75
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

Semantic verbal fluency in native speakers of Turkish: a systematic review of category use, scoring metrics and normative data in healthy individuals

, , &
Received 16 Jun 2023, Accepted 07 Mar 2024, Published online: 21 Jun 2024

ABSTRACT

Introduction

Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) is a widely used measure of frontal executive function and access to semantic memory. SVF scoring metrics include the number of unique words generated, perseverations, intrusions, semantic cluster size and switching between clusters, and scores vary depending on the language the test is administered in. In this paper, we review the existing normative data for Turkish, the main metrics used for scoring SVF data in Turkish, and the most frequently used categories.

Method

We conducted a systematic review of peer-reviewed papers using Medline, EMBASE, PsycInfo, Web of Science, and two Turkish databases, TR-Dizin and Yok-Tez. Included papers contained data on the SVF performance of healthy adult native speakers of Turkish, and reported the categories used. Versions of the SVF that required participants to alternate categories were excluded. We extracted and tabulated demographics, descriptions of groups, metrics used, categories used, and sources of normative data. Studies were assessed for level of detail in reporting findings.

Results

1400 studies were retrieved. After deduplication, abstract, full text screening, and merging of theses with their published versions, 121 studies were included. 114 studies used the semantic category “animal”, followed by first names (N = 14, 12%). All studies reported word count. More complex measures were rare (perseverations: N = 12, 10%, clustering and switching: N = 5, 4%). Four of seven normative studies reported only word count, two also measured perseverations, and one reported category violations and perseverations. Two normative studies were published in English.

Conclusions

There is a lack of normative Turkish SVF data with more complex metrics, such as clustering and switching, and a lack of normative data published in English. Given the size of the Turkish diaspora, normative SVF data should include monolingual and bilingual speakers. Limitations include a restriction to key English and Turkish databases.

Introduction

Semantic verbal fluency (SVF) is a widely used neuropsychological assessment to examine frontal executive functions (EF) and access to semantic memory (Patterson, Citation2011). For SVF, participants are asked to produce as many words of a given category as possible in a short amount of time, usually 60 seconds. The most frequently used category is animals, but other categories such as supermarket items or fruit and vegetables have also been used. SVF is often paired with another type of verbal fluency test, phonemic or letter fluency. In letter fluency, the participant is asked to produce as many words as possible that start with a specific letter. In English, these letters are usually F, A, and S or C, F, and L (Amunts et al., Citation2020).

SVF is part of several standard cognitive assessment batteries, such as the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (Mathuranath et al., Citation2000) or Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) (Delis et al., Citation2001). Many conditions such as Alzheimer’s Disease (Gomez & White, Citation2006; Monsch et al., Citation2020), Parkinson’s Disease (Piatt et al., Citation2010; Raskin et al., Citation1992) and traumatic brain injury (Kavé et al., Citation2011; Raskin & Rearick, Citation1996; Zakzanis et al., Citation2011) consistently affect participants’ SVF performance.

The most commonly used scoring metric for SVF is the total number of correct unique words produced within the time-limit (Ardila et al., Citation2006). However, SVF is considered a multifactorial task and simply considering the total number of correct words is not thought to be sufficient to fully capture a participant’s performance. Some researchers also report the number of words produced in the first, second, third, and fourth quarter of the 60 second time-frame. Errors can provide a qualitative measure of SVF performance, and are divided into two subtypes: intrusions and perseverations (Raboutet et al., Citation2010). Intrusions or category violations are additional words that do not belong to the desired category (Raskin & Rearick, Citation1996). Perseveration refers to the repetition of words and is divided into three types (Pekkala et al., Citation2008; Sandson & Albert, Citation1987): recurrent (repeated words separated by other words, e.g., cat, dog, fox, cat), stuck-in-set (repetition of words from a previous category, such as animals, in the current category, such as supermarket items), and continuous (repeated production of a single word, e.g., cat, cat, cat). Generally, all repeated words tend to be classified as perseverations (Galaverna et al., Citation2016; Henry & Phillips, Citation2006), regardless of the location of the words produced or their semantic integrity.

Other SVF scoring metrics focus on uncovering the process by which the sequences are generated. Semantic clustering is a technique of grouping words that are similar in meaning and belong to predefined subgroups (Ober et al., Citation1986; Raskin et al., Citation1992; Robert et al., Citation1998). Troyer et al. (Citation1997) proposed a formal analysis procedure that systematically identifies clusters of related words and assesses the number of switches between clusters as well as cluster size to allow for more fine grained analyses. For example, older people produce fewer words and switches than young people, but create larger clusters (Troyer, Citation2000; Troyer et al., Citation1997). While people with dementia normally produce fewer words than healthy controls, switching is impaired in frontal lobe related neurodegenerative diseases (e.g., Parkinson’s disease) and cluster size decreases in temporal lobe involved neurodegenerative diseases (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease) (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander, et al., Citation1998; Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach, et al., Citation1998). Troyer’s approach has been adapted widely to assess these scoring metrics, which are both thought to be necessary for successful SVF performance (Hurks et al., Citation2010; Lanting et al., Citation2009; Weiss et al., Citation2006; Zhao et al., Citation2013).

Given that SVF is one of the most widely used neuropsychological tests, normative data have been collected to establish expected SVF scores for healthy native speakers in several different languages. Examples include English (Tombaugh, Citation1999), Spanish (Benito-Cuadrado et al., Citation2002), Arabic (Khalil, Citation2010), Chinese (Feng et al., Citation2012), Dutch (Van Der Elst et al., Citation2006), Hebrew (Kavé, Citation2005), Greek (Kosmidis et al., Citation2004), and Persian (Ghasemian-Shirvan et al., Citation2018). Normative studies will typically ensure that variation in performance according to age, gender and education is already accounted for in the published norms. Collecting normative data for different languages/ethnic groups is considered necessary given that SVF scores have been found to vary depending on the language/ethnic group the task is administered. For example, Kempler et al. (Citation1998) found that Hispanic individuals produced significantly fewer animal names than Chinese, White, and Vietnamese groups and that Vietnamese individuals produced more animal names than Chinese, White, and Hispanic groups. These results have been linked to the length of the animal names spoken in each language (e.g., the word “dog” in Spanish is perro, while in Vietnamese is chó); longer animal names result in fewer words generated. Therefore, it is important to establish normative data for the variations of SVF in different languages, determine what metrics have been used, and establish whether additional normative data or additional scoring methods for existing normative data would be beneficial. Both demographic information and cultural background can impact semantic memory organization, category size and content (Rosselli et al., Citation2002; Strauss et al., Citation2006). Therefore, semantic categories included in SVF require scoring guidelines (Olabarrieta-Landa et al., Citation2017) and revealing culturally and linguistically unique retrieval strategies requires in-depth study of data from the population being considered (Olabarrieta-Landa et al., Citation2017). In this review, we focus on the Turkish language. Turkish is predominantly spoken by around 84 million people in Turkey (TUIK, Turkish Statistical Institute, Citation2022). There is also a large Turkish diaspora of more than 6.5 million all over the world, 5.5 million of whom live in Western Europe (Turkish Ministry of Foreign Affairs, Turkish, Citation2022). Turkish differs substantially in word structure from more frequently studied languages, such as English. Turkish is a member of the Turkic language family, which also includes Azerbaijani and Turkmen. Its morphology is agglutinative (Durrant, Citation2013). Information such as case of nouns, plural/singular, or tense is indicated by adding an affix to a root word (Istek & Cicekli, Citation2007).

Since the design of our systematic review was descriptive, we did not have any a priori hypotheses. The systematic review addresses three research questions:

RQ1:

What SVF tasks are commonly used for Turkish?

RQ2:

What are the most commonly used scoring metrics? To what extent are more complex scoring approaches, such as clustering and switching (Troyer, Citation2000), used?

RQ3:

What normative studies exist for Turkish, and how do the tasks and metrics map onto the existing literature on SVF in Turkish?

Materials and methods

Search strategy

This review was registered on Prospero with the protocol number CRD42020201585 and passed the suitability examination on 27 October 2020. In our study, we used the PICOS structure to frame the review questions, which is one of the most widely used frameworks proposed by Richardson et al. (Citation1995) to help evaluate the validity, applicability and completeness of a systematic review. The PICOS framework creates a question that identifies: (1) the Patient problem or Population, (2) the Intervention, (3) the Comparison (if there is one), (4) the Outcome(s), and (5) the Study type. The PICOS structure of the study is summarized in . We searched Web of Science Core Collection, PsycINFO, EMBASE, MEDLINE, and two Turkish databases, TR-Dizin (National Database Index by ULAKBIM, the Turkish Academic Network and Information Centre) and Yok-Tez (Databases of National Thesis Center of the Council of Higher Education).

Table 1. PICOS specification of the systematic review inclusion criteria.

The general Boolean search formula was ((semantic fluency OR category fluency OR verbal fluency OR semantic verbal fluency OR COWAT OR Controlled Oral Word Association Test OR animal fluency OR animal naming) AND turk*), applied to the Topic, Title, Abstract, Author and Keywords fields. Local database searches were performed first using the English version of the formula, then using the Turkish translation, removing the *turk at the end. English keywords and their Turkish equivalents are given in . An initial search was performed on 5 October 2020 and updated using the same search criteria for works that were published after our initial search (9 August 2023). The search was conducted without any start date restrictions. While the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT, (Rodríguez-Aranda & Martinussen, Citation2006)) assesses letter fluency, we included it as a search term because some studies use COWAT to refer to semantic verbal fluency (Ersan, Citation2014; Uslu, Citation2012) or as an umbrella term for verbal fluency (Kiraç et al., Citation2014).

Table 2. Turkish translations of English query terms for search of Turkish databases.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

We included studies that reported primary SVF data from a group of at least two native Turkish speakers, and that were available in digital form. SVF data should be collected using the standard paradigm, i.e., naming as many words as possible from a given category in a short time, typically 60 seconds. At least one group of participants should consist of healthy adults.

The exclusion criteria fell into three broad categories: population; methodology; and accessibility.

The population-related exclusion criteria were: “study of children”; “no healthy control group”; “no monolingual control group”; and “potential effect on SVF, no control”. We defined “study of children” as a study that only reported data from participants under the age of 16. However, studies that specified a minimum age of 15 or 16 years for their participants, and that reported an average age of 18 or older, were included (Bora et al., Citation2019; Yilmaz, Citation2014). Studies that only focused on people with medical conditions (“no healthy control group”) or only on bilingual people (“no monolingual control group”) were also excluded. Finally, we excluded studies that reported individuals with potential undiagnosed conditions that might affect their performance on the SVF such as coronary artery disease (“potential effect on SVF, no control”) (Ünlü et al., Citation2013).

