have been widely criticized due to lack of transparency, downplaying the extent of the disaster, and failure to warn about likely events as raising concerns, as well as shedding doubt on credibility of the government (Figueroa Citation2013; Funabashi and Kitazawa Citation2012; González et al. Citation2013; Ng and Lean Citation2012; Poortinga, Aoyagi, and Pidgeon Citation2013). There is evidence from within Japan that public support for nuclear power, which was not high before the incident, has been further reduced (Figueroa Citation2013; Kato et al. Citation2013; Poortinga, Aoyagi, and Pidgeon Citation2013).

People’s risk perceptions associated with nuclear contamination are extremely high, both in terms of general environmental contamination (Slovic Citation2012), and in relation to the food supply (Burger Citation2012). Thus, even a low level of radioactive contamination of foods may result in consumer rejection, even if the level of contamination is similar in magnitude to naturally occurring background levels of radiation. As a consequence, in the short term, foods which are technically safe to consume may be rejected by consumers (International Atomic Energy Agency Citation2013). This is particular concern in a crisis situation, for example, following a nuclear accident, where it may be difficult to provide adequate food supplies to the effected population as other crisis management activities (e.g. evacuation, provision of medical aid) may have higher priorities in terms of resource allocation. Given that contamination is likely to be perceived as ubiquitous within the region, short-term problems associated with under nutrition may occur (Spirichev et al. Citation2006). Thus, risk perceptions may result in acute nutritional deficiencies in a population which is dealing with multiple potential health, injury, and infrastructure concerns associated with the aftermath of an earthquake. In the long term, there is the potential for all foods produced or stored within the vicinity of the nuclear accident to be ‘stigmatized’ or rejected. Thus, consumer rejection of food produced in the affected region may have negative impacts on the local economy. This may extend beyond local consumers and affect export markets. National food production, unaffected by the nuclear incident itself, may be stigmatized, which will further impact on the national or even regional economy. In summary, such incidents may cause consumers to act rapidly to protect themselves from harm, but in the long term, their risk perceptions may ‘stigmatize’ foods produced within local production systems, with concomitant negative socioeconomic impacts. In a crisis, when food availability is potentially an issue, health problems associated with malnutrition may result from the perception that all local food supplies have been contaminated by radiation (WHO Citation2013). In order to quantify these relationships, it is important to measure risk perceptions and dietary choices immediately after the incident has occurred, as economic measures are unlikely to be reliable owing to multiple perturbances. Longitudinal analysis might usefully correlate economic data associated with local food production (both in terms of price and volume) with risk perceptions of local consumers, and consumers in export markets for local and national products.

3.4. GM animals applied to food production

GM technology has been applied to various crops, including those intended for food and animal feed, and to production animals (Cowan, Citationforthcoming; Frewer et al. Citation2013a). However, food products derived from GM animals have not yet entered the US and European market, although regulatory approval appears imminent for some applications (Maxmen Citation2012; US Food and Drug Administration Citation2015a; Vàzquez-Salat et al. Citation2012). Medical applications based on pharmaceuticals derived from GM animals are more widespread internationally (Houdebine Citation2009, Citation2011), in particular in relation to disease models (Laible Citation2009; Prather, Shen, and Dai Citation2008). The use of GM animals in food production systems potentially confers benefits in terms of food safety, enhanced nutrition, and improved food security (Niemann and Kues Citation2007). Consumer perceptions of risk are higher for GM animal-related food applications than plant-related applications, and may militate against their use in food production. Other areas of application such as medical applications appear more acceptable to the public, primarily because the benefits are perceived to outweigh the risks (Frewer et al. Citation2014; Frewer et al. Citation2013b). Particular concerns are associated with animal welfare issues, and perceptions that negative environmental impacts may be associated with intended or unintended environmental releases of GM animals (Einsiedel Citation2005). What distinguishes the case of GM animals applied to agriculture to the other cases presented here is that it is associated not only with high levels of risk perception, but also high levels of moral or ethical concerns on the part of the public, particularly in the US and Asia (Frewer et al. Citation2013b).

GM salmon destined for the human food chain. At the time of writing, a GM salmon destined for human consumption has been approved for sale by Health Canada (Health Canada AquAdvantage Salmon Citation2016), and also approved as safe and nutritious to eat by the FDA (US Food and Drug Administration Citation2015b). The genetic modification increases growth rate (Aerni Citation2004), anticipating increased demand for fish and fish products over the coming decade (OECD and Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Citationforthcoming). Accordingly, GM fish has been considered as a sustainable solution in terms of food security. Atlantic salmon is one of the food species that has been subjected to GM (Menozzi, Mora, and Merigo Citation2012). It has been argued that GM salmon offers nutritional advantages, including resistance to environmental stressors and pathogens, and increased availability of omega three fatty acids (US Food and Drug Administration 2013a). Disadvantages may be associated with the need to ensure allergens are not introduced into the human food chain (Nakamura et al. Citation2009), and less than 100% sterility resulting in potential cross-breeding with wild varieties of salmon (Le Curieux-Belfond et al. Citation2009). The advantage for consumers may be economic (retail price reduction), or nutritional (increased availability of foods rich in heath promoting components) (Mora et al. Citation2012). Against this, the issue of environmental impact (for example, unintended release of animals into the environment and animal welfare concerns) remains a potential source of controversy (Frewer et al. Citation2014). One recent study (Mather, Vikan, and Knight Citation2016) that measured Norwagian consumers’ willingness to purchase GM-labeled salmon found aversion to GM animal concept and highlighted the necessity of communicating consumer benefits for GM salmon to be accepted in markets where GM labeling is required. Therefore, the primary drivers of risk perceptions, at least in Europe, appear to be perceptions that the application of GM technologies to animals is risky. Consumers in North America and South-East Asia tend to be more concerned about moral and ethical issues (Frewer et al. Citation2013b). The lack of equity of distribution of benefits across different countries and across populations is regarded as another potential issue militating against the development of such production animals. Perceptions leading to consumer rejection are not linked to the use of GM animals per se, but rather focused on their use in the food supply chain.

In the case of GM animals used in food production, it is difficult to argue that consumers’ risk perceptions militate against their own interests. It could be argued that lower prices for animal proteins high in beneficial nutrients represents a considerable consumer benefit which will deliver advantages to public health, although this may not be such an important benefit given that early innovations are destined for more affluent countries (Menozzi, Mora, and Merigo Citation2012). Of potentially greater importance regarding consumer adoption is the issue of how moral and ethical concerns contribute to rejection of GM animals in food (Kaiser et al. Citation2007). It is suggested that the principle of informed choice, through adoption and implementation of an effective traceability and labeling policy, will prove beneficial if and when products are released into the market. Even if consumers perceive that adequate risk assessment procedures are introduced, that animal welfare standards are met, and that governance structures are adequate, perceived benefits may not outweigh moral and ethical concerns. However, providing further information about risk and benefit assessments is unlikely to alleviative these, and this is what distinguishes the GM case for the others presented in this paper. In this case, measuring moral concerns, and how these change (for example, after the initial commercialization of products developed using GM animals) may be a useful tool for developing policies further.

