Publication Cover
New Genetics and Society
Critical Studies of Contemporary Biosciences
Volume 28, 2009 - Issue 4
357
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Opinion survey of the Hong Kong general public regarding genomic science and technology and their ethical and social implications

, , , &
Pages 381-400 | Published online: 17 Nov 2009

Abstract

The study's objective is to survey the attitudes of Chinese people living in Hong Kong toward genomic science and technology (GST) and their ethical and social implications. Using a 24-item questionnaire, 877 Cantonese-speaking residents between age 18 and 64 with minimum high school education are interviewed by telephone. Multiple regression analysis identifies education level as the most important demographic variable. Overall, respondents have mild agreement with genetic determinism and the use of GST for disease prevention but not for non-therapeutic genetic enhancement and production of “genetically modified” crops or meat. Respondents strongly believe that GST tampers with nature and resources should be used to solve other healthcare problems first. Respondents also show little concern that personal genetic information may be abused by their employers or schools and have only a minimal willingness to share personal genetic information with their family members.

Introduction

Since the completion of the Human Genome Project and successful cloning of Dolly and other animals within the past decade, people have become more familiar with the rapid development of genomic science and technology (GST). At the same time, people have also become aware of the ethical, legal and social implications (ELSI) that GST has for society, including health and environmental threats of genetically modified (GM) crops, food animals and microbes, informed consent procedures in genetic testing, genetic discrimination, confidentiality of genetic information (GI), uses and abuses of GI, etc. This has led to many public surveys in Western countries to assess the general public's attitudes towards GST (Gaskell et al. Citation2003, Sturgis et al. Citation2004). Cross-cultural studies comparing different attitudes in different countries have also been conducted in New Zealand, Australia, Japan, India and Israel (Macer et al. Citation1997, Citation2000, Ikeda Citation2008), but studies about the Chinese people are rare. The few published studies in Mainland China, Taiwan and Singapore are focused on agricultural biotechnology (Subrahmanyan and Cheng Citation2000, Li et al. Citation2002, Chen and Li Citation2007), and in Hong Kong, only a telephone survey of public attitudes towards GM food and labeling is available (http://www.greenpeace.org.hk). Little is known about how Hong Kong people perceive the development of GST and its potential risks. The purpose of this study is to increase our understanding of the Hong Kong public's attitude towards GST and the ELSI. This is not only crucial for the development of GST in this community which has extensive international collaborative networks, but the study will also make a significant contribution to the GST literature regarding Chinese cultural factors that may facilitate or deter the development of GST in a Chinese community since over 95% of the 8 million people living in Hong Kong are ethnically Chinese.

Material and methods

(a) Telephone survey of the general public

The Research Centre of University of Hong Kong (HKU) was commissioned to conduct the telephone survey targeting Cantonese-speaking residents between age 18 and 64 with at least high school education as respondents (hereafter as “General Public” and “Phone Survey”), using a survey questionnaire containing 24 questions in addition to demographic information. Household telephone numbers were randomly sampled and only one qualified person from each residence was interviewed. A total of 1545 eligible households were contacted with 877 (56.8%) surveys completed. The maximum sampling error is±3.3%, at 95% level of confidence (α = 0.05). The phone interviews were conducted on the basis of anonymity and confidentiality and the study was approved by the local Institutional Review Board.

(b) Data management and statistical analysis

All responses are summarized by descriptive statistics. Factor analysis, derived by principal component factor analysis with varimax rotation, is performed on the 24 questions, supplemented by manual adjustment to improve the integrity of subject content of each theme, with identification of five independent themes from 17 questions: (A) Use of GST to prevent disease; (B) Human genetic enhancement; (C) Genetic modification of food and micro-organisms; (D) GST and negative reproductive implications; (E) Abusive use of genetic information. Internal reliability of themes is evaluated by Cronbach's alpha coefficient which ranges from 0.73 to 0.78 (). Each of the remaining seven ungrouped questions is each treated as an individual theme. Mean scores of each theme are given from 1 to 5 based on a five-point Likert scale with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree, and “neutral” set at 3. Demographic characteristics are given in numbers and percentages. Multiple regression modeling is used to determine statistically significant demographic variable(s) in association with responses. ANOVA is also used to evaluate differences within each demographic variable, followed by Scheffe's test or Fisher's LSD test to explore any pair-wise difference(s) between different “sub-groups” within the variable. Statistical significance is set at p < 0.05. All the statistical procedures are carried out with SPSS Version 15.0 for Windows.