The methodology-related exclusion criteria were: “only letter fluency”; “composite results”; and “no results reported”. Studies that performed only letter fluency, phonemic fluency, or COWAT tests without any SVF component, were excluded as “only letter fluency”. Studies reporting “composite results” did not provide metrics on SVF performance for a specific category. Instead, scores were merged across SVF categories (Aydınoğlu, Citation2015; Erol et al., Citation2012; İpekten, Citation2018; Kaya & Zengin Alpözgen, Citation2022), participants completed tests that involve alternating categories (Erdogan, Citation2016), scores from letter fluency and SVF were mixed (Beşer, Citation2019; Gultekin et al., Citation2017; Kılavuz Ören, Citation2020), or the type of verbal fluency test was not specified (Cevik et al., Citation2016; Gücüyener et al., Citation1998; Kandemir et al., Citation2009; Karahan et al., Citation2021). If studies reported no verbal fluency results at all, they were classified as “no results reported” (Akgün, Citation2010; Boyle et al., Citation2021; Er, Citation2014; Midi et al., Citation2011).

The accessibility-related criteria were “abstract only”, which means that no full paper was available, and “no online version available”, which applied to studies, in particular Turkish theses, that were only available in printed form.

Reference management

Database search results were exported as BibTeX or RIS format except for Yok-Tez, which contains all Master, PhD and medical proficiency degree theses for diploma accreditation in Turkey. Although this system allows searching and downloading articles, it is not possible to save search results as bibliographic information. Therefore, the first author manually input references for the studies downloaded from Yok-Tez into Mendeley. Screening and data extraction were conducted using Covidence (Citation2013). Duplicates were initially resolved automatically by Covidence. Remaining duplicates were resolved manually by prioritizing the source in the order of MEDLINE, Embase, PsycInfo, Web of Science.

Screening and extraction

All studies were screened by two members of the review team (RYK, KK, MKW, SM) in the abstract and full text screening stages. Studies that were only available in Turkish were reviewed by RYK and KK, who are native speakers of Turkish, while studies available in English were reviewed by RYK, MKW, and SM. Reviewers used the note facility to highlight potential issues and record their reasoning behind the decisions made. Disagreements were resolved through discussion between members of the review team.

Nine publications (Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Demiray & Ertan, Citation2023; Kiraç et al., Citation2014; Mutlu et al., Citation2021; Özcan et al., Citation2016; Özçelik-Eroğlu et al., Citation2014; Sezikli et al., Citation2018; Töret & Özdemir, Citation2021; Uzgan et al., Citation2021) are theses that were later published as a peer-reviewed article; for those studies, we only extracted data from the published version, which is more likely to be accessible to the international community.

The PRISMA diagram () documents the screening process. The final number of studies analyzed is 121.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart.

Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart.

The reviewers jointly designed an extraction template that was designed with the research questions in mind. Extraction was done by RYK, supported by KK. SM and MKW were consulted in case of questions.

For each study, we extracted three types of general information: Language of the study (Turkish/English); Type of publication (thesis/journal article etc.); and Country in which the study was conducted (Turkey/Other). In order to establish commonly used tasks and scoring metrics (RQs 1 and 2), we extracted all semantic categories used (e.g., animals, vegetables), the word count metrics reported (mean/minimum/maximum/standard deviation etc.), any alternative analysis methods (clustering and switching/computational etc.), and the administration time of the SVF task. In order to distinguish between normative and non-normative studies (RQ3), we extracted information about study type. First, we determined the study design (longitudinal versus cross-sectional), and whether SVF was the focus of the study or part of a general assessment. If the study included a patient sample, we noted the type of health condition (e.g., cognitive impairment or dementia/mental illness/Parkinson’s Disease etc.). If the study compared two or more groups of healthy participants, we noted how the groups were defined. Finally, we extracted demographic characteristics for the healthy native speakers of Turkish who were assessed. This included the age of the participants (older adults, aged 64, adults aged 18–64, young people aged 16–25, and children/pediatric groups (16), and whether multiple age groups were differentiated. We noted the number of healthy participants, whether gender information was recorded, whether an indicator of level of education was used (years/levels/none), whether socioeconomic status was reported (No/Yes, occupation/Yes, income bracket), and age groups reported.

In the main results tables, we report the data extracted from each study and highlight missing information. The tables are summarized in the text using counts and percentages. For the tables, studies were split into normative and non-normative (c.f. ). Non-normative studies were additionally split into studies comparing groups of healthy adults (c.f. ), studies comparing healthy controls to people with a mental health condition (c.f. ), studies comparing healthy controls to people with a neurodegenerative disease (c.f. ), and studies comparing healthy adult controls to people with other diseases (c.f. ).

Table 3. List of normative studies with full extracted data. [*]Because our scope is adults, the results of the 30 children used in the study were excluded. Therefore, our assessment is based on 120 adults and the total number of people is 30 less than the actual number reported in the study.

Table 4. List of studies that compare groups of healthy native speakers with full extracted data.

Table 5. List of studies comparing people with a mental health disorder to a healthy control group with full extracted data.

Table 6. List of studies comparing people with neurodegenerative disorders to healthy controls with full extracted data.

Table 7. List of other studies comparing a group of Turkish speakers with a disease or disorder to healthy controls with full extracted data.

Quality assessment

As we were interested in the SVF results, which may not have been the main outcome measure of the studies included, we did not conduct a formal bias assessment of the studies themselves. Rather, we noted the level of detail with which the data collection and analysis procedures for SVF scores was reported, and summarized our findings in an overall assessment of the studies. Studies received one point for each relevant detail (e.g., whether words were audio recorded or recorded in writing; whether a reference was given that describes the exact scoring method; etc.). For each study, two members of the review team independently performed this quality assessment and disagreements were resolved by discussion between three members of the review team (RYK, MKW, SM). Data synthesis was independent of the quality assessment.

Results

RQ1 and RQ2: Categories and scoring metrics

In general, animal naming is the preferred category in SVF, because it is least affected by country, cultural, educational and generational differences (Ardila et al., Citation2006). As in other languages, for Turkish, animals are the most frequently used category (n = 114, 94%). Other categories were first names (n = 14, 12%), vegetables and fruits (n = 6, 5%) and supermarket items (n = 7, 6%). Even more rarely used categories included vehicles, clothes, parts of the body, furniture, famous people, breakfast items, food, beverages, and household items. Most of the rarer categories appeared in normative studies, which tended to provide verbal fluency data for a range of possible categories.

The administration time for SVF varied. Sixty seconds was used in 87% of studies (n = 105), 12% of studies did not indicate the duration of the test (n = 14), 2% used 90 seconds (Diker et al., Citation2016; Özdemir, Citation2015) (n = 2), and one (1%) study with a dual task design used 30 seconds (Özkul et al., Citation2021).

In terms of the metrics used, all studies reported the total number of words generated. Only four studies provided the number of words provided in each quarter of the 60 second period (Çukurova, Citation2020; Kandemir, Citation2006; Özdemir, Citation2015; Özdemir & Tunçer, Citation2021). In terms of more in-depth analyses, 12 studies (Aki et al., Citation2022; Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Çukurova, Citation2020; Demiray & Ertan, Citation2023; Ersan, Citation2014; İlkmen & Büyükişcan, Citation2022; Kalafatoglu, Citation2015; Sahin, Citation2022; Sezikli et al., Citation2018; Tumaç, Citation1997; Uzgan et al., Citation2021; Yazici, Citation2019) (10%) reported perseverations, and six studies (Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Çukurova, Citation2020; Güneş et al., Citation2022; İlkmen & Büyükişcan, Citation2022; Kalafatoglu, Citation2015; Yazici, Citation2019) (5%) reported category violations. Clustering and switching was also used in only five studies (Altun, Citation2022; Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Kalafatoglu, Citation2015; Karaca, Citation2015; Uzgan et al., Citation2021) (4%). None of these papers provided a full translation for the original English taxonomy, although three papers listed the groups of animals used. Çabuk et al. (Citation2020) and Kalafatoglu (Citation2015) created a limited number of animal groups, namely farm, pets, forest, and zoological categories (insects, birds, and fish). Both studies also report perseverations. In their comparative study of frontotemporal lobe degeneration and semantic dementia, Karaca (Citation2015) used different groups, namely poultry, ovine livestock, bovine livestock, forest, farm, birds, and insects. All studies were based on manual analysis, and differences between groups were established using standard inferential statistics. Bora et al. (Citation2019) used manually established SVF scores as parameters for latent class analysis to explore group differences.

In the following subsections, we present an overview of the main tasks and metrics for non-normative studies. These fall into three categories: studies of bilingualism; comparisons between groups of healthy native speakers; and comparisons between patients and healthy control groups. Since a full meta analysis of Turkish SVF data is beyond the scope of this review, and the number of studies across all categories is substantial, we only summarize findings for studies where SVF performance was a main focus.

Bilingualism

We found only one study that focused on speaking another language in addition to Turkish, which was a Turkish Masters dissertation (c.f. ). Yazici (Citation2019) compared the executive functions of bilingual Kurdish-Turkish speakers from Eastern and Southeastern Anatolia (n = 80) to that of monolingual Turkish speakers from across the country (n = 80). Animal naming was used, and word count, perseverations, and category violations were reported. Participants had 9 years or more of education and the age range was 20–54 years. Bilingual individuals’ language proficiency levels were not formally determined, and there was no information about proficiency in other languages. While the study did not focus on SVF performance, the author argued that cultural aspects, such as languages spoken, need to be accounted for in normative studies of executive function in Turkish.

Comparison between groups of healthy native speakers

Excluding normative studies, we found 21 studies conducted with healthy individuals, which are summarized in . Only two studies were conducted outside of Turkey. These were studies of minorities in Denmark (Nielsen & Waldemar, Citation2016; Nielsen et al., Citation2012) carried out with the help of an interpreter, and did not focus on bilingual or multilingual skills. Five studies focused on people aged 50 years and over. The topics of these studies fell into five distinct categories:

Physical activity

Exercise may have a positive effect on cognitive abilities due to the acceleration of blood circulation and increase of oxygen delivery to the brain (Mandolesi et al., Citation2018). Three studies examined the overall effect of exercise on cognition and used SVF as part of a standard assessment battery (Gökçe et al., Citation2021): tennis (Gökçe, Citation2020); fencing or swimming (Yeniçeri, Citation2019); and balance training.

Cognitive reserve

Lifetime experiences, such as educational and occupational attainment, literacy attainment and the involvement in cognitive and socially stimulating activities, are thought to increase the efficacy of cognitive processing in older age (for a review, see Arenaza-Urquijo et al. (Citation2015)). Ekin and Çebi (Citation2021) examined the concept of cognitive reserve and emotion regulation in older people, while Yıldırım and Ogel-Balaban (Citation2021) studied older people’s use of social media (e.g., Facebook). İnal (Citation2019) compared professional musicians with non-musicians to investigate the effects of active interest in music. In all three studies, SVF was used as part of a standard assessment. Nielsen and Waldemar (Citation2016) studied the relationship between SVF and literacy of healthy older Turkish immigrants in Denmark. Two categories were used, animals and supermarket items. Illiterate immigrants performed worse than literate ones on animal naming, but equally well on the supermarket category.

