2,198
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Introducing the Shorter Dark Tetrad for Muslim Societies (SD4-MS): a study among young adults in Pakistan

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 539-549 | Received 04 Sep 2021, Accepted 10 Jan 2022, Published online: 11 Mar 2022

ABSTRACT

This paper examines the psychometric properties of the Short Dark Tetrad, presented for online administration, among a sample of 370 young adults between the ages of 18 and 26 who were born in the Punjab and who had lived there since their birth. With the omission of one item from each of the four scales (Machiavellianism, psychopathy, narcissism, and sadism) the clear four factor structure was recovered with cross-loadings remaining on four items. All four scales recorded acceptable or good levels of internal consistency reliability (alpha). This Shorter Dark Tetrad (SD4-MS) is commended for further use within predominantly Muslim societies.

Introduction

The classic paper by Paulhus and Williams (Citation2002) in Journal of Research in Personality firmly launched into psychology the notion of the dark triad of offensive yet non-pathological personalities: Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy. Each of these three constructs had already established a significant literature and had been operationalised by measures for clinical and for research purposes. The study by Paulhus and Williams (Citation2002) was important because it established grounds for asserting that these three dark personalities were distinct but overlapping constructs. The construct validity for this assertion was that in their operationalised form these three dark personalities functioned somewhat differently within the psychological space defined by the Big Five Factor model of personality. Narcissism, assessed by the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Hall, Citation1979) was positively correlated with extraversion (.42) and openness (.38) and negatively correlated with agreeableness (-.36), but independent of conscientiousness and neuroticism. Machiavellianism, assessed by the Mach-IV Inventory (Christie & Geis, Citation1970) was negatively correlated with both agreeableness (-.47) and conscientiousness (-.34), but independent of extraversion, neuroticism, and openness. Psychopathy, assessed by the Self-Report Psychopathy Scales (SRP-III; Hare, Citation1985) was positively correlated with extraversion (.34) and openness (.24), and negatively correlated with agreeableness (-.25), conscientiousness (-.24), and neuroticism (-.34).

Within a decade of the paper by Paulhus and Williams (Citation2002), the dark triad had become well established within psychological literature (for review see Furnham et al., Citation2013). Research was, however, hampered by the length of the instruments designed to assess these constructs. This practical problem led quite quickly to the development of two instruments designed to capture the essence of these three dark personalities: a 12-item instrument proposed by Jonason and Webster (Citation2010), known as “The Dirty Dozen” with four items assessing each construct; and a 27-item instrument proposed by Jones and Paulhus (Citation2014), known as the “Short Dark Triad (SD3)” with nine items assessing each construct.

The Dirty Dozen quickly attracted attention and was incorporated within a range of studies. For example, Aghababaei et al. (Citation2014) explored the connection between the dark triad and honesty-humility in prosociality, religiosity, and happiness. Egan et al. (Citation2014) explored the connection between the dark triad and happiness and subjective wellbeing. Haddad et al. (Citation2016) explored the connections between the dark triad and sociosexual orientation and religious affiliation. Ghorbani et al. (Citation2017) explored the connection between the dark triad and harmony control. Lowicki and Zajenkowski (Citation2017) explored the relationship between the dark triad and religiosity and empathy. Schimmenti et al. (Citation2019) explored the connection between the dark triad and empathy and alexithymia. The Dirty Dozen has also been translated into various languages, including, Bangla (Ahmed et al., Citation2020), Italian (Chiorri et al., Citation2019), Portugese (Pechorro et al., Citation2021), Spanish (Pineda et al., Citation2020) and Turkish (Özsoy et al., Citation2017). A number of commentators, however, have questioned the wisdom of trying to capture these three distinctive dark personalities in just four items, demonstrating that these short scales do not correspond well with longer and established measures of the three diverse constructs (Lee et al., Citation2013; Maples et al., Citation2014; Miller et al., Citation2012). According to Jonason et al. (Citation2013), the Dirty Dozen represents a trade-off of accuracy for efficiency.