Table 1. Grouping of different kinds of dialing responses in Phone Survey.

Table 2. Demographic characteristics of “General Public” (n = 870).

Table 3. Mean scores of themes and its related questions from “General Population”.

Results

Demographic characteristics of respondents

The “General Public” consists of 877 respondents in the Phone Survey. Male and female respondents are almost equal and 51% of respondents are between 25 and 44 years old. Respondents' mean age is about 34 years; 54% respondents have high school or matriculation and approximate 40% have university education; 66% are employed and about 70% have no religious beliefs (see ).

Multiple regression modeling of themes

Multiple regression modeling with stepwise approach is applied to various themes to evaluate the significant demographic variables(s). Stepwise model-building techniques with a single dependent variable are described in numerous sources (see e.g. Darlington Citation1990, Morrison Citation1990, Hocking Citation1996). Respondents' education level is the demographic variable that is significantly associated with the largest number of themes. It affects positively the agreement scores of themes D, E and H (the regression coefficients (β) are given by 0.23, 0.18 and 0.23 respectively; see ) and negatively to themes B, G, I and J (β = –0.16, –0.34, –0.14 and –0.14 respectively; see ). This shows that respondents with higher education tend to have higher agreement scores in the first group of themes but lower agreement scores in the second group of themes. Age and religious affiliation also affect six themes each. Age has a positive regression coefficient in themes B and I (β = 0.10 and 0.21, respectively) but a negative coefficient in themes C, D, G and K (β = –0.11 for both themes C and D, –0.25 and –0.15 for themes G and K respectively). This suggests that older respondents tend to have a higher agreement in themes B and I but have a lower agreement in themes C, D, G and K. The different religions within the “religion” variable are listed in the following order: (1) “Non-religious”; (2) Christianity; (3) Buddhism. Respondents with no religious affiliation have higher agreement scores in themes A and B (β = –0.15 and –0.16 respectively) and lower agreement scores in themes H, J, K and L (β = 0.14, 0.16, 0.19 and 0.25 respectively). Gender is listed as (1) male; and (2) female, and it affects the responses of three themes. Male respondents have higher agreement scores than female respondents in theme C (β = –0.17) but have lower agreement score in themes J and K (β = 0.14 and 0.17 respectively). Occupation only affects theme G and “Pre-U students” are listed as first in the list () and they have lower agreement score in theme G (β = 0.13).

Table 4. The results of multiple regression of various themes.

Attitudes towards the use of genomic science and technologies

(A) Use of GST to prevent disease

Respondents have an overall mild agreement for this theme (mean: 3.21) (see ), with about 40% having “agree” scores (combining “agree” and “strongly agree”), and approximately 35% having “neutral” scores (see and ). Religion is the only demographic variable with a statistically significant association with response (p = 0.014) with non-religious respondents having the highest mean score (3.26), followed by Christians (3.09) and Buddhists (3.04) (see ); this is also confirmed by the regression model (see ) that shows the endorsement to use GST to prevent disease is weakened by religious beliefs (p < 0.01). By ANOVA, there is no difference among “Non U-Employees”, “U-Employees” and “U-Students” (), indicating that they have fairly similar responses, providing mild support for use of GST for disease prevention, particularly for child disability, fatal genetic diseases, birth defects and mental retardation; support is attenuated by religious beliefs and if GST is used to predict chronic diseases such as cancer, heart disease and dementia.

Figure 1. The distribution of responses (in %) of all questions in various themes.

Figure 1. The distribution of responses (in %) of all questions in various themes.

Table 5. Different responses (in %) for various themes (A to L) by telephone respondents.

(B) Human genetic enhancement

Respondents have strong disagreement with use of GST for human genetic enhancement such as creating more intelligent or successful children (mean: 2.05) (see ), with approximately 70% disagreeing and  < 10% agreeing (see and ). The regression model shows that age (p < 0.05), education (p < 0.01) and religious belief (p < 0.01) are significant variables for this theme (see ). By ANOVA, respondents between age 25 and 44 have stronger disagreement (2.00) than age 45 to 64 (2.24) (p = 0.015; see ); university-educated respondents have stronger disagreement (1.93) than respondents with high school and matriculation education (2.16) and Christians (1.85) have stronger disagreement than non-religious respondents (2.12) (p = 0.001; see ). Significant difference is not seen between “U-Students” and other occupation groups, but is seen between “NonU-Employees” (2.20) and “U-Employees” (1.96) (p = 0.013; see ).