Cognition

In their study of the effect of various forms of training, including dual task training, on reducing fall risk, Balcı (Citation2016) used SVF as part of their assessment battery. Despite the title of the thesis, Bozdemir (Citation2008) focused on establishing Turkish normative data for the Pyramid and Palm Trees test, a semantic dementia test developed by Howard and Patterson (Citation1992). SVF was administered as part of a larger battery of tests for comparison purposes. In the only study to specifically highlight SVF performance, Talas (Citation2009) studied the relationship between magical ideation, handedness, and verbal fluency. They found a negative correlation between magical ideation scores and SVF scores, where the higher the magical thinking, the poorer the SVF performance.

Familial risk

Studies in this group include healthy individuals who are first-degree relatives of people with a mental health condition and are therefore considered to have familial risk factors compared to the general population. The conditions included are schizophrenia (Aydın et al. Citation2017; Berberoğlu, Citation2018; Gürses, Citation2009; Kapu, Citation2019; Noyan, Citation2011) and substance-induced psychotic disorder (Çukurova, Citation2020). Bora et al. (Citation2019) used Latent Class Analysis (LCA) to classify the neurocognitive performance of euthymic children of parents with bipolar disorder. They found that the performance of the children of people with bipolar disorder could be categorized as: severe impairment; intermediate impairment; and good performance. SVF word count was significantly lower for children classified in the severe and intermediate impairment groups, compared to healthy controls and the “good performance” group.

Other

Studies in this category do not share a common framework in terms of their focus. Albayrak (Citation2015) investigates procrastination in young adults, using SVF as part of their assessment battery. Evlice (Citation2016) reports the effects of age, gender, and education on healthy participants’ performance on a standard battery of neuropsychological tests. SVF performance is negatively correlated with age (where the older the age, the poorer the SVF performance) and positively correlated with education (where the higher the level of education, the higher the SVF performance). In their comparison of Turkish immigrants and Danish citizens, Nielsen et al. (Citation2012) examined the cross-cultural applicability of SVF, but without a monolingual Turkish control group. They used supermarket items as the test category. Turkish participants were administered the tests in Turkish using an interpreter. Nielsen et al. found that Turkish immigrants with higher acculturation produced more items, while older immigrants produced fewer. Güneş et al. (Citation2022) investigated SVF performance in medical school students in terms of low, normal and excessive sleep duration. They revealed that sleep duration has no significant influence on SVF performance. In another study, Sahin (Citation2023) compared video gamers, athletes, musicians and participants who do not engage in any activity and found no significant difference between groups.

Comparison between patients and healthy control groups

Ninety-two out of 121 studies (76%) compared the performance of people with an impairment or condition to a healthy control group.

Mental health

Thirty-three of 92 studies (36%), summarized in , involved mental health conditions. In descending order of frequency, conditions included schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder, depression, hyperactivity, borderline personality disorder, panic disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, and schizotypy.

Only Sumiyoshi et al. (Citation2014) examined verbal fluency as a main focus of their study. Both verbal fluency types (semantic fluency and letter fluency) were investigated for schizophrenic patients in three languages: Japanese, Turkish and English. Like Turkish, Japanese is an agglutinative language, which uses affixes to indicate most relevant grammatical information. For Japanese and Turkish, performance was also compared to healthy controls. In Japanese, which uses two syllable-based scripts and one logographic script, letter fluency was more impaired than semantic fluency; in Turkish, which uses an alphabetic script, semantic fluency was more impaired than letter fluency. Uzgan et al. (Citation2021) were the only authors to examine perseverations and number of switches in addition to SVF word count. There were no differences in any of the SVF measures between people with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and healthy controls. Unfortunately, the study, which used animals as a category, does not provide information on how the animal groups were formed.

Neurodegenerative diseases

Twenty-four of 92 studies (26%) studied neurodegenerative diseases (c.f. ). Conditions that are frequently examined are the dementias, in particular Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), Parkinson’s Disease, Multiple Sclerosis, and Essential Tremor.

Most studies used animals as their category, except for two studies which used the vegetables and fruits category. The data collection period was 60 seconds, except for Diker (Citation2014) (90 seconds), Demiray and Ertan (Citation2023); Hanagasi et al. (Citation2002); Yılmaz et al. (Citation2020) (no information), and Özkul et al. (Citation2021) (30 seconds). Özkul et al. (Citation2021) also used a novel scoring method, the number of correct words per second, which is an uncommon way of reporting SVF performance.

Five studies, which all use the standard animal category, focus on SVF. The longitudinal study by Dinç (Citation2019), which compared MCI patients, MCI patients who progressed to AD, and healthy controls, used the standard word count metric. They found that SVF was a good predictor of conversion to AD. The other three studies (Altun, Citation2022; Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Kalafatoglu, Citation2015; Karaca, Citation2015) used a clustering and switching approach, but defined their own categories. Çabuk et al. (Citation2020) and Kalafatoglu (Citation2015) created a limited number of animal groups, namely farm, pets, forest, and zoological categories (insects, birds, and fish). Both studies also report perseverations. In their comparative study of frontotemporal lobe degeneration and semantic dementia, Karaca (Citation2015) used different groups, namely poultry, ovine livestock, bovine livestock, forest, farm, birds, and insects. Altun (Citation2022) stated that the original Troyer animal groups were used, but did not provide information on how they were handled if a different animal name existed. The study excluded perseverations for the total word count, but included them for the cluster analysis. If subcategories were provided, the parent category name was excluded while creating clusters. For example, if “fish” was mentioned along with different fish names (e.g., salmon, sardine), the word “fish” was omitted.

Çabuk et al. (Citation2020), comparing patients with amnestic MCI to healthy controls, found no difference in clustering and switching related parameters for SVF performance, but patients with amnestic MCI produced more perseverations. Kalafatoglu (Citation2015) reported smaller SVF cluster sizes in patients with probable dementia compared to healthy controls. In Karaca (Citation2015)’s study of patients with frontotemporal lobe degeneration, they produce fewer switches than healthy controls. Another study, Altun (Citation2022), focused on multiple sclerosis patients and found that multiple sclerosis patients produce significantly fewer total words compared to healthy controls. No difference was observed between the groups in terms of mean cluster size, but fewer switches were produced in those with multiple sclerosis.

Other conditions

The remaining 35 (29%) studies, summarized in , covered six types of conditions, most of which are neurological. Ten studies focus on epilepsy, followed by addiction and stroke. All studies reported animal naming. Two studies added first names as a second category (Altunkaynak et al., Citation2019; Demir & Uluğ, Citation2002). In addition to word count, 3 studies report perseverations (Ersan, Citation2014; Sahin, Citation2022; Sezikli et al., Citation2018), while Kandemir (Citation2006) reports performance every 15 seconds. SVF was always used as a supporting tool, never as a central focus of the research studies.

RQ3: Normative studies

We found a total of seven normative studies that focus on SVF performance. Two further studies, Bozdemir (Citation2008) and Evlice (Citation2016), also collected data on SVF performance across a large sample of participants, but did not specifically aim to produce normative data for SVF. The details of the studies are given in in order of publication year.

Except for Tuncer (Citation2012), all studies recruited a balanced number of male and female participants. While Özdemir and Tunçer (Citation2021) focused only on people aged 60 years and older, the other six studies recruited participants across the entire adult age range. All normative studies report level of education based on the Turkish national education system. Only Tuncer (Citation2012) additionally explored illiteracy. No socioeconomic status information is reported, except Tumaç (Citation1997). The number of participants varied between 58 (Özdemir & Tunçer, Citation2021) and 1431 (İlkmen & Büyükişcan, Citation2022).

In terms of category type, the most comprehensive study was conducted by Tuncer (Citation2012), who collected SVF data for six different categories, namely animals, vegetables and fruits, vehicles, clothes, parts of the body, and furniture, from 400 participants. Tuncer (Citation2012) was also the only study that focused on literacy. Two studies (Özdemir, Citation2015; Özdemir & Tunçer, Citation2021) examined five categories in addition to animals, namely breakfast items, famous people, food, beverages, and household items. Tuncer (Citation2012) and Özdemir (Citation2015) also list the most frequently used words and the first words produced for each category (e.g., dog, cat, horse, and donkey, for the animal category). While İlkmen and Büyükişcan (Citation2022); Şentürk (Citation2019); Tumaç (Citation1997) focused only on animal naming, Aki et al. (Citation2022) examined both animal names and first names.

Usually, the time allowed for participants to produce the words is 60 seconds. In the only study using 90 seconds (Özdemir, Citation2015), the total time was divided into 3 equal parts with 30 second chunks and the differences between the chunks were examined. This is equivalent to the common method implemented by İlkmen and Büyükişcan (Citation2022) of splitting 60 seconds into 15 second chunks and counting the number of words produced in each chunk. Word counts are reported excluding perseverations and category violations. Aki et al. (Citation2022) and Tumaç (Citation1997) report perseverations and İlkmen and Büyükişcan (Citation2022) report perseverations and category violations in addition to word counts. No other analysis approaches were found.

Tumaç (Citation1997), an unpublished Masters dissertation, reports normative results for a test battery that is sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Considering age and education levels together, nine different groups were evaluated based on three age groups (young (15–18 years), middle-aged (32–45 years) and older (50–75 years)) and three education levels (low-medium-high) using animal naming. In terms of total word count, the highest score was achieved by the middle-aged group with the highest education (Mean = 26.65, SD = 5.04), while the older group with the lowest education (Mean = 19.25, SD = 4.46) and the middle age group with the lowest education (Mean = 19.40, SD = 3.66) resulted in the lowest scores. The older age group with the lowest education produced the highest number of perseverations (Mean = 1.05, SD = 0.94), followed by the middle-aged group with the highest education (Mean = 0.75, SD = 1.02). Although the participants’ occupational information was presented in the background demographic information, it was not included in the analysis.

In Tuncer (Citation2012)’s study, SVF scores were higher than letter fluency scores. The category yielding the highest scores was body parts, followed by fruits and vegetables, animals, clothes, vehicles, and furniture. Highly educated participants had higher scores; there was no difference with people who had only a few years of education and those who were illiterate. The 25–34 years age group had the highest scores, and the 65+ age group had the lowest.

Özdemir (Citation2015) confirms Tuncer’s results with regard to age and education in a sample of 150 participants aged between 15 and 81 years. In an in-depth study of the effect of education in older people aged 60–81, Özdemir and Tunçer (Citation2021) showed that education level affected SVF scores, where the mean number of words every 15 seconds decreased linearly, but not chronological age.

Aki et al. (Citation2022) report results from 415 participants. Age, gender, and level of education affected performance on SVF for first names. Higher scores on this task are linked to female gender, 9 or more years of education, and being in the 15–24 and 25–34 age groups. Animal SVF performance, in contrast, is only affected by level of education. Those with 12 or more years outperform those with 9–11 years of education, who in turn produce more words than those with only 5–8 years. There were not sufficient perseverations in either the animal or the names SVF tasks to warrant analysis. Şentürk (Citation2019)’s study of the SVF performance of 200 healthy volunteers aged between 18 and 49 years confirms a strong effect of level of education with higher education associated with better SVF performance. In addition, male participants produced significantly more words than females.