Responding to the challenge that the Dirty Dozen failed to offer an adequate short form of the dark triad, Jones and Paulhus (Citation2014) reported on a project that first tried to clarify the conceptualisation of the three constructs of Machiavellianism, subclinical Narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy by sharpening the differentiation among these three constructs and then tried to operationalise these conceptualisations with sets of nine items each. Distinguishing Machiavellianism from subclinical Psychopathy, Jones and Paulhus (Citation2014) argue that Machiavellians plan ahead, build alliances, and do their best to maintain their positive reputation, while psychopaths behave impulsively, abandon friends and family, and give little heed to their reputation (Hare & Newmann, Citation2008). Machiavellians tend to be strategic rather than impulsive (Jones & Paulhus, Citation2011a), and they tend to avoid manipulating family members (Barber, Citation1998). Psychopaths tend to lie for immediate rewards, even if those lies compromise their long-term interest (Paulhus & Jones, Citation2017). Their callous manipulation, combined with recklessness and thrill-seeking, tends to be reflected in bold and relentless pursuit of self-interest (Hare & Newmann, Citation2008).

For their understanding of subclinical Narcissism, Jones and Paulhus (Citation2011b, Citation2014) drew on Kernberg (Citation1975) and Kohut (Citation1978). They argued that:

narcissistic behaviour was marked by manipulation and callousness, much like Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Intraphysically, however, narcissism was defined by a clash between a grandiose identity and underlying insecurities. (Jones & Paulhus, Citation2014, p. 29)

Narcissistic individuals are endlessly seeking ego-reinforcement (Morf & Rhodewalt, Citation2001) that can lead to self-destructive behaviours (Vazire & Funder, Citation2006). Narcissistic grandiosity promotes a sense of entitlement (Bushman et al., Citation2003) which may result in aggression if that grandiosity is threatened (Bushman & Baumeister, Citation1998; Jones & Paulhus, Citation2010). Narcissism involves self-deception: belief in their boastful claims tend to be maintained even when it can be verified that they exaggerate their competence (Paulhus & Williams, Citation2002).

Drawing on this sharpened differentiation between Machiavellianism, subclinical Narcissism, and subclinical Psychopathy, Jones and Paulhus (Citation2014) developed a pool of 41 items in which: the Machiavellianism items comprised four themes (cynicism, coalition building, planning, and reputation); the subclinical Narcissism items comprised four themes (entitlement, exhibitionism, grandiosity, and leadership); and the subclinical Psychopathy items comprised four themes (antisocial behaviour, callous affect, erratic lifestyle, and short-term manipulation). Drawing on data provided by 489 adults, Jones and Paulhus (Citation2014) employed exploratory factor analysis and exploratory structural equation modelling to identify three sets of nine items each that distinguished between the three constructs embraced by the Dark Triad.

The Short Dark Triad also quickly attracted attention and was incorporated within a range of studies. For example, various studies have explored the connection between the short dark triad and depression and life satisfaction (Bonfá-Araujo et al., Citation2021), honesty rules in romantic relationships (Muñoz & De Los Reyes, Citation2021), perceptions of stalking (Chung & Sheridan, Citation2021), problem gambling (Onyedire et al., Citation2021), social media addiction (Chung et al., Citation2019), and willingness to share (Malesza & Kalinowski, Citation2021). Like the Dirty Dozen, the Short Dark Triad has been translated into various languages, including, Chinese (Zhang et al., Citation2020), German (Malesza et al., Citation2019), Italian (Somma et al., Citation2020), Polish (Rogoza & Cieciuch, Citation2019), Spanish (Pineda et al., Citation2020), and Turkish (Özsoy et al., Citation2017).