4. Discussion

Four food safety cases where risk perceptions associated with the food hazards have been presented. In three of these (E.coli outbreaks, linked to fresh spinach and fenugreek sprouts, organochlorine contaminants in farmed salmon, and radioactive contamination following the Fukushima accident), it was concluded that risk perceptions may lead consumers to behave in a way contrary to their own, and societal interests. In the fourth (GM salmon destined for the human food chain), moral concerns may influence consumer behavior to a greater extent than risk perceptions. In relation to these cases, how technological vs. natural and acute vs. chronic nature of food hazards affected people’s risk and benefit perceptions, and associated attitudes and behaviors, has been examined.

An initial starting point to examine the acute vs. chronic dimension is that in risk assessment more is known about the acute effects of a food safety incident compared to long-term impacts, in particular when examining (changes in) risk perceptions and subsequent consumption behaviors. In the case of the E. coli outbreaks, and the reporting of contamination of Atlantic salmon, short-term impacts can be ‘metricized’ through analysis of changes in risk perceptions and consumption and sales patterns. The long-term impacts on dietary choices have not, to our knowledge, been analyzed, and it is not clear how risk and benefit perceptions affect dietary choices in the long term. These chronic effects are particularly complex because vulnerabilities to the risk change through the lifecycle of consumers (for example, with respect to age and immune status) (Ma and Fang Citation2013; Wada et al. Citation2013), and also vary between different demographic groups (for example, with respect to gender) (McCombe, Greer, and Mackay Citation2009; Yan et al. Citation2010). In addition, improved scientific knowledge, for example about toxicology may result in food choice dilemmas in the future. A recent example is that of inorganic arsenic in the food supply (Llorente-Mirandes et al. Citation2014), where reports of relatively high levels of arsenic in vegetables may fuel consumer risk perceptions, with the consequence of reduced vegetable consumption. This reduction may lead to net negative impact on long-term public health. Longitudinal assessment of the relationship between risk perceptions and consumer choices is required. Linking these data with economic assessment would be useful in order to determine the socio-economic impacts (for example, to local producers in the case of Fukushima). In the case of GM Salmon, such analysis would need to be projected at present, as approval is pending. If approval is given to commercialize GM salmon in the human food chain, there may be ample opportunity to assess the relative influence of consumer risk perceptions and moral and ethical concerns, on purchasing and consumption. Analysis of external changes (for example, societal debate about synthetic biology applications in food production) might further crystallize public opinion regarding the biological sciences in general (Torgersen Citation2009; Torgersen and Schmidt Citation2013).

The analysis of the cases over the risk origin (technological vs. natural) presented support for the contention that technologies applied to food production are associated with higher levels of risk perception, potentially because they are perceived to be unnatural. However, the available evidence suggests that intrinsic (or intuitive) consumer concerns about ethical or moral issues are closely associated with the introduction of GM animals applied to food production, more than ‘objective’ hazards like health risks. The issue of whether alternative, less controversial, technological approaches may be available to deliver the same benefits may also need to be considered, as this is an issue influencing consumer acceptance (Gupta, Fischer, and Frewer Citation2012). In contrast, while it is possible to construct extrinsic ethical arguments regarding the risks of nuclear power (for example, the potential for environmental harm), this would relate to risks of a nuclear accident, rather than a concern located in the development and application of the (enabling) technology itself.

The importance of developing trust has been highlighted in all of the cases, although this may have greatest impact in terms of long-term consumer responses to risk–benefit communications (Berg Citation2004; Frewer et al. Citation2015). Communicating risk uncertainty to the public has emerged as an important issue. Therefore, communicating transparent and honest information, in particular telling the consumers what the authorities know and do not know, with clear recommendations for actionable behavior changes if relevant, may increase trust in information following the occurrence of a food safety incident (Frewer et al. Citation2015; Kaptan and Fischhoff Citation2010). Communication of uncertainties associated with the scientific assessment of risks and benefits may also be relevant where these exist, and need to be communicated to consumers in terms of consumer protection or the generation of consumer confidence in information (Beck and Kropp Citation2011; Thompson Citation2002). In the case of both acute and chronic risks, it is noticeable that transparency about new scientific information, novel technologies, and internal decision-making processes of regulatory agencies rises as an important determinant affecting risk perceptions.

The Fukushima accident case exemplifies the importance of prior attitudes and value orientations toward nuclear power in risk information, suggesting that there is no single public with regard to energy preferences and corresponding risk beliefs but rather there are multiple populations with different viewpoints (Greenberg and Truelove Citation2011; Whitfield et al. Citation2009). This result seems to some extent similar to those of experimental studies involving prior attitudes toward food (Fischer and Frewer Citation2009; Van Dijk et al. Citation2012).

In all the cases presented here, consumers needed to make informed decisions by understanding and balancing their decisions regarding both risks and benefits associated with associated food choice behaviors. If relevant risk–benefit information is not available, people may rely on judgmental heuristics, or rules of thumb such as availability heuristic (Gilovich, Griffin, and Kahneman Citation2003; Kahneman, Slovic, and Tversky Citation1982). The availability heuristic may explain why foods are rejected when only risk information is provided. As a consequence, the associated risk attitude will be the most available and most influential attitudinal influence on consumers’ decisions. Other inferences are derived from people’s existing mental models allowing them a framework to interpret issues in the news media, participate in discussions, feel competent to make decisions, and generate options (Fischhoff Citation2009). Mental models can provide essential structure in understanding risk communication, but also produce incorrect conclusions if they contain incorrect beliefs and/or misconceptions. Therefore communications need to be tested before (and evaluated after) they are utilized because mental models of risk communicators and the target audience may be different, thus leading to unexpected impacts of the communication.

It should be possible to extrapolate the results of the case studies presented here to emerging food risks, where there is little existing data regarding risk perceptions. Emerging food risk issues include synthetic biology (both in general and as applied to food production) (Pauwels Citation2013), increased mycotoxin levels in the global food supply (Wu Citation2006), and inorganic arsenic in food and water (Moreno-Jimenez, Esteban, and Penalosa Citation2012; Smith and Steinmaus Citation2009). The case studies highlight an important research need in the context of examining the relationship between risk perceptions and impacts (whether on health or socioeconomic functioning of affected societies). While there is some evidence to assess the short-term impacts of food risk events (for example, in relation to sales volumes of foods and food commodities associated with a food risk incident), the long-term impacts are not understood, as other external factors also influence consumer demand. Developing metrics to assess this would not only enable greater understanding of the relationship between risk perceptions and consumer behaviors, but also allow mapping of the ‘stigmatization’ following a risk incident of foods in potentially affected food chains or regions. The analysis of big data, in particular internet searches and social media activity such as Twitter may serve to link perceptions and concerns to food safety incidents (Wilson and Brownstein Citation2009). While such analyses may be limited in terms of pinpointing local incidents, in particular in developing countries (Carneiro and Mylonakis Citation2009), consumer concern following an announcement of such incidents can be monitored, and risk communication adjusted accordingly. In addition, methods to simultaneously quantify risk perceptions, psychological impacts, and impacts on local and regional economies are needed, which have to be utilized in conjunction with assessments of public health and environmental impacts, possibly utilized the same models. Methodologies which can harmonize natural and social science data sets are needed to generate predictive power in this respect.