İlkmen and Büyükişcan (Citation2022) conducted the largest normative study in terms of number of participants with a total of 1431 healthy participants (male = 727, female = 704) from Istanbul. Eight different age groups (ranging from 18 to 89 years) were compared for four educational levels (elementary to postgraduate). They found that SVF performance was significantly negatively correlated with age but is positively related to education. Also, they emphasized that there was no difference in SVF performance between genders. Total word count decreased for people aged 50 and older.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first systematic review focusing on available semantic fluency data for native speakers of Turkish. We reviewed normative and non-normative studies that report data on SVF frequencies that were produced in Turkish by healthy native speakers of the Turkish language, including studies where healthy native speakers formed a control group.

We found that, while SVF is a key part of neuropsychological assessment in studies of Turkish speakers, there is a paucity of normative data for all categories typically administered. In addition, one of the categories, first names, appears specific to Turkish. Unlike categories such as animals, fruits, or supermarket items, first names do not easily lend themselves to analyses that focus on the structure of a person’s mental lexicon.

Most studies limit themselves to reporting word counts. We only found 15 (12%) studies that used more complex metrics. In cases where sequences were annotated with clusters and switches, the rules for scoring animal groups were not published, and the animal groups did not cover all of the groups defined by Troyer and collaborators (Altun, Citation2022; Çabuk et al., Citation2020; Kalafatoglu, Citation2015; Karaca, Citation2015; Uzgan et al., Citation2021). The lack of categorization rules may explain why studies do not consider additional clustering and switching SVF metrics in Turkish studies. Moreover, without these rules, clinicians cannot consider clustering and switching in their patients’ assessments. While word counts provide clinicians with a quick and easy measure, more detailed qualitative analysis for scoring SVF data can provide additional insights into human cognitive performance (Abwender et al., Citation2001; Mayr & Kliegl, Citation2000; Troyer, Citation2000; Troyer et al., Citation1997). Therefore, future work should provide normative data with the categorization rules for the switching and clustering metrics in Turkish.

Our review revealed that there were very little data on the performance of bilingual versus monolingual speakers in Turkish, which is a clear gap in the literature given that 6.5 million Turkish speakers live in the diaspora abroad (Turkish Ministry of Foreign Affairs, Turkish, Citation2022). Within Türkiye itself, several different languages are spoken, the most prominent of which is Kurdish. The Turkish Demographic and Health Survey from 2003 reports that 83% of Turkish people speak Turkish as their native language, 14% Kurdish, 2% Arabic, and 1% other languages (Koc et al., Citation2008). The only study in our data set that actually incorporates bilingualism involved Kurdish speakers in Turkey (Yazici, Citation2019). Two studies compared Turkish immigrants to Denmark to older native speakers of Danish (Nielsen & Waldemar, Citation2016; Nielsen et al., Citation2012), but they do not focus on bilingual competence.

Previous studies comparing bilinguals’ and monolinguals’ SVF performance in other languages have shown mixed results (Bialystok et al., Citation2008; Luo et al., Citation2010; Paap et al., Citation2017; Patra et al., Citation2020; Sandoval et al., Citation2010). While monolinguals have been found to produce significantly more correct words than bilinguals (e.g., Sandoval et al., Citation2010), the bilingual disadvantage is no longer found when the groups are matched for vocabulary (e.g., Bialystok et al., Citation2008). However, both the monolinguals and bilinguals were tested in English, which may not have been the dominant language for the bilingual group and a lack of proficiency in English may explain their disadvantage. In a recent systematic review (Giovannoli et al., Citation2023), more than half of the studies did not report significant differences between monolinguals and bilinguals on SVF, a small number of studies demonstrated some bilingual difficulties but no study reported a bilingual advantage. As in our own review, most studies have assessed participants using the “animal” category, with only three studies (Gollan et al., Citation2002; Keijzer & Schmid, Citation2016; Sandoval et al., Citation2010) adopting multiple categories. However, not all categories may be suitable for individuals living in different countries or speaking different languages; moreover, while animals may be the best cross linguistic category (Ardila, Citation2020), categories that draw upon everyday experience are better for assessing lower educated individuals (Nielsen & Waldemar, Citation2016).

When examining the provenance of the original 130 publications, we found that 82 studies (63%) were PhD or Master’s theses, and only 48 studies (37%) were peer-reviewed papers. All theses, except for Baran (Citation2008), were written in English. Most of them were prepared by medical doctors during their specialization training in areas such as neurology or psychiatry. As noted in the Methods section, only 9 theses (Baysal Kiraç, Citation2012; Çabuk, Citation2018; Mutlu, Citation2018; Önder, Citation2019; Özcan, Citation2010; Özçelik-Eroğlu, Citation2012; Sezikli, Citation2014; Töret, Citation2019; Yavuz-Demiray, Citation2011) were published as peer-reviewed versions. Of the 48 peer-reviewed studies, 38 (79%) were published in English, nine (21%) in Turkish, and one (Töret & Özdemir, Citation2021) in both languages. Normative studies available in English were Aki et al. (Citation2022); İlkmen and Büyükişcan (Citation2022). This makes it very difficult for international researchers and clinicians to access data on SVF performance in Turkish.

Limitations

Relevant studies are often published in the grey literature, i.e., not by commercial publishing organizations. In this review, we focused on an easily searchable type of grey literature, namely dissertations. Other types, such as preprints only published on preprint archives, government white papers, or hospital reports (Adams et al., Citation2016), were not considered. There was one normative study for which we were unable to obtain the full text. Bingöl et al. (Citation1994) is frequently cited as a key normative study, but no information about study design and results could be found. The other study, Tumaç (Citation1997), was found through a hand search of references (Cengiz-Al et al., Citation2023; İlkmen & Büyükişcan, Citation2022) where they cited the study as a normative study. Tumaç (Citation1997)’s study was added into this review and study details were shared in . Yet, given our difficulty accessing Bingöl et al. (Citation1994)'s study, it is unlikely that their normative data are available to other researchers and clinicians working in Turkish.

There were also a few terminological issues that made our search more difficult. For example, we found two studies that reported SVF as a subordinate task of COWAT, which is why it was included in the original search (Çakar, Citation2020; Kiraç et al., Citation2014). Ersan (Citation2014) used “Word List Generation” for semantic fluency, and two studies (Ersan, Citation2014; Özcan et al., Citation2016) viewed category fluency as having two parts, animal naming and alternate fruit and animal naming.

Finally, due to the heterogeneity of the studies reported, we did not perform a meta-analysis of the SVF scores reported in the literature. Instead, we hope that researchers can use the groundwork laid in this paper to perform meta-analyses for smaller, well-defined groups of conditions.

Implications

The results of our systematic review indicate that individuals administering the SVF in non-clinical Turkish populations tend to use the animal naming category most frequently. Moreover, most studies simply report the total number of words produced. While a small number of studies provide more in-depth analyses that include clustering and switching, these studies do not provide the rules for scoring the various animal groups, making it difficult for individuals to apply these metrics to their own data. Therefore, there is a need for better Turkish normative data, which include the various semantic categories administered, the various scoring metrics and the category grouping rules.

While a simple count of the number of words produced provides a quick and easy measure of SVF performance, applying Troyer’s scoring method manually is a little more time-consuming as it requires careful reanalysis of the sequence of words produced; clinicians may have limited time to spend with their patients and so using additional scoring metrics is less feasible. Moreover, the established procedure specifically addresses animal naming, but offers insufficient subgroups for many animals (local animals, mythological animals etc.). Some names may be located in more than one group (e.g., parrot can be considered within the human use category as a pet or the zoological category as a bird), which makes cluster identification difficult (Woods et al., Citation2016). Researchers have been investigating alternative approaches to identifying semantic clusters using generalizable, fast, and robust models that can be easily adapted to different languages and scenarios. Frequently investigated computational linguistics approaches are lexical relations (WordNet) (Paula et al., Citation2018; Quaranta et al., Citation2019), distributional semantics (word2vec, glove etc.) (Holmlund et al., Citation2019; Kim et al., Citation2019; Linz et al., Citation2017; Voppel et al., Citation2021), and transformer language models (BERT) (Alaçam et al., Citation2022).

Conclusions

In summary, animal naming appears to be the preferred category when SVF is administered to Turkish-speaking individuals. Moreover, those administering the test appear to opt for the quick and easy method of scoring the SVF by simply considering the number of unique words generated. However, the SVF is considered a multifactorial test and so this score may not entirely represent an individual’s performance; other scoring metrics such as cluster size and switching (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Leach, et al., Citation1998) may be differentially spared and impaired. Therefore, the methodology for producing semantic subcategories as well as normative data for the various SVF metrics is needed in Turkish to allow clinicians and researchers to understand their patients’ cognitive profiles qualitatively.

Author contributions

RYK, SM, and MKW designed the review. RYK performed the literature search, and RYK, KK, SM, and MKW participated in abstract screening and full text review. RYK and KK extracted data. RYK wrote the first draft of this paper, which was edited by MKW and SM. All authors read and commented on the final draft.

Prospero statement

This review was registered on Prospero with the protocol number CRD42020201585 and passed the suitability examination on 27 October 2020.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

The paper presents all included studies together with the full extracted data. The tables are also provided as a supplementary Excel file. We can provide access to the Covidence repository associated with this review on request.

Additional information

Funding

This study was supported by the Ministry of National Education of Türkiye as part of Rabia Yasa Kostas’s PhD education.