In principle the Short Dark Triad has two advantages over the Dirty Dozen. The items have been selected on the basis of theory that proposes four themes within each construct and each theme is represented by at least one item in the relevant scale. Each construct is represented by nine items rather than four. Comparing the two measures, Maples et al. (Citation2014) concluded that the longer scales of the Short Dark Triad yield data that are more consistent with the three constructs as assessed by more established and validated measures. There is a third way in which these two measures differ. All items in the Dirty Dozen are scored in the same direction. In the Short Dark Triad five items are reverse coded, three in the scale of subclinical narcissism and two in the scale of subclinical psychopathy. The inclusion of reverse coded items was routinely commended by classic texts in psychological testing and measurement of personality traits to guard against response style bias (Anastasi, Citation1961; Edwards, Citation1970). More recently, however, employing reversed items has been identified as a questionable practice suggesting that the unidimensionality of the test may be jeopardised by secondary sources of variance (Suárez-Alvarez et al., Citation2018). Also, reverse coded items may cause a problem within some cultural contexts as evidenced by Astley et al. (Citation2012), Musharraf and Lewis (Citation2016), Francis et al. (Citation2016), and Erken and Francis (Citation2021) within Muslim societies.

From triad to tetrad

With the publication of the Short Dark Tetrad, Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) not only expanded their original family of three offensive yet non-pathological personalities, with a fourth compatriot, sadism, they also renuanced the collection of items designed to assess Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy. Each of these two developments will be explored in turn.

From among a range of potentially related constructs, including spitefulness, borderline, antagonism, moral disengagement, schadenfreude, and status-driven risk-taking, Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) argued that they invited sadism into their dark tetrad on the grounds that this construct met two conditions. First, sadism shared with Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, and subclinical psychopathy the shared component of callousness or impaired empathy. Second, sadism added a unique element not shared with the three members of the dark triad, namely intrinsic pleasure in hurting others (Nell, Citation2006) or enjoyment in other people’s suffering (Paulhus & Dutton, Citation2016).

In developing their distinctive measure of sadism for incorporation within the dark triad, Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) began by reviewing existing measures of the construct, including the Short Sadistic Impulse Scale (O’Meara et al., Citation2011), the Varieties of Sadistic Tendences (Paulhus & Jones, Citation2015), and the Assessment of Sadistic Personality (Plouffe et al., Citation2017), Then they refined, sharpened, and shaped the construct of sadism within the environment of the family of the four dark personalities by emphasising the unique contribution of each construct and by minimising the overlap among the constructs.

It was this challenge to minimise the overlap among the constructs, read in light of some of the critique of the Short Dark Triad (Furnham et al., Citation2014; Zeigler-Hill & Marcus, Citation2019), that also caused Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) to renuance their conceptualisation of the other three dark constructs, especially Machiavellianism. Another crucial change that occurred as part of this process was the elimination of all reverse coded items.

Part of the strategy employed by Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) to demonstrate the construct validity of the relative independence of the four measures was to examine their individual relation with each of the big five factors of personality as proposed by Costa and McCrae (Citation1992). They reported that extraversion was positively correlated with narcissism and with psychopathy; agreeableness was negatively correlated with Machiavellianism, psychopathy and sadism; conscientiousness was negatively correlated with Machiavellianism, psychopathy, and sadism; neuroticism was negatively correlated with narcissism; and openness was positively correlated with narcissism.

Furnham and Horne (Citation2021) confirmed the factor structure of the Short Dark Triad among a sample of 502 participants in the United States and reported the following alpha coefficients: Machievellianism, α = .65; Narcissism, α = .81; Psychopathy, α = .79; Sadism, α = .79. They reported significantly higher scores for males on all four measures. The construct validity of the German translation of the Short Dark Tetrad was tested by Blötner et al. (Citation2021) alongside the big five factors of personality, honesty-humility, maladaptive personality traits, impulsivity, aggression, motives, values, and sociosexual orientation.

Research problems

Against this background the aim of the present study was to explore the performance of the Short Dark Tetrad within a predominantly Muslim society. Given the elimination of reverse coded items from this recently developed instrument it was hypothesised that the four factor structure would be recovered, although it was also recognised that not all of the 28 items may transfer comfortably within this specific social context.

Method

Procedure

The Short Dark Tetrad was included within the online survey Parental Attachment and Life designed for completion by young adults between the ages of 18 and 26 who were born in Punjab and had lived there since their birth. Participants were assured of confidentiality. The project was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Advanced Studies Research Board GC University Lahore.