Table 6A. Comparison of mean score by gender, age and education of “General Public” in Phone Survey.

Table 6B. Comparison of mean score by occupation and religious belief of “General Public” in Phone Survey.

(C) Genetic modification (GM) of food and other organisms

Respondents have an overall mild disagreement with this theme (mean: 2.56) with about 50% disagreeing and 30% remaining “neutral”. There is stronger disagreement in respect of GM meat (2.34) than crops (2.83) (see ). The regression model shows that gender (p < 0.01) and age (p < 0.05) are significant demographic variables (see ), with female respondents (2.47) having stronger disagreement than male (2.65), and older respondents (age 45 to 64) having stronger disagreement than younger respondents (18 to 24). ANOVA shows a significant mean difference between different occupation groups (p = 0.012), with significant pair-wise difference seen between “Pre-U Students” and “Unemployed” respondents but not among “U-Students”, “Non U-Employees” and “U-Employees”. The responses of the latter three groups are found in a narrow range between 2.53 and 2.58, suggesting very similar “disagree” responses to GM food.

(D) GST and negative reproductive implications

Respondents mildly disagree (mean: 2.91) with the claim that GST has negative reproductive implications related to abortion (see ). The number of respondents “agreeing” (32.3%) and “disagreeing” (35.9%) is roughly equal ( and ). The regression model shows that age (p < 0.05) and education (p < 0.001) are significant demographic variables, with older respondents tending to have stronger disagreement than younger respondents (β for age = –0.11; see ), and less-educated respondents tending to have stronger disagreement (β for education = 0.23). ANOVA shows significant mean difference between different occupation groups (p < 0.001), with pair-wise differences between “Non U-Employees” (2.78) and “U-Employees” (3.07) and between “U-Employees” and “Unemployed” (2.71) (see ).

(E) Abuses of genetic information

Respondents have an overall mild disagreement (mean = 2.68) with potential abuse of genetic information by insurance companies, employers and schools (see ), with about 28% “agreeing” and 47% “disagreeing” ( and ). The regression model shows that education (p < 0.01) is the only significant demographic variable. ANOVA confirms that the less-educated respondents would have stronger disagreement (2.58) than university-educated respondents (2.79) (p = 0.002; see ). Among different occupational groups, there are significant pair-wise differences between “U-Students” (2.84) and “Non U-Employees” (2.54), and between “Non U-Employees” and “U-Employees” (2.76) (p = 0.023; see ). This suggests that university education is associated with higher suspicion of GI abuses.

(F) Genetic determinism

Respondents have an overall mild agreement (mean: 3.23) that health and disease are determined by genes (see ) with about 50% scoring “neutral”, 33% agreeing and 17% disagreeing ( and ). Responses are not affected by demographic variables (see , 6A and 6B).

(G) GST reduces personal responsibility for health

Respondents have an overall weak disagreement (mean: 2.87) with this theme (see ), with approximately 40% disagreeing, 30% agreeing and 30% neutral ( and ). The regression model shows that age (p < 0.001), education (p < 0.001) and occupation (p < 0.01) are significant demographic variables. Older respondents tend to have stronger disagreement than younger respondents (β for age = –0.25; see ) and less-educated respondents tend to have higher agreement than more educated (β for education = –0.34; see ). From ANOVA, the response is also affected by gender (p = 0.031), education (p < 0.001) and occupation (p = 0.002), with female (2.95) and less-educated respondents (2.99) having weaker disagreement than male (2.78) or university-educated respondents (2.78) (see and ). Comparisons among occupation groups are significant (p = 0.002) with “U-Employees” having stronger disagreement than “Non U-Employees”.

(H) Society cannot guarantee strictly fair use of genetic information

Respondents have an overall agreement (mean: 3.40) that it is impossible for society to guarantee the strictly fair use of genetic information (see ) with approximately 47% agreeing and 20% disagreeing ( and ). The regression model shows that education (p < 0.001) and religion (p < 0.05) are significant demographic variables. ANOVA shows postgraduate respondents (4.00) having stronger agreement than university-educated (3.46) and high school-educated (3.30) respondents (β for education = 0.23; see ), and “U-Employees” (3.57) having stronger agreement than “Non U-Employees” (3.27) (p = 0.011) (see and ).