References

  • Abwender, D. A., Swan, J. G., Bowerman, J. T., & Connolly, S. W. (2001). Qualitative analysis of verbal fluency output: Review and comparison of several scoring methods. Assessment, 8(3), 323–338. https://doi.org/10.1177/107319110100800308
  • Adams, J., Hillier-Brown, F. C., Moore, H. J., Lake, A. A., Araujo-Soares, V., White, M., & Summerbell, C. (2016). Searching and synthesising ‘Grey literature’and ‘Grey information’in public health: Critical reflections on three case studies. Systematic Reviews, 5(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13643-016-0337-y
  • Akdemir, E. M. (2021) Assessment of cognitive functions in patients with chronic pain [PhD thesis]. Eskişehir Osmangazi University.
  • Akgün, B. (2010). Examination of alcohol dependents’ neuropsychological test performance in the context of treatment motivaton [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Aki, Ö. E., Alkan, B., Demirsöz, T., Velibaşoğlu, B., Taşdemir, T., Erbaş, S. P., Selvi, K., Ergenç, İ., Barişkin, E., & Özdemir, P. (2022). Effects of age, gender and education on phonemic and semantic verbal fluency. Turk Psikiyatri Dergisi = Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 33(1), 53–64. https://doi.org/10.5080/u25553
  • Aksoy-Poyraz, C., Poyraz, B., Çağrı, T. Ş., & Arıkan, M. K. (2011). Minor physical anomalies and neurological soft signs in patients with schizophrenia and their siblings. Psychiatry Research, 190(1), 85–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.023
  • Alaçam, Ö., Schüz, S., Wegrzyn, M., Kißler, J., & Zarrieß, S. (2022). Exploring semantic spaces for detecting clustering and switching in verbal fluency. Proceedings of the 29th International Conference on Computational Linguistics (pp. 178–191).
  • Albayrak, E. (2015). The investigation of people who have or have not general procrastination in terms of executive functioning [ PhD thesis]. Halic University.
  • Alibas, H., Uluc, K., Kahraman Koytak, P., Uygur, M. M., Tuncer, N., Tanridag, T., & Gogas Yavuz, D. (2017). Evaluation of depressive mood and cognitive functions in patients with acromegaly under somatostatin analogue therapy. Journal of Endocrinological Investigation, 40(12), 1365–1372. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40618-017-0722-y
  • Altun, M. B. (2022). Investigation of verbal fluency skills of individuals with relapse remitting multiple sclerosis [ PhD thesis]. İstinye University.
  • Altunkaynak, Y., Usta, Ş., Ertem, D. H., Köksal, A., Ceyhan Dirican, A., & Baybaş, S. (2019). Cognitive functioning and silent neurological manifestations in Behçet’s disease with ocular involvement. Noropsikiyatri Arsivi, 56(3), 173–177. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2017.19406
  • Amunts, J., Camilleri, J. A., Eick, S. B., Heim, S., & Weis, S. (2020). Executive functions predict verbal fluency scores in healthy participants. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-65525-9
  • Arat, H. E. (2015). Comparing Impulsivity, Risk Taking Behavior And Neurocognitive Functions On Bipolar Disrder Comorbidity Of Adhd Versus Healthy Controls. [PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Ardila, A. (2020). A cross-Linguistic comparison of category verbal fluency test (ANIMALS): A systematic review. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 35(2), 213–225. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acz060
  • Ardila, A., Ostrosky-Solís, F., & Bernal, B. (2006). Cognitive testing toward the future: The example of semantic verbal fluency (ANIMALS). International Journal of Psychology, 41(5), 324–332. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590500345542
  • Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Wirth, M., & Chételat, G. (2015). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle: Moving towards preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 134. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2015.00134
  • Arisoy, E. T., Mayda Domaç, F., Gica, S., Ulker, M., & Ozgen Kenangil, G. (2023). The effect of disease severity and chronic CPAP-therapy on cognitive functions and event related potentials in OSAS. Ideggyógyászati Szemle, 76(3–4), 129–139. http://doi.org/10.18071/isz.76.0129
  • Aydın, E., Ülgen, M. C., Tabo, A., Balaban, Ö. D., Yeşilyurt, S., & Yumrukçal, H. (2017).Executive function and genetic loading in nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry research, 248, 105-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.027
  • Aydınoğlu, Ü. (2015). The prevalence of subthreshold autistic symptoms, effect on theory of mind and comorbidities with psychiatric disorders [ PhD thesis]. Atatürk University.
  • Bakar, E., Erdogan, & Bakar, B. (2010). Neuropsychological Assessment of Adult Patients with Shunted Hydrocephalus. Journal of Korean Neurosurgical Society, 47(3), 191 10.3340/jkns.2010.47.3.191
  • Balcı, L. A. (2016). Investigation of effects of cognitive, balance and walking education with dual task training on fall risk in elderly [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul Medipol University.
  • Balcik, Z. E. (2019). Investigation of the relationship between EEG findings and cognitive functions of patients with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Balcılar, B. (2019). Examination of Time-Dependent Changes in Neurocognitive Functions in Individuals with Bipolar Disorder: A Cohort Study [ Master thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Baran, B. (2008). Feeling-of-knowing, verb processing and executive functions in non- demented parkinson’s disease patients [ PhD thesis]. Bogazici University.
  • Baysal Kiraç, L. (2012). Assessment of early cognitive impairment in patients with clinically isolated syndromes and multiple sclerosis [ PhD thesis]. Ege University.
  • Begenen, A. K. (2020). The effect of mizophonia comorbidity on executive functions in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Benito-Cuadrado, M. M., Esteba-Castillo, S., Böhm, P., Cejudo-Bolivar, J., & Peña-Casanova, J. (2002). Semantic verbal fluency of animals: A normative and predictive study in a Spanish population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(8), 1117–1122. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.8.1117.8376
  • Berberoğlu, E. (2018). Investigation of social cognitive functions in familial risk group for psychosis [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul Faculty of Medicine.
  • Beşer, B. (2019). Evaluation of decision-making capacity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with lewy bodies [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2008). Lexical access in Bilinguals: Effects of vocabulary size and executive control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(6), 522–538. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.07.001
  • Bingöl, A., Eroğlu, G., & Haktanır, I. (1994). Türk Toplumunda sözel Akıcılık Becerisi; Bir Standardizasyon çalışması. Ulusal Nöroloji Kongresi.
  • Bora, E., Can, G., Ildız, A., Ulas, G., Ongun, C. H., Inal, N. E., & Ozerdem, A. (2019). Neurocognitive heterogeneity in young offspring of patients with bipolar disorder: The effect of putative clinical stages. Journal of Affective Disorders, 257(1606), 130–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.07.015
  • Bora, E., Vahip, S., Akdeniz, F., Gonul, A., Saffet, Eryavuz, A., Ogut, M., & Alkan, M. (2007). The effect of previous psychotic mood episodes on cognitive impairment in euthymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disorders, 9(5), 468–477. https://doi.org/10.1111/j1399-5618.2007.00469.x.
  • Boyle, R., Jollans, L., Rueda-Delgado, L. M., Rizzo, R., Yener, G. G., McMorrow, J. P., Knight, S. P., Carey, D., Robertson, I. H., Emek-Savaş, D. D., Stern, Y., Kenny, R. A., & Whelan, R. (2021). Brain-predicted age difference score is related to specific cognitive functions: A multi-site replication analysis. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 15(1), 327–345. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-020-00260-3
  • Bozdemir, M. (2008). To determine the relationship between the pyramid and pine trees test and the category fluency test [ PhD thesis]. Maltepe University.
  • Buyukgok, D. (2017). Apathy and frontal lob functions in alzheimer’s dementia patients [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Büyükgök, D., Özdemir, Ö., Ünal, T., Cem, & Barlas, O. (2021). When to Assess: Cognitive Impact of Ventriculoperitoneal Shunt Operation in Elderly Adults with Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus. World Neurosurgery, 154, e302–e312. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wneu.2021.07.015
  • Çabuk, T. (2018). Analysis of verbal fluency skills on mild cognitive impairment patients [ Master’s thesis]. Anadolu University.
  • Çabuk, T., Torun, S., & Adapınar, D. Ö. (2020). Quantitative and qualitative assessment of verbal fluency in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Turkish Journal of Neurology, 26(3), 212–219. https://doi.org/10.4274/tnd.2020.66750
  • Çakar, M. M. (2020). Evaluation of optical coherence tomography results and cognitive functions in Epilepsy patients [ PhD thesis]. Trakya University.
  • Çakmak, I. B. (2022). Investigation of the relationship between social cognitive performance and social cognition-related comt and SLC6A4 gene polymorphisms in patients with opiate use disorder [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Canli, D. (2011). The assessment of emotion recognition, working memory and other cognitive functions in schizotypy [ PhD thesis]. Kırıkkale University.
  • Cengiz, Z. (2018). The relationship between cavum septum pellucidum with neuropsychological functions and psychopathology [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Cengiz-Al, Z., Demirci, H., Kenar, J. G., & Özer, Ö. A. (2023). The relationship between cavum septum pellucidum and psychopathology and neuropsychological functions. Neuropsychiatric Investigation, 61(2), 57–64. https://doi.org/10.5152/NeuropsychiatricInvest.2023.23006
  • Cevik, N. (2011). Evaluation of MR Spectroscopy and cognitive functions in Juvenile Myoclonic Epilepsy Patients [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Cevik, N., Köksal, A., Burcu Doğan, V., Ceyhan Dirican, A., Bayramoğlu, S., Özturk, M., & Baybas, S. (2016). Evaluation of cognitive functions of juvenile myoclonic epileptic patients by magnetic resonance spectroscopy and neuropsychiatric cognitive tests concurrently. Neurological Sciences, 37(4), 623–627. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10072-015-2425-5
  • Covidence. (2013). Systematic review management software. Veritas Health Innovation.
  • Çukurova, M. (2020). The investigation of cognitive functions and clinical high risk status for psychosis in first-degree relatives of patients with substance induced psychotic disorder [ PhD thesis]. Sağlık Bilimleri University.
  • Degirmenci, H. (2016). Analogical reasoning in Parkinson’s disease [ PhD thesis]. Süleyman Demirel University.
  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, F., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive functions system. The Psychological Corporation.
  • Demiray, D. Y., & Ertan, S. (2023). Evaluation of cognitive functions in patients with essential tremor, Parkinson’s disease and combination of essential tremor-Parkinson’s disease. Nobel Medicus Journal, 19(1).
  • Demirci, H. (2010). Investigation of cognitive processes of patients with mutation by using potential recording and neuropsychological tests. [ Master’s thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Demirci, H. (2018). An analysis of neuropsychological profile of duschenne muscular dystrophy disease carrier mothers. [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Demir, B., & Uluğ, B. (2002). Neuropsychological functions in early and late onset alcoholism. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi= Turkish Journal of Psychiatry, 13(1), 15–21.
  • Diker, S. (2014). Association of cognitive impairment with cortical lesion load detected by Double Inversion Recevery (DIR) in clinically isolated syndromes [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Diker, S., Ceylan Has, A., Kurne, A., Göçmen, R., Karlı Oğuz, K., & Karabudak, R. (2016). The association of cognitive impairment with gray matter atrophy and cortical lesion load in clinically isolated syndrome. Multiple Sclerosis and Related Disorders, 10, 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.msard.2016.08.008
  • Dinc, S. (2014). Relationship between depression and vascular risk factors in elderly people [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Dinç, C. (2019). Predicting the disease trajectory of mild cognitive impairment with the discrepancy between semantic and phonemic fluency performance [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Dogan, H. (2020). Evaluation of executive functions in patients with essential tremor [ Phd thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Durrant, P. (2013). Formulaicity in an Agglutinating language: The case of Turkish. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 9(1), 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1515/cllt-2013-0009