Instrument

The 28-item Short Dark Tetrad (Paulhus et al., Citation2021) proposes four seven-item measures of Machiavellianism, subclinical Narcissism, subclinical Psychopathy, and Sadism (see for the items). Participants were invited to rate their “degree of agreement with each item on a five-point scale”: strongly disagree (1), disagree (2), neutral (3), agree (4), and strongly agree (5). There are no reverse coded items in this instrument.

Table 1. The Shorter Dark Tetrad: Factor structure.

Participants

The Parental Attachment and Life Survey was fully completed by 370 participants who met the profile of young adults between the ages of 18 and 26 who were born in Punjab and had lived there since their birth. The participants comprised 151 males, 217 females, and 2 who preferred not to say: 45 were aged 18 or 19, 131 were aged 20 or 21, 116 were aged 22 or 23, 65 were aged 24, 25, or 26, and 13 preferred not to say.

Analysis

The data were analysed by SPSS using the frequency, correlation, factor, and reliability routines.

Results and discussion

The first step in data analysis employed confirmatory factor analysis to explore the structure of the 28 items proposed by the Short Dark Tetrad. The varimax rotated solution from principle components analysis, constrained to four factors, produced a fairly clean solution after the removal of one item from each of the four hypothesised factors. The causalities in this analysis were the following four items:

  • I know how to hurt someone with words alone (sadism)

  • I like to show off every now and then (narcissism)

  • People who mess with me always regret it (psychopathy)

  • Flattery is a good way to get people on your side (Machiavellianism)

With these four items eliminated, demonstrates strong cross-loadings on just four items. The item “I love it when a tricky plan succeeds’ from the Machiavellianism Scale cross-loaded on narcissism (.32). The item “Watching a fist-fight excites me” from the sadism scale cross-loaded with psychopathy (.30). Two items from the psychopathy scale cross-loaded on sadism: “I’ve been in more fights than most people of my age and gender” (.34), and “I’ve been in trouble with the law” (.39).

The second step in data analysis employed the alpha coefficient (Cronbach, Citation1951) and the correlations between individual items and the sum of the other five items within each of the proposed scales to explore the internal consistency reliability of these measures of four dark personalities. The alpha coefficients presented in demonstrate good reliability characteristics for the measures of sadism (.81), subclinical narcissism (.81), and subclinical psychopathy (.79), but less strong reliability for the measure of Machiavellianism (.66). These differences are also reflected by the correlations between individual items and the sum of the other five items, presented in . These correlations range between .48 and .65 for sadism, between .54 and .61 for narcissism, and between .47 and .58 for psychopathy, but drop to between .33 and .43 for Machiavellianism. The poorer performance of Machiavellianism was also reported by Paulhus et al. (Citation2021) in their foundation paper: Machiavellianism (.78), compared with sadism (.82), psychopathy (.82), and narcissism (.83), although in this case the difference was not so pronounced.

Table 2. The Shorter Dark Tetrad: Item correlations.

The third step in data analysis compared the main scale scores recorded on each of the four dark personalities by males and by females. The data presented in demonstrates that males recorded significantly higher scores than females on sadism and on psychopathy, although there were no significant sex differences on narcissism or Machiavellianism. In their exploration of sex differences on the Short Dark Tetrad, Neumann et al. (Citation2021) reported significantly higher scores by males on all four scales, but with the strongest sex differences in respect of sadism and psychopathy.

Table 3. The Shorter Dark Tetrad: Mean scale scores by sex.

The fourth step in data analysis explored the bivariate correlations between Machiavellianism, subclinical narcissism, subclinical psychopathy, and sadism. The data presented in indicate that the strongest association is between sadism and subclinical psychopathy. This is consistent with the findings reported by Paulhus et al. (Citation2021).

Table 4. Correlation matrix.

Conclusion

This study set out to explore the performance of the Short Dark Tetrad within a predominantly Muslim society, against the background of two hypotheses. The first hypothesis suggested that elimination of the negative items formerly present in the Short Dark Triad would make this instrument a successful transfer for use within Muslim societies. The second hypothesis suggested that, nonetheless, there may be dark items within this newer instrument that could be detrimental to a successful transfer within Muslim societies. These two hypotheses were tested first by confirmatory factor analysis and then by reliability analyses.