5. Conclusions

In this study, two axes, risk origin (natural vs. technological) and acute vs. chronic dimension have framed the analysis of four different food safety incidents. In the case of the ‘naturally’ occurring incidents, it was concluded that there is potential for risk perceptions to override consumer best interests from the perspective of optimal nutrition (in particular relative to under consumption of health promoting nutrients). In the case of technological potential hazards (e.g. a food safety incident linked to a nuclear accident), consumers own interests may also be harmed in the short term, and therefore developing a comprehensive understanding of consumer perceptions as well as technical risk estimates is needed to develop effective communication. In the case of GM animals applied to food production, other concerns, which are potentially moral or ethical in nature, may be more relevant to consumer acceptance than their risk perceptions. In this case, it is difficult to argue that consumer risk perceptions are operating contrary to their own interests. The principle of consumer choice (implemented through effective traceability and labeling policies for GM animal food products) may allow consumers to make judgments according to their own ethical priorities, although this will not be helpful for those individuals for whom a moratorium on, or discontinuation of, research is required. In the case of acute and chronic food safety incidents, long-term analysis linking perceptions to robust measures of impact is important but infrequent. Future research should aim to quantify the links between risk perceptions, behaviors economic effects, and public health and environmental risk indicators.

Funding

This work was supported by a travel grant from the Harvard Center for Risk Analysis to present at the Risk, Perception, and Response Conference that was held at the Harvard School of Public Health on 20–21 March 2014.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