(I) Sharing genetic information (GI) among family members

Respondents have an overall agreement (mean: 3.32) that people have responsibility and obligation to share GI with family members (see ), with approximately 45% agreeing and 20% disagreeing ( and ). The regression model shows that age (p < 0.001) and education (p < 0.05) are significant variables; older respondents tend to have stronger agreement (β for age = 0.21; see ) and less-educated respondents have higher agreement scores (β for education = –0.14, see ). ANOVA shows differences among different occupation groups (p = 0.039) with significant pair-wise difference between “Pre-U Students” (3.22) and “Unemployed” (3.57), between “U-Students” (3.18) and “Unemployed”, and between “U-Employees (3.24) and “Unemployed” (), indicating that “Unemployed” respondents (consisting of housewives and retirees) have the strongest agreement for this theme.

(J) Limited healthcare resources and GST

Respondents have an overall strong agreement (mean: 3.63) that limited healthcare resources should be used to solve other healthcare problems first rather than to develop GST (see ), with approximately 53% agreeing and 10% disagreeing ( and ). The regression model shows that gender (p < 0.05), education (p < 0.05) and religion (p < 0.05) are significant variables. Therefore, less-educated respondents would have higher agreement score than other education groups (β for education = –0.14; see ). T-test shows that female respondents have stronger agreement than male (3.70 and 3.55 respectively, p = 0.025; see ). Less-educated respondents have stronger agreement than postgraduate respondents (3.69 and 3.37 respectively, p = 0.042; see ). ANOVA shows differences in response due to gender, education, religion and occupation (p = 0.025, 0.042, 0.021 and 0.036 respectively; see and ). Female respondents have stronger agreement than male (3.70 and 3.55 respectively); significant pair-wise differences are seen between “high school” (3.69) and “postgraduate” respondents (3.37), and between “non-religious” (3.57) and “Christian” (3.77) respondents, suggesting that better educated and non-religious respondents have weaker agreement with this theme. Among different occupational groups, pair-wise differences are found between “Pre-U Students” (3.51) and “Unemployed” (3.80), between “Non U-Employees” (3.70) and “U-Employees” (3.52) and between “U-Employees” and “Unemployed”. This suggests that “Unemployed” respondents and “Non U-Employees” have higher agreement for this theme and “Pre-U Students” and “U-Employees” have lower agreement.

(K) GST and tampering with nature

Respondents have a strong overall agreement (mean: 4.00) that GST ultimately tampers with nature (), with approximately 72% agreeing and 8% disagreeing ( and ). The regression model shows that gender (p < 0.05), age (p < 0.01) and religion (p < 0.01) are significant variables. Younger respondents would have higher agreement scores (β for age =−0.15; see ) than the other two age groups. ANOVA shows that female respondents have stronger agreement than male (4.09 and 3.92 respectively, p = 0.021); younger respondents from 18 to 24 (4.09) and 25 to 44 (4.05) have stronger agreement than older respondents from 45 to 64 (3.78, p = 0.007); and Christians have stronger agreement than non-religious respondents (4.21 and 3.94 respectively, p = 0.006) ( and ). There is no significant difference among occupational groups.

(L) GST should be prohibited before regulatory mechanisms are in place

Respondents have a mild overall agreement (mean: 3.30) with this theme (see ). There are approximately 39% agreeing, 21% disagreeing and 40% staying “neutral” ( and ). The regression model shows that religion (p < 0.001) is the only demographic variable that affects response. ANOVA shows that the difference is between Christians (3.47) and non-religious respondents (3.24) ().

Discussion

Dissemination and abuses of genetic information

The proliferation and management of genetic information (GI) is one of the most important ramifications of GST, which has far-reaching ethical, legal and social implications. In this study, respondents agree that it is impossible for society to guarantee strict fairness when GI is used. On the other hand, excepting insurance companies, respondents disagree that employers or schools would abuse GI. Less-educated respondents have stronger disagreement than university-educated respondents and this is consistent with findings in the West that better-educated people are more suspicious of abuses of GI (Human Genetics Commission Citation2001) Furthermore, respondents show no concern that personal GI stored in databanks may be accessed by third parties without permission. In the West, people are much more suspicious of potential abuses of personal GI and this possibility has been used as an important argument to either ban employers, insurance companies and government agencies from accessing GI (Gaskell et al. Citation2003) or ban genetic testing altogether (Hietala et al. Citation1995). Further studies are needed to investigate why our respondents have so much confidence in schools, employers and databanks.