The data indicated that, with the removal of one item from each of the four scales of dark personalities, the four-factor structure was recovered with some cross-loadings remaining on four items. The alpha coefficients for three of the scales (subclinical narcissism, subclinical psychopathy, and sadism) were good (in excess of .80). The alpha coefficient for Machiavellianism was, however, less strong, but acceptable for a six-item scale (α = .66). On these grounds the Shorter Dark Tetrad for Muslim Societies (SD4-MS) can be commended for further use.

As an initial study, there are clear limitations imposed on the generalisability of the findings, restricted to a sample of young adults in Punjab. The findings, however, are of sufficient worth to command replication and extension studies within other age groups within Punjab, and within other predominantly Muslim societies.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Aghababaei, N., Mohammadtabar, S., & Saffarinia, M. (2014). Dirty Dozen vs. the H factor: Comparison of the dark triad and honesty-humility in prosociality, religiosity, and happiness. Personality and Individual Differences, 67(1), 6–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.03.026
  • Ahmed, O., Naher, L., Islam, R., Akter, M., & Deb, S. (2020). Psychometric analyses of the Bangla version of the dark triad Dirty Dozen. Heliyon, 6(11), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e05341
  • Anastasi, A. (1961). Psychological testing. Macmillan.
  • Astley, J., Francis, L. J., & Robbins, M. (2012). Assessing attitude towards religion: The Astley-Francis Scale of Attitude Toward Theistic Belief. British Journal of Religious Education, 34(2), 183–193. https://doi.org/10.1080/01416200.2011.614735
  • Barber, N. (1998). Sex differences in disposition towards kin, security of adult attachment, and sociosexuality as a function of parental divorce. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 19(2), 125–132. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00004-X
  • Blötner, C., Ziegler, M., Wehner, C., Back, M. D., & Grosz, M. P. (2021). The nomological network of the Short Dark Tetrad Scale (SD4). European Journal of Psychological Assessment, Online First June, 2021, https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000655
  • Bonfá-Araujo, B., Lima-Costa, A. R., Baptista, M. N., & Hauck-Filho, N. (2021). Depressed or satisfied? The relationship between the dark triad traits, depression, and life satisfaction. Current Psychology, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01726-3
  • Bushman, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: Does self-love or self-hate lead to violence? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 219–229. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.1.219
  • Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., van Dijk, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Narcissism, sexual refusal, and aggression: Testing a narcissistic reactance model of sexual coercion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(5), 1027–1040. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1027
  • Chiorri, C., Garofalo, C., & Velotti, P. (2019). Does the dark triad manifest similarly in men and women? Measurement invariance of the Dirty Dozen across sex. Current Psychology, 38(3), 659–675. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9641-5
  • Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-174450-2.50006-3
  • Chung, K. L., Morshidi, I., Yoong, L. C., & Thian, K. N. (2019). The role of the dark tetrad and impulsivity in social media addiction: Findings from Malaysia. Personality and Individual Differences, 143, 62–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.016
  • Chung, K. L., & Sheridan, L. (2021). Perceptions of stalking in Malaysia and England: The influence of perpetrator-target prior relationship and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 182, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111064
  • Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, 16(3), 297–334. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02310555
  • Edwards, A. L. (1970). The measurement of personality traits by scales and inventories. Holt Rinehart.
  • Egan, V., Chan, S., & Shorter, G. W. (2014). The dark triad, happiness and subjective wellbeing. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 17–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.004
  • Erken, H. G., & Francis, L. J. (2021). Internal consistency reliability and construct validity of the Astley-Francis Scale of Attitude Toward Theistic Faith revised among Muslim secondary school students in England, alongside Christian and religiously unaffiliated students. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 24(3), 261–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2020.1851667
  • Francis, L. J., Tekke, M., & Robbins, M. (2016). The psychometric properties of the Sahin-Francis Scale of Attitude Toward Islam revised among Sunni students in Malaysia. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 19(5), 433–439. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2016.1193480