References

  • Aerni, P. 2004. “Risk, Regulation and Innovation: The Case of Aquaculture and Transgenic Fish.” Aquatic Sciences 66: 327–341.
  • Anderson, Richard. 2011. “E. coli: Economic Impact on the Agriculture Industry.” BBC News, June 3. fish. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/business-13627083
  • Arendt, S., L. Rajagopal, C. Strohbehn, N. Stokes, J. Meyer, and S. Mandernach. 2013. “Reporting of Foodborne Illness by U.S. Consumers and Healthcare Professionals.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 10: 3684–3714. doi:10.1057/jphp.2010.42.
  • Arnade, Carlos, Linda Calvin, and Fred Kuchler. 2009. “Consumer Response to a Food Safety Shock: The 2006 Food-borne Illness Outbreak of E. coli O157: H7 Linked to Spinach.” Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy 31 (4): 734–750. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9353.2009.01464.x.
  • Baba, M. 2013. “Fukushima Accident: What Happened?” Radiation Measurements 55: 17–21.10.1016/j.radmeas.2013.01.013
  • BBC News. 2004. “Scare Over Farmed Salmon Safety.” January 8. Salmon farmed in Scotland is among the most tainted with cancer-causing chemicals, US scientists have warned. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/health/3380735.stm
  • BBC News. 2011. “Chernobyl Radiation Checks on Welsh Farms Reviewed.” April 26. The Foods Standards Agency is Reviewing how it Monitors 330 Hill Farms in Wales, 25 years after the World’s Worst Nuclear Accident at Chernobyl. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-wales-north-west-wales-13196041
  • Beach, H. 1990. “Perceptions of Risk, Dilemmas of Policy: Nuclear Fallout in Swedish Lapland.” Social Science & Medicine 30 (6): 729–738.10.1016/0277-9536(88)90259-6
  • Beck, G., and C. Kropp. 2011. “Is Science Based Consumer Advice Prepared to Deal with Uncertainties in Second Modernity? – The Role of Scientific Experts in Risk Communication in the Case of Food Supplements.” Science, Technology & Innovation Studies 6 (2): 203–224.
  • Belyakov, A. 2015. “From Chernobyl to Fukushima: An Interdisciplinary Framework for Managing and Communicating Food Security Risks after Nuclear Plant Accidents.” Journal of Environmental Studies and Sciences 5 (3): 404–417. doi:10.1007/s13412-015-0284-2.
  • Berg, Lisbet. 2004. “Trust in Food in the Age of Mad Cow Disease: A Comparative Study of Consumers’ Evaluation of Food Safety in Belgium, Britain and Norway.” Appetite 42 (1): 21–32. doi:10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00112-0.
  • Blendon, R. J., J. M. Benson, G. K. SteelFisher, K. J. Weldon, and M. J. Herrman. 2010. “Harvard Opinion Research Program Harvard School of Public Health Egg Recall Survey (August 27–30, 2010).” http://www.fda.gov/downloads/AdvisoryCommittees/CommitteesMeetingMaterials/RiskCommunicationAdvisoryCommittee/UCM231802.pdf
  • Bloch, S. K., A. Felczykowska, and B. Nejman-Faleńczyk. 2012. “Escherichia coli O104:H4 Outbreak – Have We Learnt a Lesson From it?” Acta Biochemica Polonica 59: 483–488.
  • Brook, Barry W. 2012. “Could Nuclear Fission Energy, etc., Solve the Greenhouse Problem? The Affirmative Case.” Energy Policy 42: 4–8.10.1016/j.enpol.2011.11.041
  • Buerk, Roland. 2011. “Japan PM Says Fukushima Nuclear Site Finally Stabilised.” BBC News, Tokyo, December 16. The Crippled Nuclear Reactors at Japan’s Fukushima Power Plant Have Finally Been Stabilised, Prime Minister Yoshihiko Noda has Announced. Accessed January 11, 2015. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world-asia-16212057
  • Burger, Joanna. 2012. “Rating of Worry about Energy Sources with Respect to Public Health, Environmental Health, and Workers.” Journal of Risk Research 15 (9): 1159–1169. doi:10.1080/13669877.2012.705316.
  • Carneiro, Herman A., and Eleftherios Mylonakis. 2009. “Google Trends: A Web-based Tool for Real-time Surveillance of Disease Outbreaks.” Clinical Infectious Diseases 49 (10): 1557–1564. doi:10.1086/630200.
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2006. “Update on Multi-state Outbreak of E. coli O157:H7 Infections from Fresh Spinach.” October 6, 2006. Accessed on January 13, 2015. http://www.cdc.gov/ecoli/2006/september/updates/100606.htm
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2013. Surveillance for Foodborne Disease Outbreaks – United States, 2009–2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 62:3: U.S. Government Printing Office. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/pdf/wk/mm6203.pdf: 41–60.
  • Cohen, J., D. Bellinger, W. Connor, Penny M. Krisetherton, R. Lawrence, D. Savitz, B. Shaywitz, S. Teutsch, and G. Gray. 2005. “A Quantitative Risk-benefit Analysis of Changes in Population Fish Consumption.” American Journal of Preventive Medicine 29 (4): 325. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2005.07.003.
  • Cowan, Tadlock. Forthcoming. “Biotechnology in Animal Agriculture: Status and Current Issues.” RL33334. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://cnie.org/NLE/CRSreports/10Oct/RL33334.pdf
  • Cuite, C. L., S. C. Condry, M. L. Nucci, and W. K. Hallman. Forthcoming. “Public Response to the Contaminated Spinach Recall of 2006.” RR-0107-013. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://foodpolicy.rutgers.edu/docs/pubs/2007_PublicResponsetotheContaminatedSpinachRecallof2006.pdf
  • van Dijk, Heleen, Arnout R. H. Fischer, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2011. “Consumer Responses to Integrated Risk-benefit Information Associated with the Consumption of Food.” Risk Analysis 31 (3): 429–439. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01505.x.
  • van Dijk, Heleen, Ellen van Kleef, Helen Owen, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2012. “Consumer Preferences Regarding Food-related Risk-benefit Messages.” British Food Journal 114 (3): 387–400. doi:10.1108/00070701211213483.
  • Dreyer, Marion, Ortwin Renn, Shannon Cope, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2010. “Including Social Impact Assessment in Food Safety Governance.” Food Control 21 (12): 1620–1628. doi:10.1016/j.foodcont.2009.05.007.
  • Driscoll, D., A. Sorensen, and M. A. Deerhake. 2012. “A Multidisciplinary Approach to Promoting Healthy Subsistence Fish Consumption in Culturally Distinct Communities.” Health Promotion Practice 13 (2): 245–251. doi:10.1177/1524839910380156.
  • Drottz-Sjöberg, B. M., and L. Sjoberg. 1990. “Risk Perception and Worries after the Chernobyl Accident.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 10 (2): 135–149.10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80124-0
  • Einsiedel, E. F. 2005. “Public Perceptions of Transgenic Animals.” Revue Scientifique et Technique de l’OIE 24 (1): 149–157.10.20506/rst.issue.24.1.28
  • Eisenhardt, K. M. 1989. “Building Theories From Case Study Research.” Academy of Management Review 14 (4): 532–550.
  • Engelberth, Haley, Mario F. Teisl, Eric Frohmberg, Karyn Butts, Kathleen P. Bell, Sue Stableford, and Andrew E. Smith. 2013. “Can Fish Consumption Advisories do Better? Providing Benefit and Risk Information to Increase Knowledge.” Environmental Research 126: 232–239. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2013.08.012.
  • European Food Safety Authority. 2011. Technical Report of EFSA on Tracing Seeds, in Particular Fenugreek (Trigonella foenum-graecum) Seeds, in Relation to the Shiga Toxin-producing E. coli (STEC) O104:H4 2011 Outbreaks in Germany and France. July 5. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.2903/sp.efsa.2011.EN-176/pdf