  • Ekin, E., & Çebi, M. (2021, 3). The relationship between cognitive reserve, mood state and emotion regulation in healthy elderly. Turkiye Klinikleri Journal of Health Sciences, 6(3), 538–548. https://doi.org/10.5336/healthsci.2020-77772
  • Ekinci, A. (2017). Neuropsychological assessment of obsesive compulsive disorder, panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder [ Phd thesis]. Haliç University.
  • Er, A. (2015). Relationship Between Atypical Antipsychotic Treatment And Neurocognitive Functions In Bipolar Disorder [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Er, Z. C. (2014). Cardiovascular and neurocognitive changes observed in patients who underwent bypass return early to compare the relationship between intraoperative cerebral oximetry values [ PhD thesis]. Marmara University.
  • Eray, B. (2013). Neurophysiological and neuropsychological assessment of cognitive function in patients with restless legs syndrome [ PhD thesis]. Akdeniz University.
  • Erdal, Y., Perk, S., Keskinkılıc, C., Bayramoglu, B., Mahmutoglu, A., Soydan, & Emre, U. (2021). The assessment of cognitive functions in patients with isolated cerebellar infarctions: A follow-up study. Neuroscience Letters, 765, 136252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136252
  • Erdogan, Ç. (2016). Effects of antidepressants with different mechanisms of action on cognitive functions, side effects and social functioning [ PhD thesis]. Kırıkkale University.
  • Eren‐Koçak E, Kılıç C, Aydın I and Hızlı F G. (2009). Memory and prefrontal functions in earthquake survivors: differences between current and past post–traumatic stress disorder patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand, 119(1), 35–44. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01281.x
  • Erol, A., Bayram, S., Kosger, F., & Mete, L. (2012). Executive functions in patients with familial versus sporadic schizophrenia and their parents. Neuropsychobiology, 66(2), 93–99. https://doi.org/10.1159/000337738
  • Ersan, F. (2014). Comparison with in terms neurocognitive functions, impulsivity and theory of mind between alcohol dependence diagnosed adults and healthy volunteers [ PhD thesis]. Maltepe University.
  • Esen, Ö. D. (2010). Neuropsychological Performance Among Bipolar Disorder Patients, Their Unaffected First Degree Relatives And Healthy Controls [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Evlice, A. (2016). Demografik verilerin nöropsikolojik testler üzerine etkisi. Cukurova Medical Journal (Çukurova Üniversitesi Tıp Fakültesi Dergisi), 41(3), 528–532. https://doi.org/10.17826/cukmedj.237507
  • Evlice, A., Demir, T., Bozdemir, H., & Aslan, K. (2016). Neurocognitive Evaluation in Epilepsy Patients Treated with Monotheraphy: A Preliminary Report. EPILEPSI, vol.22, no.2, 51–54. https://doi.org/10.14744/epilepsi.2016.18894
  • Feng, L., Sian Chong, M., Shiong Lim, W., & Pin Ng, T. (2012). The modified mini-mental state examination test: Normative data for Singapore Chinese older adults and its performance in detecting early cognitive impairment. Singapore Medical Journal, 53(7), 458–462.
  • Galaverna, F., Bueno, A. M., Morra, C. A., Roca, M., & Torralva, T. (2016). Analysis of errors in verbal fluency tasks in patients with chronic schizophrenia. The European Journal of Psychiatry, 30(4), 305–320.
  • Ghasemian-Shirvan, E., Molavi Shirazi, S., Aminikhoo, M., Zareaan, M., & Ekhtiari, H. (2018). Preliminary normative data of Persian phonemic and semantic verbal fluency test. Iranian Journal of Psychiatry, 13(4), 288–295.
  • Giovannoli, J., Martella, D., & Casagrande, M. (2023). Executive functioning during verbal fluency tasks in Bilinguals: A systematic review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 58(4), 1316–1334. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12855
  • Gökçe, E. (2020). The effect of open and closed skill exercises on the development of cognitive skills and the level of peripheral protein signals in athletes [ PhD thesis]. Ankara University.
  • Gökçe, E., Güneş, E., Hayme, S., Aslan, E., Asutay, O., Aşar, B., Çetin, M. N., & Çevik, F. (2021). Effects of playing tennis on cognition: A Pilot study to examine hand preference effect. Journal of Ankara University Faculty of Medicine, 74(1), 27–32. https://doi.org/10.4274/atfm.galenos.2020.30301
  • Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A. (2002). Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English Bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16(4), 562–576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.562
  • Gomez, R. G., & White, A. (2006). Using verbal fluency to detect very mild dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(8), 771–775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.012
  • Gücüyener, D., Özdemir, G., & Uzuner, N. (1998). The place of lizuride in aphasia pharmacotheraopy. Türk Beyin Damar Hastalıklari Dergisi, 4(1). https://app.trdizin.gov.tr/makale/TlRreU5UVT0/
  • Gultekin, G., Batun, G. C., Yürüyen, M., Yavuzer, H., Akcan, F. E., & Emül, M. (2017). Cognitive impairment and plasma phoenixin level. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 27, S1027–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0924-977X(17)31799-6
  • Güneş, E., Üstün, S., Gökçe, E., Akat, F., Armağan, E., Gündoğdu, H. E., Bataş, K. K., Ekicioğlu, N., Akkuş, S. N., & Çil, Y. (2022). Effect of sleep duration on working memory and verbal fluency functions of medical faculty students. Ankara Universites Tip Fakultesi Mecmuasi= Journal of Ankara University Faculty of Medicine, 75(4), 479. https://doi.org/10.4274/atfm.galenos.2022.76768
  • Gürses, A. G. (2022). Evaluation of levels of serum IL-33 and SST2 in patients with psychotic disorder and their HEALTHY siblings [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Gürses, N. (2009). Correlations between neurocognitive performances and schizotypal features in first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Hanagasi, H. A., Hakan Gürvit, I., Ermutlu, N., Kaptanoğlu, G., Karamursel, S., İdrisoğlu, H. A., Emre, M., & Demiralp, T. (2002). Cognitive impairment in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis: Evidence from neuropsychological investigation and event-related potentials. Cognitive Brain Research, 14(2), 234–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00110-6
  • Hangun, S. F. (2022). Evaluation of cognitive functions, functional capacity, HEALTHY lifestyle behaviors and fatigue in individuals with COVİD-19 [ Master’s thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Hatipoglu, E., Hacioglu, Y., Polat, Y., Arslan, H. F., Oner, S., Ekmekci, O. B., & Niyazoglu, M. (2022). Do neurosteroids have impact on depression and cognitive functions in cases with acromegaly?. Growth Hormone & IGF Research, 66, 101496. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ghir.2022.101496
  • Henry, J. D., & Phillips, L. H. (2006). Covariates of production and perseveration on tests of phonemic, semantic and alternating fluency in normal aging. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition, 13(3–4), 529–551. https://doi.org/10.1080/138255890969537
  • Holmlund, T. B., Cheng, J., Foltz, P. W., Cohen, A. S., & Elvevåg, B. (2019). Updating verbal fluency analysis for the 21st Century: Applications for psychiatry. Psychiatry Research, 273, 767–769. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2019.02.014
  • Howard, D., & Patterson, K. E. (1992). The pyramids and palm trees test. Bury St EdmundsThames Valley Test Company.
  • Hurks, P. P., Schrans, D., Meijs, C., Wassenberg, R., Feron, F. J. M., & Jolles, J. (2010). Developmental changes in semantic verbal fluency: Analyses of word productivity as a function of time, clustering, and switching. Child Neuropsychology, 16(4), 366–387. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297041003671184
  • İlkmen, Yasemin S., & Büyükişcan, Ezgi Soncu (2022). Verbal fluency tests: Normative data stratified by age and education in an Istanbul sample. Turkish Journal of Neurology/Turk Noroloji Dergisi, 28(2), 102–110. https://doi.org/10.4274/tnd.2022.36824
  • İnal, S. Ç. (2019). Effect of music performance on cognitive functions and its relation with BDNF val66met and COMT val158met Polymorphisms [ PhD thesis]. Ankara University.
  • İpekten, E. (2018). Cognitive functions of the amatuer football players and its relation with indicators of brain damage, neurotrophic factors and Myokines [ PhD thesis]. Selçuk University.
  • Iscen, P. (2022). Evaluation of individuals with glucoserabrosidase (GBA) gene mutation with neuropsychological tests and neuroimaging methods [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Isikli, S. (2011). Change in hippocampus volume due to major depressive disorder [ PhD thesis]. Ege University.
  • Istek, O., & Cicekli, I. (2007). A link grammar for an agglutinative language. Proceedings of Recent Advances in Natural Language Processing (RANLP 2007) (pp. 285–290).
  • Kalafatoglu, P. (2015). Verbal fluency performance in Alzheimer’s disease [ PhD thesis]. Anadolu University.
  • Kandemir, M. (2006). Cognitive impairment in infratentorial strokes [ PhD thesis]. Bakırköy Ruh Sağlığı ve Sinir Hastalıkları Hastanesi.
  • Kandemir, M., Örnek, İ., & Kirbaş, D. (2009). Cognitive impairment in infratentorial strokes. Turk Norol Derg, 15, 166–173.
  • Kapu, A. (2019). Psychopathological vulnerability among first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: the relationship between executive functions, metacognition and emotional regulation with psychopathological symptoms [ PhD thesis]. Saglik Bilimleri University.
  • Karaca, E. (2015). The verbal fluency performances of people with frontotemporal lobar degeneration [ PhD thesis]. Anadolu University.
  • Karahan, M., Kocabeyoğlu, S. S., Kervan, Ü., Sert, D. E., Erdogan Bakar, E., Aygun, E., Tola, M., Demirkan, B., Mungan, S., Catav, Z., & Pac, M. (2021). More continuous flow, better learning? The effect of aortic valve opening in patients with left ventricular assist device. The International Journal of Artificial Organs, 44(5), 325–331. https://doi.org/10.1177/0391398820963284
  • Kartal, P. (2015). Obsessive compulsive disorder in adult patients diagnosed neuropsychological functions, risk of impulsivity open up and comparison with healthy volunteers [ PhD thesis]. Maltepe University.
  • Kavé, G. (2005). Phonemic fluency, semantic fluency, and difference scores: Normative data for adult Hebrew speakers. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(6), 690–699. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390490918499
  • Kavé, G., Heled, E., Vakil, E., & Agranov, E. (2011). Which verbal fluency measure is most useful in demonstrating executive deficits after traumatic brain injury? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(3), 358–365. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2010.518703
  • Kaya, B. K., & Zengin Alpözgen, A. (2022). Comparing the cognitive functioning effects of aerobic and pilates exercises for inactive young adults: A randomized controlled trial. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 129(1), 134–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/00315125211051178
  • Kayserili, G. (2010). The study of validity and reability of b. dubois 5 word test for the adult individuals at the age of 50 and over [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Keijzer, M. C., & Schmid, M. S. (2016). Individual differences in cognitive control advantages of elderly late Dutch-English bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6(1–2), 64–85. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.14032.kei
  • Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Dick, M., Taussig, I. M., & Davis, D. S. (1998). The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal fluency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 531–538. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798466013
  • Khalil, M. S. (2010). Preliminary Arabic normative data of neuropsychological tests: The verbal and design fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(9), 1028–1035. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803391003672305
  • Kılavuz Ören, G. (2020). The effects of 8-week aerobic exercise training in individuals with focal Epilepsy [ PhD thesis]. Pamukkale University.
  • Kilic, M. K. (2015). Panic disorder: Neurocognitive functions and impulsivity [ PhD thesis]. Maltepe University.
  • Kilic, S. (2012). Evaluation of hippocampal and extrahippocampal structures by using magnetic resonance volumetry and their relation to cognitive decline among patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy [ PhD thesis]. Kocaeli University.