  • Furnham, A., & Horne, G. (2021). The tetradic heart of darkness: Comparing three dark-side instruments. Personality and Individual Differences, 179, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110918
  • Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The dark triad of personality: A 10-year review. Social and Personality Compass, 7(3), 199–216. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12018
  • Furnham, A., Richards, S., Rangel, L., & Jones, D. N. (2014). Measuring malevolence: Quantitative issues surrounding the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 114–121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.02.001
  • Ghorbani, N., Watson, P. J., Zarei, A., & Chen, Z. (2017). Muslim attitudes and spirituality: Relationships with dark triad and harmony control in Iranian teachers. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 20(1), 20–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2017.1320367
  • Haddad, B., Angman, M., Archer, T., & Garcia, D. (2016). Dark triad, sociosexual orientation and religious affiliation: An association and moderation study. Clinical and Experimental Psychology, 2(2), 1–5. https://doi.org/10.4172/2471-2701.1000124
  • Hare, R. D. (1985). Comparison of procedures for the assessment of psychopathy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53(1), 7–16. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.53.1.7
  • Hare, R. D., & Newmann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4(4), 217–241. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452
  • Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., & Czarna, A. Z. (2013). Quick and dirty: Some psychosocial costs associated with the dark triad in three countries. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(1), 172–185. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100116
  • Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The Dirty Dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 420–432. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019265
  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2010). Different provocations trigger aggression in narcissists and psychopaths. Social and Personality Psychology Science, 1(1), 12–18. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550609347591
  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011a). The role of impulsivity in the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(5), 670–682. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011
  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011b). Differentiating the dark triad within the interpersonal circumplex. In L. M. Horowitz & S. Strack (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (pp. 249-268). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118001868.ch15
  • Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28–41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105
  • Kernberg, O. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Jason Aronson.
  • Kohut, H. (1978). Discussion of ‘The function of the analyst in the therapeutic process’ by Samuel D. Lipton. In P. H. Ornstein (Ed.), The search for the self: Selected writings of Heinz Kohut: 1950–1978 (Vol. 1, pp. 159-166). International Universities Press.
  • Lee, K., Ashton, M. C., Wiltshire, J., Bourdage, J. S., Visser, B. A., & Gallucci, A. (2013). Sex, power, and money: Prediction from the dark triad and honesty-humility. European Journal of Personality, 27(2), 169–184. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1860
  • Lowicki, P., & Zajenkowski, M. (2017). No empathy for people nor for god: The relationships between the dark triad, religiosity and empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 115, 169–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.02.012
  • Malesza, M., & Kalinowski, K. (2021). Willingness to share, impulsivity and the dark triad traits. Current Psychology, 40(8), 3888–3896. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00351-5
  • Malesza, M., Ostaszewski, P., Büchner, S., & Kaczmarek, M. C. (2019). The adaptation of the Short Dark Triad personality measure: Psychometric properties of a German sample. Current Psychology, 38(3), 855–864. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9662-0
  • Maples, J. L., Lamkin, J., & Miller, J. D. (2014). A test of two brief measures of the dark triad: The Dirty Dozen and Short Dark Triad. Psychological Assessment, 26(1), 326–331. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035084
  • Miller, J. D., Few, L. R., Seibert, L. A., Watts, A., Zeichner, A., & Lynam, D. R. (2012). An examination of the Dirty Dozen measure: A cautionary tale about the costs of brief measures. Psychological Assessment, 24(4), 1048–1053. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028583
  • Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unravelling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12(4), 177–196. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1204_1
  • Muñoz, M. E., & De Los Reyes, S. (2021). The dark triad and honesty rules in romantic relationships. Current Psychology, 1–3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02171-y