  • European Food Safety Authority. 2013. “The European Union Summary Report on Trends and Sources of Zoonoses, Zoonotic Agents and Food-borne Outbreaks in 2011.” Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.efsa.europa.eu/en/efsajournal/doc/3129.pdf
  • Exner, Ulrich. 2011. “Spaniens Gurken waren es nicht. Und nun?” Die Welt, May 31. http://www.welt.de/gesundheit/article13405862/Spaniens-Gurken-waren-es-nicht-Und-nun.html.
  • FDA. FDA Has Determined that the AquAdvantage Salmon is as Safe to Eat as Non-GE Salmon. 2015b. FDA Consumer Updates, November 2015. Accessed June 11, 2016. http://www.fda.gov/downloads/ForConsumers/ConsumerUpdates/UCM473578.pdf
  • “FDA AquAdvantage Salmon Environmental Assessment. 2015a. Accessed June 11, 2016. http://www.fda.gov/downloads/AnimalVeterinary/DevelopmentApprovalProcess/GeneticEngineering/GeneticallyEngineeredAnimals/UCM466218.pdf
  • “FDA Finalizes Report on 2006 Spinach Outbreak.” 2007. News Release, March 23. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.fda.gov/newsevents/newsroom/pressannouncements/2007/ucm108873.htm.
  • “FDA has Determined that the AuaAdvantage Salmon is as Safe to Eat as Non-GE Salmon.” 2015. FDA Consumer Health Information, November 2015. Accessed April 30, 2016. http://www.fda.gov/downloads/ForConsumers/ConsumerUpdates/UCM473578.pdf
  • “FDA Leafy Greens Safety Initiative Continues (2nd year).” 2013b. News Release, July 30. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.fda.gov/Food/FoodborneIllnessContaminants/BuyStoreServeSafeFood/ucm115898.htm
  • “FDA Statement on Foodborne E. coli O157:H7 Outbreak in Spinach.” 2006a. News Release, September 22. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/2006/ucm109578.htm
  • “FDA Statement on Foodborne E.coli O157:H7 Outbreak in Spinach.” 2006b. News Release, September 15. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/2006/ucm108732.htm
  • “FDA Warning on Serious Foodborne E. coli O157:H7 Outbreak: One Death and Multiple Hospitalizations in Several States.” 2006c. News Release, September 14. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.fda.gov/NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/2006/ucm108731.htm
  • Figueroa, P. M. 2013. “Risk Communication Surrounding the Fukushima Nuclear Disaster: An Anthropological Approach.” Asia Europe Journal 11: 53–64.10.1007/s10308-013-0343-9
  • Fischer, Arnout R. H., and Lynn J. Frewer. 2009. “Consumer Familiarity With Foods and the Perception of Risks and Benefits.” Food Quality and Preference 20 (8): 576–585. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.06.008.
  • Fischer, Arnout R. H., Lynn J. Frewer, and Maarten J. Nauta. 2006. “Toward Improving Food Safety in the Domestic Environment: A Multi-item Rasch Scale for the Measurement of the Safety Efficacy of Domestic Food-handling Practices.” Risk Analysis 26 (5): 1323–1338. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2006.00813.x.
  • Fischhoff, Baruch. 2009. “Risk Perception and Communication.” In Oxford Textbook of Public Health, edited by Roger Detels, R. Beaglehole, M. A. Lansing, and M. Gulliford, 5th ed., 940–952. Oxford: Oxford Medical Publications, Oxford University Press.
  • Fischhoff, Baruch, and John D. Kadvany. 2011. Risk: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/actrade/9780199576203.001.0001
  • Fischhoff, Baruch, Paul Slovic, S. Lichtenstein, S. Read, and B. Combs. 1978. “How Safe is Safe Enough? A Psychometric Study of Attitudes Towards Technological Risks and Benefits.” Policy Sciences 9: 127–152.10.1007/BF00143739
  • Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations/World Health Organization (FAO/WHO). Forthcoming. “A Handbook on Risk Communication Applied to Food Safety.” Accessed May 1, 2015. http://www.auv-ks.net/repository/docs/Final_version_Handbook_28-11-2014.pdf
  • Food Standards Agency. 2004a. “FSA Response to Salmon Study Published in Science Magazine.” News Release, January 8. Accessed January 14, 2015. http://www.reading.ac.uk/foodlaw/news/uk-04002.htm
  • Food Standards Agency. 2004b.“Response to Salmon Study Published in Science Magazine.” News Release, January 9. Accessed January 14, 2015. http://www.food.gov.uk/multimedia/salmon080104
  • Food Standards Agency. 2013. “Annual Report of the Chief Scientist 2012/13.” Accessed January 13, 2015. http://food.gov.uk/multimedia/pdfs/publication/cstar_2013.pdf
  • Frank, Christina, Dirk Werber, Jakob P. Cramer, Mona Askar, Mirko Faber, Matthias an der Heiden, Helen Bernard, et al. 2011. “Epidemic Profile of Shiga-Toxin–Producing Escherichia coli O104:H4 Outbreak in Germany.” New England Journal of Medicine 365 (19): 1771–1780. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1106483.
  • Frewer, Lynn J., K. Bergmann, M. Brennan, R. Lion, R. M. Meertens, Gene Rowe, M. Siegrist, and C. M. J. L. Vereijken. 2011. “Consumer Response to Novel Agri-food Technologies: Implications for Predicting Consumer Acceptance of Emerging Food Technologies.” Trends in Food Science & Technology 22 (8): 442–456. doi:10.1016/j.tifs.2011.05.005.
  • Frewer, Lynn J., Gijs A. Kleter, M. Brennan, David Coles, Arnout R. H. Fischer, Louis-Marie Houdebine, Cristina Mora, Kate Millar, and Brian Salter. 2013a. “Genetically Modified Animals From Life-science, Socio-economic and Ethical Perspectives: Examining Issues in an EU Policy Context.” New Biotechnology 30 (5): 447–460. doi:10.1016/j.nbt.2013.03.010.
  • Frewer, Lynn J., Ivo A. van der Lans, Arnout R. H. Fischer, Machiel J. Reinders, Davide Menozzi, Xiaoyong Zhang, Isabelle van den Berg, and Karin L. Zimmermann. 2013b. “Public Perceptions of Agri-food Applications of Genetic Modification – A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis.” Trends in Food Science & Technology 30 (2): 142–152. doi:10.1016/j.tifs.2013.01.003.
  • Frewer, Lynn J., David Coles, Louis-Marie Houdebine, and Gijs A. Kleter. 2014. “Attitudes Towards Genetically Modified Animals in Food Production.” British Food Journal 116 (8): 1291–1313. doi:10.1108/BFJ-08-2013-0211.
  • Frewer, L. J., A. R. H. Fischer, M. Brennan, D. Banati, R. Lion, R. M. Meertens, G. Rowe, et al. 2015. “Risk/Benefit Communication About Food – A Systematic Review of the Literature.” Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 56 (10): 1728–1745. doi:10.1080/10408398.2013.801337.
  • Funabashi, Y., and K. Kitazawa. 2012. “Fukushima in Review: A Complex Disaster, a Disastrous Response.” Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists 68 (2): 9–21.10.1177/0096340212440359
  • Gelting, Richard J., Mansoor A. Baloch, Max A. Zarate-Bermudez, and Carol Selman. 2011. “Irrigation Water Issues Potentially Related to the 2006 Multistate E. coli O157:H7 Outbreak Associated with Spinach.” Agricultural Water Management 98 (9): 1395–1402. doi:10.1016/j.agwat.2011.04.004.
  • Gilovich, T., D. Griffin, and Daniel Kahneman, eds. 2003. Judgment Under Uncertainty II: Extensions and Applications. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Glik, Deborah C. 2007. “Risk Communication for Public Health Emergencies.” Annual Review of Public Health 28 (1): 33–54. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.021406.144123.
  • González, Abel J., Makoto Akashi, John D. Boice Jr., Masamichi Chino, Toshimitsu Homma, Nobuhito Ishigure, Michiaki Kai, et al. 2013. “Radiological Protection Issues Arising During and After the Fukushima Nuclear Reactor Accident.” Journal of Radiological Protection 33 (3): 497–571. doi:10.1088/0952-4746/33/3/497.