  • Kim, N., Kim, J.-H., Wolters, M. K., MacPherson, S. E., & Park, J. C. (2019). Automatic scoring of semantic fluency. Frontiers in Psychology, 10(May), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01020
  • Kiraç, L. B., Ekmekçi, Ö., Yüceyar, N., & Kocaman, A. S. (2014). Assessment of early cognitive impairment in patients with clinically isolated syndromes and multiple sclerosis. Behavioural Neurology, 2014, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/637694
  • Kiylioglu, Z. Ç. (2018). Adaptation of the perceived deficits questionnaire to turkish, validity and reliability study in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder [ Master’s thesis]. Ankara University.
  • Kizil, A. (2015). The examination of cognitive functions, affective processes and social contact concerning executive functions in adults with epileptic psycho-syndrome [ Master’s thesis]. Haliç University.
  • Koc, I., Hancioglu, A., & Cavlin, A. (2008). Demographic differentials and demographic integration of Turkish and Kurdish Populations in Turkey. Population Research and Policy Review, 27(4), 447–457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11113-008-9072-y
  • Kocuk, N. (2010). Effects of alcohol on neurocognitive functions and brain [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Kosmidis, M. H., Vlahou, C. H., Panagiotaki, P., & Kiosseoglou, G. (2004). The verbal fluency task in the Greek population: Normative data, and clustering and switching strategies. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 10(2), 164. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617704102014
  • Kurt, E. (2013). A comparison of executive functions with obsessive compulsive disorder, panic disorder and healthy controls [ PhD thesis]. Marmara University.
  • Kurt, P. (2008) Examination of the Effect of Impairment in Attentional Processes on Other Cognitive Domains: 6 Years Follow-up Study [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylul University.
  • Lanting, S., Haugrud, N., & Crossley, M. (2009). The effect of age and sex on clustering and switching during speeded verbal fluency tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(2), 196–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709090237
  • Linz, N., Tröger, J., Alexandersson, J., & König, A. (2017). Using neural word embeddings in the analysis of the clinical semantic verbal fluency task. IWCS 2017-12th International Conference on Computational Semantics (pp. 1–7).
  • Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and Executive control on verbal fluency performance in Bilinguals. Cognition, 114(1), 29–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014
  • Mandolesi, L., Polverino, A., Montuori, S., Foti, F., Ferraioli, G., Sorrentino, P., & Sorrentino, G. (2018). Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: Biological and psychological benefits. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509
  • Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Rakowicz, W., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 315–320. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000434309.85312.19
  • Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Complex semantic processing in old age: Does it stay or does it go? Psychology and Aging, 15(1), 29. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.15.1.29
  • Midi, İ., Doğan, M., Pata, Y. S., Koçak, İ., Mollahasanoğlu, A., & Tuncer, N. (2011). The effects of verbal reaction time in Alzheimer’s disease. The Laryngoscope, 121(7), 1495–1503. https://doi.org/10.1002/lary.21753
  • Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L. J. (2020). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the of dementia of the alzheimer. Archives of Neurology.
  • Mutlu, E. (2018). Adaptation of thought and language disorder scale into Turkish and the relation of formal thought disorder with illness severity, cognitive functions, global and social functioning and quality of life in patients with Schizophrenia [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Mutlu, E., Abaoğlu, H., Barışkın, E., Gürel, Ş. C., Ertuğrul, A., Yazıcı, M. K., Akı, E., & Yağcıoğlu, A. E. (2021). The cognitive aspect of formal thought disorder and its relationship with global social functioning and the quality of life in schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 56(8), 1399–1410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-021-02024-w
  • Mutlu E, Abaoğlu H, Barışkın E, Gürel Ş Can, Ertuğrul A, Yazıcı M Kazım, Akı E and Yağcıoğlu A Elif. (2021). The cognitive aspect of formal thought disorder and its relationship with global social functioning and the quality of life in schizophrenia. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol, 56(8), 1399–1410. 10.1007/s00127-021-02024-w
  • Nielsen, T. R., Vogel, A., & Waldemar, G. (2012). Comparison of performance on three neuropsychological tests in healthy Turkish immigrants and Danish elderly. International Psychogeriatrics, 24(9), 1515–1521. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610212000440
  • Nielsen, T. R., & Waldemar, G. (2016). Effects of literacy on semantic verbal fluency in an immigrant population. Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition, 23(5), 578–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2015.1132668
  • Noyan, H. (2011). Cognitive dysfunction related to subtypes of schizotypy in healthy first-degree relatives of patient with Schizophrenia [ PhD thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Ober, B. A., Dronkers, N. F., Koss, E., Delis, D. C., & Friedland, R. P. (1986). Retrieval from semantic memory in Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(1), 75–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638608401298
  • Olabarrieta-Landa, L., Torre, E. L., López-Mugartza, J. C., Bialystok, E., & Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2017). Verbal fluency tests: Developing a new model of administration and scoring for Spanish Language. NeuroRehabilitation, 41(2), 539–565. https://doi.org/10.3233/NRE-162102
  • Önder, B. (2019). Neurocognitive flexibility, perfectionism and obsessive beliefs in obsessive-compulsive disorder [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Önder Uzgan, B., Tetik Oktay, M., Aykaç, C., Ermiş. Ç., & Alkın, T. (2021). Neurocognitive flexibility, perfectionism, obsessive beliefs in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder (eng). Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 24(4), 439–449. https://doi.org/10.5505/kpd.2021.90187
  • Ozak, N. (2019). Reliability and validity study of schizophrenia cognition rating scale (SCORS) which assess cognitive impairment in schizophrenia patients [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Özcan, H. (2010). Neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurological impairments in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder and their healthy siblings [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Özcan, H., Özer, S., & Yağcıoğlu, S. (2016). Neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurological impairments in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder, their healthy siblings and healthy controls: Identifying potential endophenotype(s). Psychiatry Research, 240, 110–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.013
  • Özçelik-Eroğlu, E. (2012). The influence of clozapine on diffusion tensor imaging measures in patients with Schizophrenia [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Özçelik-Eroğlu, E., Ertuğrul, A., Oguz, K. K., Has, A. C., Karahan, S., & Yazıcı, M. K. (2014). Effect of clozapine on white matter integrity in patients with schizophrenia: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging, 223(3), 226–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.06.001
  • Özçelik Eroğlu, E., Ertuğrul, A., Oğuz, K. K., Karahan, S., Yazıcı, M. K. (2020). Effect of Clozapine on Proton Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy Findings in Hippocampus. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 31(3), 159–167. https://doi.org/10.5080/u25195
  • Özdemir, S. (2015). Assesment of semantic and action fluency skills among school age children ranging in Age from 15 to 17 and adults above 18 years Age [ PhD thesis]. Anadolu University.
  • Özdemir, S., & Tunçer, A. (2021, 1). Verbal fluency: An investigation of time variable among elderly people. Clinical & Experimental Health Sciences, 11(1), 133–139. https://doi.org/10.33808/clinexphealthsci.727477
  • Özden, H. C. (2022). Investigation of the relationship between impulse control, emotion regulation, cognitive flexibility and magical ideation in healthy controls and major depression patients [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Ozer Celik, A., Kurt, P., Yener, G., Alkin, T., Oztura, I., & Baklan, B. (2015). Comparison of Cognitive Impairment between Patients having Epilepsy and Psychogenic Nonepileptic Seizures. Noro Psikiyatr Ars., 52(2), 163–168. https://doi.org/10.5152/npa.2015.7290
  • Özkul, Ç., Güçlü-Gunduz, A., Eldemir, K., Apaydin, Y., Gülsen, Ç., Yazici, G., Söke, F., & Irkec, C. (2021). Dual-task cost and related clinical features in patients with multiple sclerosis. Motor Control, 25(2), 211–233. https://doi.org/10.1123/mc.2020-0035
  • Ozkul, C., Guclu-Gunduz, A., Eldemir, K., Apaydin, Y., Yazici, G., & Irkec, C. (2020). Clinical features and physical performance in multiple sclerosis patients with and without cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 43(4), 316–323. https://doi.org/10.1097/MRR.0000000000000428
  • Oz Didem, O. I. Y., Kiyi, I., & Yener, G. G. (2022). Alzheimer hastalığı klinik ölçütlerine göre demans ve hafif kognitif bozukluk ayrımında sözel akıcılık testlerinin mini mental durum testi ile birlikte kullanılması yararlı olabilirVerbal fluency tests together with mini mental state examination may be useful in differentiating between dementia and mild cognitive impairment stages according to Alzheimer’s disease clinical criteria. Cukurova Medical Journal, 47(4), 1403–1411. https://doi.org/10.17826/cumj.1179620
  • Paap, K. R., Myuz, H. A., Anders, R. T., Bockelman, M. F., Mikulinsky, R., & Sawi, O. M. (2017). No compelling evidence for a bilingual advantage in switching or that frequent language switching reduces switch cost. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(2), 89–112. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2016.1248436
  • Patra, A., Bose, A., & Marinis, T. (2020). Performance difference in verbal fluency in bilingual and monolingual speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 204–218. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918001098
  • Patterson, J. (2011). Verbal Fluency. In J. S. Kreutzer, J. DeLuca, & B. Caplan (Eds.), Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 2603–2606). Springer: New York. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-79948-3_1423.
  • Paula, F., Wilkens, R., Idiart, M., & Villavicencio, A. (2018). Similarity measures for the detection of clinical conditions with verbal fluency tasks. Proceedings of NAACL-HLT 2018 (pp. 231–235).
  • Pekkala, S., Albert, M. L., Spiro, A., & Erkinjuntti, T. (2008). Perseveration in Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 25(2), 109–114. https://doi.org/10.1159/000112476
  • Piatt, A. L., Fields, J. A., Paolo, A. M., Koller, W. C., Tröster, A. I., Piatt, A. L., & Fields, J. A. (2010). Lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson ’ s disease with and without dementia lexical, semantic, and action verbal fluency in Parkinson ’ s disease with and without dementia *. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 3395. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.21.4.435.885
  • Quaranta, D., Piccininni, C., Caprara, A., Malandrino, A., Gainotti, G., & Marra, C. (2019). Semantic relations in a categorical verbal fluency test: An exploratory investigation in mild cognitive impairment. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2797. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02797
  • Raboutet, C., Sauzéon, H., Corsini, M.-M., Rodrigues, J., Langevin, S., & N’kaoua, B. (2010). Performance on a semantic verbal fluency task across time: Dissociation between clustering, switching, and categorical exploitation processes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(3), 268–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390902984464
  • Raskin, S. A., & Rearick, E. (1996). Verbal fluency in individuals with mild traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 10(3), 416–422. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.10.3.416
  • Raskin, S. A., Sliwinski, M., & Borod, J. C. (1992). Clustering strategies on tasks of verbal fluency in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 30(1), 95–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(92)90018-H