  • Musharraf, S., & Lewis, C. A. (2016). Urdu translation of the Sahin-Francis Scale of Attitude Toward Islam revised: A response and update to Francis, Tekke, and Robbins (2016). Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 19(5), 459–463. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2016.1198754
  • Nell, V. (2006). Cruelty’s rewards: The gratifications of perpetrators and spectators. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(3), 211–224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009058
  • Neumann, C. S., Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2021). Examining the Short Dark Tetrad (SD4) across models, correlates, and gender. Assessment, 1–17. Online First, https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120986624
  • O’Meara, A., Davies, J., & Hammond, S. (2011). The psychometric properties and utility of the Short Sadistic Impulse Scale (SSIS). Psychological Assessment, 23(2), 523–531. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022400
  • Onyedire, N. G., Chukwuorji, J. C., Orjiakor, T. C., Onu, D. U., Aneke, C. I., & Ifeagwazi, C. M. (2021). Associations of dark triad traits and problem gambling: Moderating role of age among university students. Current Psychology, 40(5), 2083–2094. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0093-3
  • Özsoy, E., Rauthmann, J. F., Jonason, P. K., & Ardıç, K. (2017). Reliability and validity of the Turkish versions of Dark Triad Dirty Dozen (DTDD-T), Short Dark Triad (SD3-T), and single item Narcissism Scale (SINS-T). Personality and Individual Differences, 117, 11–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.05.019
  • Paulhus, D. L., Buckels, E. E., Trapnell, P. D., & Jones, D. N. (2021). Screening for dark personalities. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 37(3), 208–222. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000602
  • Paulhus, D. L., & Dutton, D. G. (2016). Everyday sadism. In V. Zeigler-Hill & D. Marcus (Eds.), Dark side of personality (pp. 109-120). APA. https://doi.org/10.1037/14854-006
  • Paulhus, D. L., & Jones, D. N. (2015). Measures of dark personalities. In G. J. Boyle, D. H. Saklofske, & G. Matthews (Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological constructs (pp. 562-594). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6
  • Paulhus, D. L., & Jones, D. N. (2017). Duplicity among the dark triad: Three faces of deceit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(2), 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000139
  • Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556–563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6
  • Pechorro, P., Jonason, P. K., Raposo, V., & Maroco, J. (2021). Dirty Dozen: A concise measure of dark triad traits among at-risk youths. Current Psychology, 40(7), 3522–3531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00288-9
  • Pineda, D., Sandín, B., & Muris, P. (2020). Psychometrics properties of the Spanish version of two dark triad scales: The Dirty Dozen and the Short Dark Triad. Current Psychology, 39(5), 1873–1881. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9888-5
  • Plouffe, R. A., Saklofske, D. H., & Smith, M. M. (2017). The assessment of sadistic personality: Preliminary psychometric evidence for a new measure. Personality and Individual Differences, 104, 166–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.043
  • Raskin, R., & Hall, C. S. (1979). A Narcissistic Personality Inventory. Psychological Reports, 45(2), 590. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1979.45.2.590
  • Rogoza, R., & Cieciuch, J. (2019). Structural investigation of the Short Dark Triad questionnaire in Polish population. Current Psychology, 38(3), 756–763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9653-1
  • Schimmenti, A., Jonason, P. K., Passanisi, A., La Marca, L., Di Dio, N., & Gervasi, A. M. (2019). Exploring the dark side of personality: Emotional awareness, empathy, and the dark triad traits in an Italian sample. Current Psychology, 38(1), 100–109. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9588-6
  • Somma, A., Paulhus, D., Borroni, S., & Fossati, A. (2020). Evaluating the psychometric properties of the Short Dark Triad (SD3) in Italian adults and adolescents. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 36(1), 185–195. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000499
  • Suárez-Alvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L. M., García-Cueto, E., Cuesta, M., & Muñiz, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: A questionable practice. Psicothema, 30(2), 149–158. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2018.33
  • Vazire, S., & Funder, D. C. (2006). Impulsivity and the self-defeating behaviour of narcissists. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(2), 154–165. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1002_4
  • Zeigler-Hill, V. E., & Marcus, D. K. (2019). The dark side of personality. In P. J. Corr (Ed.), Personality and individual differences: Revising the classic studies (pp. 245–262). Sage.
  • Zhang, J., Ziegler, M., & Paulhus, D. L. (2020). Development and evaluation of the Short Dark Triad: Chinese version (SD3-C). Current Psychology, 39(4), 1161–1171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00272-3