  • Goodfellow, Martin J., Hugo R. Williams, and Adisa Azapagic. 2011. “Nuclear Renaissance, Public Perception and Design Criteria: An Exploratory Review.” Energy Policy 39 (10): 6199–6210. doi:10.1016/j.enpol.2011.06.068.
  • Greenberg, Michael, and Heather B. Truelove. 2011. “Energy Choices and Risk Beliefs: Is It Just Global Warming and Fear of a Nuclear Power Plant Accident?” Risk Analysis 31 (5): 819–831. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2010.01535.x.
  • Gregory, R., Paul Slovic, and J. Flynn. 1996. “Risk Perceptions, Stigma, and Health Policy.” Health & Place 2 (4): 213–220.10.1016/1353-8292(96)00019-6
  • Gupta, N., Arnout R. H. Fischer, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2012. “Socio-psychological Determinants of Public Acceptance of Technologies: A Review.” Public Understanding of Science 21 (7): 782–795. doi:10.1177/0963662510392485.
  • Hall, Troy E., and Shannon M. Amberg. 2013. “Factors Influencing Consumption of Farmed Seafood Products in the Pacific Northwest.” Appetite 66: 1–9.10.1016/j.appet.2013.02.012
  • Hallman, W. K., C. L. Cuite, and N. H. Hooker. 2009. “Consumer Responses to Food Recalls: 2008 National Survey Report.” RR-0109-018. Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey, Food Policy Institute. http://foodpolicy.rutgers.edu/docs/pubs/RR-0109-018.pdf.
  • Hamada, Nobuyuki, and Haruyuki Ogino. 2012. “Food Safety Regulations: What We Learned from the Fukushima Nuclear Accident.” Journal of Environmental Radioactivity 111: 83–99.10.1016/j.jenvrad.2011.08.008
  • Health Canada AquAdvantage Salmon. 2016. Approved Products. May 19. Accessed June 11, 2016. http://www.hc-sc.gc.ca/fn-an/gmf-agm/appro/aquadvantage-salmon-saumon-eng.php
  • Hee, N. K., T. J. Cho, Y. B. Kim, B. I. Park, H. S. Kim, and M. S. Rhee. 2014. “Implications for Effective Food Risk Communication Following the Fukushima Nuclear Accident Based on a Consumer Survey.” Food Control 50: 304–312. doi:10.1016/j.foodcont.2014.09.008.
  • Highfield, Roger. 2004. “Scottish Farmed Salmon ‘is Full of Cancer Toxins’.” The Telegraph, January 9. Salmon Farmed in Scotland is among the Most Tainted with Cancer-causing Chemicals, US Scientists have Warned. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/science-news/3317547/Scottish-farmed-salmon-is-full-of-cancer-toxins.html
  • Hoekstra, Jeljer, Heidi P. Fransen, Jan C. H. van Eijkeren, Janneke Verkaik-Kloosterman, Nynke de Jong, Helen Owen, Marc Kennedy, Hans Verhagen, and Andy Hart. 2013a. “Benefit–risk Assessment of Plant Sterols in Margarine: A QALIBRA Case Study.” Food and Chemical Toxicology 54: 35–42.10.1016/j.fct.2012.08.054
  • Hoekstra, Jeljer, Andy Hart, Helen Owen, Marco Zeilmaker, Bas Bokkers, Björn Thorgilsson, and Helga Gunnlaugsdottir. 2013b. “Fish, Contaminants and Human Health: Quantifying and Weighing Benefits and Risks.” Food and Chemical Toxicology 54: 18–29.10.1016/j.fct.2012.01.013
  • Hooper, L. 2006. “Risks and Benefits of Omega 3 Fats for Mortality, Cardiovascular Disease, and Cancer: Systematic Review.” BMJ 332 (7544): 752–760. doi:10.1136/bmj.38755.366331.2F.
  • Houdebine, Louis-Marie. 2009. “Production of Pharmaceutical Proteins by Transgenic Animals.” Comparative Immunology, Microbiology and Infectious Diseases 32 (2): 107–121. doi:10.1016/j.cimid.2007.11.005.
  • Houdebine, Louis-Marie. 2011. “Production of Human Polyclonal Antibodies by Transgenic Animals.” Advances in Bioscience and Biotechnology 2: 138–141.10.4236/abb.2011.23022
  • IAEA Annual Report 2012. Forthcoming. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://www.iaea.org/sites/default/files/publications/reports/2012/anrep2012_full.pdf
  • Kahneman, Daniel, Paul Slovic, and Amos Tversky, eds. 1982. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases: Heuristics and Biases. Cambridge University Press. http://www.lob.de/cgi-bin/work/suche2?titnr=1199441&flag=citavi.10.1017/CBO9780511809477
  • Kaiser, Matthias, Kate Millar, Erik Thorstensen, and Sandy Tomkins. 2007. “Developing the Ethical Matrix as a Decision Support Framework: GM Fish as a Case Study.” Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 20 (1): 65–80. doi:10.1007/s10806-006-9023-8.
  • Kanda, Reiko, Satsuki Tsuji, and Hidenori Yonehara. 2012. “Perceived Risk of Nuclear Power and Other Risks During the Last 25 Years in Japan.” Health Physics 102 (4): 384–390. doi:10.1097/HP.0b013e31823abef2.
  • Kaptan, Gülbanu, and Baruch Fischhoff. 2010. “Sticky Decisions: Peanut Butter in a Time of Salmonella.” Emerging Infectious Diseases 16 (5): 900–904. doi:10.3201/eid1605.AD1605.
  • Kaptan, Gülbanu, and Baruch Fischhoff. 2011. “Diagnosing Food-borne Illness: A Behavioral Analysis of Barriers to Testing.” Journal of Public Health Policy. 32 (1): 60–72. doi:10.3390/ijerph10083684.
  • Kato, Takaaki, Shogo Takahara, Masashi Nishikawa, and Toshimitsu Homma. 2013. “A Case Study of Economic Incentives and Local Citizens’ Attitudes Toward Hosting a Nuclear Power Plant in Japan: Impacts of the Fukushima Accident.” Energy Policy 59: 808–818.10.1016/j.enpol.2013.04.043
  • van Kleef, Ellen, Øydis Ueland, Gregory Theodoridis, Gene Rowe, Uwe Pfenning, Julie Houghton, Heleen van Dijk, George Chryssochoidis, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2009. “Food Risk Management Quality: Consumer Evaluations of Past and Emerging Food Safety Incidents.” Health, Risk & Society 11 (2): 137–163. doi:10.1080/13698570902784265.
  • Kris-Etherton, Penny M., William S. Harris, and Lawrence J. Appel. 2003. “Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Cardiovascular Disease: New Recommendations From the American Heart Association.” Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology 23 (2): 151–152. doi:10.1161/01.ATV.0000057393.97337.AE.
  • Laible, G. 2009. “Enhancing Livestock Through Genetic Engineering – Recent Advances and Future Prospects.” Comparative Immunology, Microbiology and Infectious Diseases 32 (2): 123–137. doi:10.1016/j.cimid.2007.11.012.
  • Lando, A. M., S. B. Fein, and C. J. Choinière. 2012. “Awareness of Methylmercury in Fish and Fish Consumption Among Pregnant and Postpartum Women and Women of Childbearing Age in the United States.” Environmental Research 116: 85–92. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2012.04.002.
  • Le Curieux-Belfond, Olivier, Louise Vandelac, Joseph Caron, and Gilles-Éric Séralini. 2009. “Factors to Consider Before Production and Commercialization of Aquatic Genetically Modified Organisms: The Case of Transgenic Salmon.” Environmental Science & Policy 12 (2): 170–189. doi:10.1016/j.envsci.2008.10.001.
  • Llorente-Mirandes, Toni, Josep Calderón, Francesc Centrich, Roser Rubio, and José F. López-Sánchez. 2014. “A Need for Determination of Arsenic Species at Low Levels in Cereal-based Food and Infant Cereals. Validation of a Method by IC–ICPMS.” Food Chemistry 147: 377–385.10.1016/j.foodchem.2013.09.138
  • Ma, Yong Chao, and Min Fang. 2013. “Immunosenescence and Age-related Viral Diseases.” Science China Life Sciences 56 (5): 399–405. doi:10.1007/s11427-013-4478-0.
  • Mather, Damien, Rasmus Vikan, and John Knight. 2016. “Marketplace Response to GM Animal Products.” Nature Biotechnology 34 (3): 236–238. doi:10.1038/nbt.3494.
  • Maxmen, Amy. 2012. “Transgenic Fish Swims up Regulatory Stream.” Nature News, December 22. Accessed January 12, 2015. 10.1038/nature.2012.12130.