  • Richardson, W. S., Wilson, M. C., Nishikawa, J., & Hayward, R. S. (1995). The well-built clinical question: A key to evidence-based decisions. ACP Journal Club, 123(3), 7–9. https://doi.org/10.7326/ACPJC-1995-123-3-A12
  • Robert, P. H., Lafont, V., Medecin, I., Berthet, L., Thauby, S., Baudu, C., & Darcourt, G. U. Y. (1998). Clustering and switching strategies in verbal fluency tasks: Comparison between schizophrenics and healthy adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(6), 539–546. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798466025
  • Rodríguez-Aranda, C., & Martinussen, M. (2006). Age-related differences in performance of phonemic verbal fluency measured by Controlled Oral Word Association Task (COWAT): A meta-analytic study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(2), 697–717. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn3002_3
  • Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Salvatierra, J., Marquez, M., Luis, M., & Weekes, V. A. (2002). A cross-linguistic comparison of verbal fluency tests. International Journal of Neuroscience, 112(6), 759–776. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450290025752
  • Sahin, M. C. (2023). Cognitive functions and posture analysis of university students who play video games, Athletes and Musicians [ Master’s thesis]. Bursa Uludağ University.
  • Sahin, R. (2022). Evaluation of executive functions and attention in epilepsy patients with multimethod measurement techniques [ PhD thesis]. Akdeniz University.
  • Sandoval, T. C., Gollan, T. H., Ferreira, V. S., & Salmon, D. P. (2010). What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency? The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 231–252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990514
  • Sandson, J., & Albert, M. L. (1987). Perseveration in behavioral neurology. Neurology, 37(11), 1736–36. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.37.11.1736
  • Sen, S. (2014). The relationship between cortical thickness and cognitive status in patients with essential tremor [ PhD thesisi]. OnDokuz Mayıs University.
  • Senol, G. (2017). The relationship between impulse control, emotional and cognitive regulation mechanisms and heart rate variability [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Sen Ozdemir, Ö. (2020). Evaluation of cognitive functions in neuromyelitis optica spectrum cases [ PhD thesis]. Ege University.
  • Şentürk, T. (2019). Turkish normative data of semantic and phonemic verbal fluency tests [ PhD thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Şevik, A., Emre, Anıl Yağcıoğlu, A., Elif, Yağcıoğlu, S., Karahan, S., Gürses, N., & Yıldız, M. (2011). Neuropsychological performance and auditory event related potentials in schizophrenia patients and their siblings: A family study. Schizophrenia Research, 130(1–3), 195–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.04.018
  • Sezikli, S. (2014). Comparison of frontal lob cognitive functions of JME patients who have asymmetrical Eeg results and those who have not [ PhD thesis]. Mashar Osman Ruh Sağlığı ve Sinir Hastalıklari Eğitim ve Araştırma Hastanesi.
  • Sezikli, S., Argün Pulat, T., Tekin, B., Dogan Ak, P., Keskinkiliç, C., & Atakli, D. (2018). Frontal lobe cognitive functions and electroencephalographic features in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 86, 102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.009
  • Sogutlu, L., & Alaca, N. 2019. The Relationship Between Subjective Memory Complaints and Objective Memory Performance, Depression and Anxiety Levels in Patients Under 55 Years of Age. Turkish Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.5080/u23084
  • Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary. American chemical society.
  • Sumiyoshi, C., Ertuğrul, A., Yağcıoğlu, A. E. A., Roy, A., Jayathilake, K., Milby, A., Meltzer, H. Y., & Sumiyoshi, T. (2014). Language-dependent performance on the letter fluency task in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 152(2–3), 421–429. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.12.009
  • Talas, M. S. (2009). Examination of the relationships between verbal fluency, magical ideation and motor asymmetry in healthy young adults [ PhD thesis]. Ankara University.
  • Temel, M. (2020). Determination of cognitive profile according to lesion localization in patients with thalamic hemorrhage [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Tombaugh, T. N. (1999). Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14(2), 167–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(97)00095-4
  • Topçuoğlu, Ö. B., Kavas, M., Alibaş H., Afşar, G. Ç., Arınç, S., Midi, İ., Elmacı, N. T. (2018). Executive functions in sarcoidosis: a neurocognitive assessment study. Sarcoidosis Vasculitis and Diffuse Lung Diseases, 35(1), 26–34. https://doi.org/10.36141/svdld.v35i1.5940
  • Töret, Z. (2019). The comparison of the effects of gesture use on the lexical access of adults with visual impairments and sighted adults [ PhD thesis]. Gazi University.
  • Töret, Z., & Özdemir, S. (2021). A comparison of the effects of gesture use on the lexical access process of adults with visual impairments and sighted adults. Ankara Üniversitesi Eğitim Bilimleri Fakültesi Özel Eğitim Dergisi, 22(2), 395–417. https://doi.org/10.21565/ozelegitimdergisi.696281
  • Troyer, A. K. (2000). Normative data for clustering and switching on verbal fluency tasks. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(3), 370–378. https://doi.org/10.1076/1380-3395(200006)22:3;1-V;FT370
  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138
  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M. P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal-and temporal-Lobe Lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36(6), 499–504. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00152-8
  • Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Leach, L., & Freedman, M. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency tests in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4(2), 137–143. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617798001374
  • TUIK, Turkish Statistical Institute. (2022). World Population Day, 2022. https://data.tuik.gov.tr/Bulten/Index?p=Dunya-Nufus-Gunu-2022-45552
  • Tükel, R., Gürvit, H., Özata, B., Öztürk, N., Ertekin, B. A., Ertekin, E., Baran, B., Kalem, Ş. A., Büyükgök, D., & Direskeneli, G, S. ( 2012). Brain–derived neurotrophic factor gene Val66Met polymorphism and cognitive function in obsessive–compulsive disorder. American Journal of Medial Genetics Pt B, 159B (7), 850–858. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32092
  • Tumaç, A. (1997). Effects of age and education to performance in some frontal lobe tests in normal subjects [ Master’s thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Tuncer, A. M. (2012). Verbal fluency performance of Turkish speaking adults [ PhD thesis]. Anadolu University.
  • Tuncer, N., Mıdı, İ., & Feyzioğlu, A. (2012). Cognitive Impairment In Relapsing Remitting Multiple Sclerosis Patients. Journal of Neurological Sciences, 29(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.12.027
  • Turkish Ministry of Foreign Affairs, Turkish. (2022). Turkish citizens living abroad. https://www.mfa.gov.tr/the-expatriate-turkish-citizens.en.mfa
  • Tutan, A. (2019). Evaluation of anxiety, alexithymia, trauma and cognitive functions of chronic tinnitus patients [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Ulasoglu, Ç. (2010). The Korsakoff amnestic syndrome as a stimulus generalization deficit [ Master’s thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Ünlü, C., Yapıcı, N., İzgi, F. C., Kudisoğlu, T., Ünlü, C., & Aykaç, Z. (2013). Effects of N-acetylcysteine on neurocognitive functions after coronary artery bypass graft surgery. Turkish Journal of Thoracic and Cardiovascular Surgery, 21(2), 364–370. https://doi.org/10.5606/tgkdc.dergisi.2013.6802
  • Uslu, E. (2012). The investigation of bipolar subtypes which have been proposed to have different genetic etiology with respect to neurocognitive functions and temperamental traits: a controlled study [ PhD thesis]. Hacettepe University.
  • Uslu, S. (2018). The evaluation of hemodynamic changes in the prefrontal cortex of bipolar depressive and major depressive disorders with near-infrared spectroscopy method [ Master’s thesis]. Akdeniz University.
  • Uslu, S. C. (2015). Comparison Of Drug Responsive And Pharmacoresistant Mtle Patient Interms of Memory and Attention Function [ PhD thesis]. University of Health Sciences.
  • Uzgan, B. Ö., Oktay, M. T., Aykaç, C., Ermiş, Ç., & Alkın, T. (2021). Neurocognitive flexibility, perfectionism, obsessive beliefs in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 24(4), 439–449. https://doi.org/10.5505/kpd.2021.90187
  • Van Der Elst, W. I. M., Van Boxtel, M. P., Van Breukelen, G. J., & Jolles, J. (2006). Normative data for the Animal, profession and letter M naming verbal fluency tests for Dutch speaking participants and the effects of age, education, and sex. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(1), 80–89. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060115
  • Vilkki, J., & Holst, P. (1994). Speed and flexibility on word fluency tasks after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 32(10), 1257–1262. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90107-4
  • Voppel, A. E., de Boer, J. N., Brederoo, S. G., Schnack, H. G., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2021). Quantified language connectedness in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Research, 304, 114130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2021.114130
  • Weiss, E. M., Daniel Ragland, J., Brensinger, C. M., Bilker, W. B., Deisenhammer, E. A., & Delazer, M. (2006). Sex differences in clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4), 502–509. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060656
  • Woods, D. L., Wyma, J. M., Herron, T. J., & Yund, E. W. (2016). Computerized analysis of verbal fluency: Normative data and the effects of computerized analysis of verbal fluency: Normative data and the effects of repeated testing, simulated malingering, and traumatic brain injury. PLoS ONE, 11(12), e0166439. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0166439
  • Yavuz-Demiray, D. (2011). Assessment of cognitive impairment and depression in patients with essential tremor, patients with essential tremor and Parkinson’s disease concomitance and parkinson’s disease [ Master’s thesis]. Istanbul University.
  • Yazici, M. (2019). Neuropsychological evaluation of the population of eastern and Southeastern anatolia on executive functions and complex attention [ Master’s thesis]. Ondokuz Mayis University.
  • Yazıhan N Torun and Yetkin S. (2020). Sleep, sleep spindles, and cognitive functions in drug-naive patients with first-episode psychosis. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 16(12), 2079–2087. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.8776
  • Yeniçeri, F. E. (2019). The effect of balance exercises with cognitive task on motor and cognitive functions in healthy young adults [ PhD thesis]. Medipol University.
  • Yildirim, E. (2012). Investigation of decision making and impuls control disorders in parkinson’s disease patients [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Yıldırım, E., & Ogel-Balaban, H. (2021). Cognitive functions among healthy older adults using online social networking. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 30(4), 401–408. https://doi.org/10.1080/23279095.2021.1951269
  • Yilmaz, T. (2014). Comparison of adults diagnosed with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder with healthy controls on neurocognitive functions, impulsivity and theory of mind [ PhD thesis]. Maltepe University.
  • Yılmaz, N. H., Çalışoğlu, P., Güntekin, B., & Hanoğlu, L. (2020). Correlation between alpha activity and neuropsychometric tests in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience Letters, 738, 135346.
  • Zagli, N. (2011). The relation between psychosis like experiences and cognitive functions in a population based sample [ Master’s thesis]. Dokuz Eylül University.
  • Zakzanis, K. K., Mcdonald, K., Troyer, A. K., Mcdonald, K., Zakzanis, K. K., Troyer, A. K., Troyer, A. K., Zakzanis, K. K., & Mcdonald, K. (2011). Component analysis of verbal fluency in patients with mild traumatic brain injury component analysis of verbal fluency in patients with mild traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(7), 785–792. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.558496
  • Zhao, Q., Guo, Q., & Hong, Z. (2013). Clustering and switching during a semantic verbal fluency test contribute to differential diagnosis of cognitive impairment. Neuroscience Bulletin, 29(1), 75–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12264-013-1301-7