  • Mazzocchi, M. 2006. “No News Is Good News: Stochastic Parameters versus Media Coverage Indices in Demand Models after Food Scares.” American Journal of Agricultural Economics 88 (3): 727–741. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8276.2006.00891.x.
  • McCombe, Pamela A., Judith M. Greer, and I. R. Mackay. 2009. “Sexual Dimorphism in Autoimmune Disease.” Current Molecular Medicine 9 (9): 1058–1079. doi:10.2174/156652409789839116.
  • McCullough, M. P., T. L. Marsh, and R. Huffaker. 2013. “Reconstructing Market Reactions to Consumption Harms.” Applied Economics Letters 20 (2): 173–179. doi:10.1080/13504851.2012.687091.
  • Menozzi, Davide, Cristina Mora, and A. Merigo. 2012. “Genetically Modified Salmon for Dinner? Transgenic Salmon Marketing Scenarios.” AgBioForum 15 (3): 276–293.
  • Miles, S., and V. Scaife. 2003. “Optimistic Bias and Food.” Nutrition Research Reviews 16 (1): 3–19.10.1079/NRR200249
  • Mora, Cristina, Davide Menozzi, Gijs A. Kleter, Lusine H. Aramyan, Natasha I. Valeeva, Karin I. Zimmermann, and Pakki R. Giddalury. 2012. “Factors Affecting the Adoption of Genetically Modified Animals in the Food and Pharmaceutical Chains.” Bio-based and Applied Economics 1 (3): 313–329.
  • Moreno-Jimenez, E., E. Esteban, and J. M. Penalosa. 2012. “The Fate of Arsenic in Soil-plant Systems.” In Reviews of Environmental Contamination and Toxicology, edited by David M. Whitacre, Vol. 215, 1–37. New York: Springer.
  • Morgan, M. G., Baruch Fischhoff, A. Bostrom, and C. Atman. 2002. Risk Communication: A Mental Models Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Nakamura, Rika, Rie Satoh, Yukari Nakajima, Nana Kawasaki, Teruhide Yamaguchi, Jun-ichi Sawada, Hiroyuki Nagoya, and Reiko Teshima. 2009. “Comparative Study of GH-transgenic and Non-transgenic Amago Salmon (Oncorhynchus masou ishikawae) Allergenicity and Proteomic Analysis of Amago Salmon Allergens.” Regulatory Toxicology and Pharmacology 55 (3): 300–308. doi:10.1016/j.yrtph.2009.08.002.
  • Nauta, Maarten J., Arnout R. H. Fischer, Esther D. van Asselt, Aarieke E. I. de Jong, Lynn J. Frewer, and Rob de de Jonge. 2008. “Food Safety in the Domestic Environment: The Effect of Consumer Risk Information on Human Disease Risks.” Risk Analysis 28 (1): 179–192. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01012.x.
  • Nesheim, Malden C., and Ann L. Yaktine. 2007. Seafood Choices: Balancing Benefits and Risks. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
  • Ng, Kwan-Hoong, and Mei-Li Lean. 2012. “The Fukushima Nuclear Crisis Reemphasizes the Need for Improved Risk Communication and Better Use of Social Media.” Health Physics 103 (3): 307–310. doi:10.1097/HP.0b013e318257cfcb.
  • Niemann, Heiner, and Wilfried A. Kues. 2007. “Transgenic Farm Animals: An Update.” Reproduction, Fertility, and Development 19 (6): 762. doi:10.1071/RD07040.
  • Norat, T., S. Bingham, P. Ferrari, N. Slimani, M. Jenab, M. Mazuir, K. Overvad, et al. 2005. “Meat, Fish, and Colorectal Cancer Risk: The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition.” JNCI Journal of the National Cancer Institute 97 (12): 906–916. doi:10.1093/jnci/dji164.
  • OECD – FAO Agricultural Outlook 2013–2022: Highlights. Forthcoming. Accessed January 13, 2015. http://www.oecd.org/site/oecd-faoagriculturaloutlook/highlights-2013-EN.pdf
  • Oniki, Shunji. 2006. “Valuing Food-borne Risks Using Time-Series Data: The Case of E. coli O157:H7 and BSE Crises in Japan.” Agribusiness 22 (2): 219–232. doi:10.1002/agr.20081.
  • Pauwels, E. 2013. “Public Understanding of Synthetic Biology.” BioScience 63 (2): 79–89.
  • Pidgeon, Nick F., Roger E. Kasperson, and Paul Slovic, eds. 2003. The Social Amplification of Risk. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Pieniak, Zuzanna, Wim Verbeke, Joachim Scholderer, Karen Brunsø, and Svein Ottar Olsen. 2008. “Impact of Consumers’ Health Beliefs, Health Involvement and Risk Perception on Fish Consumption.” British Food Journal 110 (9): 898–915. doi:10.1108/00070700810900602.
  • Poortinga, Wouter, Midori Aoyagi, and Nick F. Pidgeon. 2013. “Public Perceptions of Climate Change and Energy Futures Before and After the Fukushima Accident: A Comparison Between Britain and Japan.” Energy Policy 62: 1204–1211.10.1016/j.enpol.2013.08.015
  • Poudelet, Éric. 2012. “2011 E. Coli O104: H4 Outbreak in Germany and in France: Lessons Learnt.” Bulletin de l Academie Veterinaire de France 165 (1): 347–354.10.4267/2042/48818
  • Prather, R. S., M. Shen, and Y. Dai. 2008. “Genetically Modified Pigs for Medicine and Agriculture.” Biotechnology and Genetic Engineering Reviews 25 (1): 245–265. doi:10.5661/bger-25-245.
  • “Press Release on E. coli Outbreak: EU Withdraws Egyptian Seeds from the Market and Temporarily Bans their Import.” 2011. News Release. July 5. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://europa.eu/rapid/press-release_IP-11-831_en.htm
  • Raley, M. E., M. Ragona, S. J. Sijtsema, Arnout R. H. Fischer, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2016. “Barriers to Using Consumer Science Information in Food Technology Innovations: An Exploratory Study using Delphi Methodology.” International Journal of Food Studies 5 (1): 39–53.
  • Redmond, Elizabeth C., and Christopher J. Griffith. 2004. “Consumer Perceptions of Food Safety Risk, Control and Responsibility.” Appetite 43 (3): 309–313. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2004.05.003.
  • Renn, O. 1990. “Public Responses to the Chernobyl Accident.” Journal of Environmental Psychology 10: 151–167.10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80125-2
  • “Report of the Joint FAO/WHO Expert Consultation on the Risks and Benefits of Fish Consumption.” Forthcoming. FAO Fisheries and Aquaculture Report 978. Accessed January 12, 2015. http://www.fao.org/docrep/014/ba0136e/ba0136e00.pdf
  • Rosati, Simona, and Anna Saba. 2004. “The Perception of Risks Associated with Food-related Hazards and the Perceived Reliability of Sources of Information.” International Journal of Food Science & Technology 39 (5): 491–500. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2621.2004.00808.x.
  • Rozin, Paul, Mark Spranca, Zeev Krieger, Ruth Neuhaus, Darlene Surillo, Amy Swerdlin, and Katherine Wood. 2004. “Preference for Natural: Instrumental and Ideational/Moral Motivations, and the Contrast Between Foods and Medicines.” Appetite 43 (2): 147–154. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2004.03.005.
  • Ruxton, C. H. S., S. C. Reed, M. J. A. Simpson, and K. J. Millington. 2004. “The Health Benefits of omega-3 Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids: A Review of the Evidence.” Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 17 (5): 449–459. doi:10.1111/j.1365-277X.2004.00552.x.
  • Saba, A., and F. Messina. 2003. “Attitudes Towards Organic Foods and Risk/Benefit Perception Associated with Pesticides.” Food Quality and Preference 14: 637–645.10.1016/S0950-3293(02)00188-X
  • “SACN Advice on Fish Consumption: Benefits and Risks.” 2004. SACN: Reports and Position Statements. Unpublished manuscript. Accessed January 14, 2015. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/sacn-advice-on-fish-consumption
  • Sailor, W. C., D. Bodansky, C. Braun, S. Fetter, and B. van der Zwaan. 2000. “Nuclear Power: A Nuclear Solution to Climate Change?” Science 288: 1177–1178.10.1126/science.288.5469.1177
  • Sawada, M., H. Aizaki, and K. Sato. 2014. “Japanese Consumers’ Valuation of Domestic Beef After the Fukushima Daiichi Nuclear Power Plant accident.” Appetite 80 (1): 225–235. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.018.
  • Schlag, Anne K., and Kaja Ystgaard. 2013. “Europeans and Aquaculture: Perceived Differences Between Wild and Farmed Fish.” British Food Journal 115 (2): 209–222. doi:10.1108/00070701311302195.
  • Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition. 2004. “Advice on Fish Consumption: Benefits and Risks.” Accessed on January 13, 2015. https://www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/338801/SACN_Advice_on_Fish_Consumption.pdf
  • Shimshack, J. P., M. B. Ward, and T. K. M. Beatty. 2007. “Mercury Advisories: Information, Education, and Fish Consumption.” Journal of Environmental Economics and Management 53: 158–179. doi:10.1016/j.jeem.2006.10.002.
  • Siegrist, M. 2008. “Factors Influencing Public Acceptance of Innovative Food Technologies and Products.” Trends in Food Science & Technology 19 (11): 603–608. doi:10.1016/j.tifs.2008.01.017.
  • Sirot, Véronique, Jean-Charles Leblanc, and Irène Margaritis. 2012. “A Risk–Benefit Analysis Approach to Seafood Intake to Determine Optimal Consumption.” British Journal of Nutrition 107 (12): 1812–1822. doi:10.1017/S0007114511005010.
  • Slovic, Paul. 2000. The Perception of Risk. Risk, Society and Policy Series. London: Earthscan.
  • Slovic, Paul. 2012. “The Perception Gap: Radiation and Risk.” Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists 68 (3): 67–75.10.1177/0096340212444870
  • Smith, Allan H., and Craig M. Steinmaus. 2009. “Health Effects of Arsenic and Chromium in Drinking Water: Recent Human Findings.” Annual Review of Public Health 30 (1): 107–122. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.031308.100143.
  • Sovacool, B. K., and C. Cooper. 2008. “Nuclear Nonsense: Why Nuclear Power is No Answer to Climate Change and the World’s Post-Kyoto Energy Challenges.” William & Mary Environmental Law. & Policy Reviews 33 (1). Accessed January 12, 2015. http://scholarship.law.wm.edu/wmelpr/vol33/iss1/2
  • Spirichev, V. B., G. V. Donchenko, N. V. Blazheevich, IuM Parkhomenko, S. I. Aleĭnik, N. A. Golubkina, O. A. Vrzhesinskaia, et al. 2006. “To the 20th Anniversary of the Chornobyl Accident Study of Vitamin Status and Provision With Micro- and Macroelements of Limited Groups of People at Different Time Periods Since the Accident at Chornobyl Nuclear Power Plant.” Ukrainskii Biokhimicheskii Zhurnal 78 (2): 5–26.
  • SteelFisher, G., R. Blendon, J. Hero, and E. Ben-Porath. 2013. “Adoption of Self-protective Behaviors in Response to a Foodborne Illness Outbreak: Perspectives of Older Adults.” Journal of Food Safety 33: 149–162. doi:10.1111/jfs.12035.
  • Tariq, Luqman, Juanita Haagsma, and Arie Havelaar. 2011. “Cost of Illness and Disease Burden in The Netherlands Due to Infections with Shiga Toxin-producing Escherichia coli O157.” Journal of Food Protection 74 (4): 545–552. doi:10.4315/0362-028X.JFP-10-252.
  • Teisl, M. F., E. Fromberg, A. E. Smith, K. J. Boyle, and H. M. Engelberth. 2011. “Awake at the Switch: Improving Fish Consumption Advisories for at-risk Women.” Science of the Total Environment 409: 3257–3266. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2011.05.006.
  • Thompson, K. M. 2002. “Variability and Uncertainty Meet Risk Management and Risk Communication.” Risk Analysis 22 (3): 647–654.10.1111/risk.2002.22.issue-3
  • Torgersen, Helge. 2009. “Synthetic Biology in Society: Learning from Past Experience?” Systems and Synthetic Biology 3 (1–4): 9–17. doi:10.1007/s11693-009-9030-y.
  • Torgersen, Helge, and Markus Schmidt. 2013. “Frames and Comparators: How Might a Debate on Synthetic Biology Evolve?” Futures 48: 44–54.10.1016/j.futures.2013.02.002
  • Turcanu, C., B. Carlé, F. Hardeman, G. Bombaerts, and K. Van Aeken. 2007. “Food Safety and Acceptance of Management Options after Radiological Contaminations of the Food Chain.” Food Quality and Preference 18 (8): 1085–1095. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.05.005.
  • US Department of Energy. 2005. “Energy Policy Act of 2005.” Accessed on January 13, 2015. http://energy.gov/sites/prod/files/2013/10/f3/epact_2005.pdf
  • Van Dijk, Heleen, Arnout R. H. Fischer, J. de Jonge, Gene Rowe, and Lynn J. Frewer. 2012. “The Impact of Balanced Risk-benefit Information and Prior Attitudes on Post-information Attitudes.” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 42: 1958–1983.10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012.00926.x
  • Vàzquez-Salat, Núria, Brian Salter, Greet Smets, and Louis-Marie Houdebine. 2012. “The Current State of GMO Governance: Are we Ready for GM Animals?” Biotechnology Advances 30 (6): 1336–1343. doi:10.1016/j.biotechadv.2012.02.006.
  • Verbeke, Wim, Lynn J. Frewer, Joachim Scholderer, and Hubert F. De Brabander. 2007. “Why Consumers Behave as They Do With Respect to Food Safety and Risk Information.” Analytica Chimica Acta 586 (1–2): 2–7. doi:10.1016/j.aca.2006.07.065.
  • Verbeke, Wim, Filiep Vanhonacker, Lynn J. Frewer, Isabelle Sioen, Stefaan de Henauw, and John van Camp. 2008. “Communicating Risks and Benefits from Fish Consumption: Impact on Belgian Consumers’ Perception and Intention to Eat Fish.” Risk Analysis 28: 951–967.
  • van der Voet, Hilko, Anika de Mul, and Jacob D. van Klaveren. 2007. “A Probabilistic Model for Simultaneous Exposure to Multiple Compounds from Food and Its Use for Risk–benefit Assessment.” Food and Chemical Toxicology 45 (8): 1496–1506. doi:10.1016/j.fct.2007.02.009.
  • Wada, N., L. P. Jacobson, M. Cohen, A. French, J. Phair, and A. Munoz. 2013. “Cause-specific Life Expectancies After 35 Years of Age for Human Immunodeficiency Syndrome-infected and Human Immunodeficiency Syndrome-negative Individuals Followed Simultaneously in Long-term Cohort Studies, 1984–2008.” American Journal of Epidemiology 177 (2): 116–125. doi:10.1093/aje/kws321.
  • Werber, Dirk, Gérard Krause, Christina Frank, Angelika Fruth, Antje Flieger, Martin Mielke, Lars Schaade, and Klaus Stark. 2012. “Outbreaks of Virulent Diarrheagenic Escherichia coli – Are We in Control?” BMC Medicine 10 (1): 681. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-10-11.
  • Whitfield, Stephen C., Eugene A. Rosa, Amy Dan, and Thomas Dietz. 2009. “The Future of Nuclear Power: Value Orientations and Risk Perception.” Risk Analysis 29 (3): 425–437. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01155.x.
  • WHO (World Health Organization). 2011. “Foodborne Outbreaks: Managing the Risks.” Bulletin of the World Health Organization 89: 554–555.
  • WHO (World Health Organization). 2013. “Global Report on Fukushima Nuclear Accident Details Health Risks.” News Release. February 28. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/news/releases/2013/fukushima_report_20130228/en/
  • Wilson, K., and J. S. Brownstein. 2009. “Early Detection of Disease Outbreaks Using the Internet.” Canadian Medical Association Journal 180 (8): 829–831. doi:10.1503/cmaj.1090215.
  • Wu, Felicia. 2006. “Bt Corn’s Reduction of Mycotoxins: Regulatory Decisions and Public Opinion.” Natural Resource Management and Policy 30: 179–200.10.1007/978-0-387-36953-2
  • Yan, Jun, Judith M. Greer, Renee Hull, John D. O’Sullivan, Robert D. Henderson, Stephen J. Read, and Pamela A. McCombe. 2010. “The Effect of Aging on Human Lymphocyte Subsets: Comparison of Males and Females.” Immunity & Ageing 7 (1): 4. doi:10.1186/1742-4933-7-4.