If management of GI in the public domain is a difficult problem, the dissemination of GI within a family is equally challenging. When a person has inherited disease-causing genes, it implies that the mutated genes can potentially be found in other family members or passed on to the next generation. In the present study, respondents have a mild agreement that people have responsibility and obligation to share GI with their family members, with older (between age 45 and 64) and less-educated respondents having the highest agreement and university students being the least enthusiastic. This is consistent with an earlier finding that >50% of Mainland Chinese would disclose GI to relatives at risk (Mao and Wertz Citation1997). In the West, findings are also quite inconsistent, with some findings affirming family members' right to know and patients' willingness to share GI (Julian-Reynier et al. Citation1996, Lehmann et al. Citation2000), while others showing a general reluctance to share GI except with one's spouse and children (Henneman et al. Citation2004). The right to know and the duty to inform increases if at-risk family members are part of a nuclear family or are threatened by a preventable disease (Pentz et al. Citation2005); willingness to share decreases if the disease is non-preventable and the at-risk person is a distant biologic relative (Peterson et al. Citation2003). Hence, sharing GI with family members in the West entails maintaining a delicate balance between several competing values: between a family member's “need to know” and the patient's right to privacy and confidentiality; between members of a nuclear family and extended family; and between “family-owned GI”, e.g. information of a gene mutation running in the family and “individual-owned GI”, e.g. an individual's own genetic test result (Pentz et al. Citation2005). If we consider the importance and dominance of the family in Chinese culture, these considerations seem to be less relevant. In a Chinese context, even members of the extended family would be entitled to own and therefore to know GI related to the family, as long as they share the genetic (blood) ties. From this perspective, the findings of the present study may not be representative of a general Chinese response because they reflect a relatively weak endorsement for sharing GI among family members. This is probably related to the relatively high education level of our respondents (60% high school graduates and 40% university graduates) and in Hong Kong, to be well educated implies strong exposure to Western influences that tend to “dilute” traditional Confucian sentiments. This is confirmed by our findings that university students in our sample have the weakest agreement to sharing GI with family members. More research is needed to explore the factors that may influence sharing GI with family members in Hong Kong since it is expected that more families will be confronted with genetic risk information in the future.

Genetic determinism, genetic testing and negative reproductive implications

Genetic testing presupposes a degree of genetic determinism and in our study, respondents have a mild agreement that diseases are genetically determined. Many lay people mistakenly believe that genes are rigidly deterministic, when in fact most genes are only mildly deterministic because of developmental and environmental factors that affect gene expressions, as proved by identical twin studies. The fact that 50% of respondents remain “neutral” in this question suggests considerable uncertainty and ambivalence, and this may also be the reason why their support for use of GST in disease prevention is very reserved. Support is highest for the purpose of preventing serious diseases and lowest for predicting risks of chronic diseases. This is in contrast to the widespread enthusiasm Westerners have for genetic testing of a wide spectrum of human diseases ranging from severe genetic diseases to psychiatric conditions and common disorders (Hietala et al. Citation1995, Jallinoja et al. Citation1998, Milner et al. Citation1998, Iredale and Longley Citation2000). A low view of genetics in disease causation and prevention held by the respondents probably explains why they also disagree that GST is related to people feeling less responsible for their own health. At the same time, respondents have an overall neutral score regarding certain negative reproductive implications of GST including an increase in unjustified abortions. This reflects respondents' general ambivalence towards the medical application of GST. Interpreters of public surveys tend to explain ambivalent attitudes as a reflection of the respondents' incomplete comprehension or inadequacies of the questionnaire. While this may be true in some cases, more commonly it is due to the very nature of the technology (GST in this case) being very complex and offering both advantages and disadvantages. Ambivalent responses then reflect the respondents' efforts to balance pros and cons of GST and account for the simultaneous coexistence of approval and support of certain aspects of GST, and doubt and disapproval of other aspects of GST (Furu et al. Citation1993, Jallinoja et al. Citation1998). The ambivalent attitudes of our respondents likely reflect the same predicament.

The ambivalence towards GST is even more manifested in respondents' disagreement with genetic modification (GM) of food. This finding is consistent with the findings of a government survey in 2000 that >50% of Hong Kong residents would not buy GM food (FEHD Citation2001), a 2004 Hong Kong Greenpeace survey that >75.0% of respondents rejected food containing GM ingredients (Greenpeace Citation2004) and findings from Mainland Chinese studies that GM foods are generally not accepted by Chinese consumers (Zhang Citation2002, Wang Citation2003). In this regard, Chinese consumers' attitudes are closer to European consumers' strongly negative attitudes than US consumers' more accepting attitudes (Gaskell et al. Citation1999). Our study also shows a trend that GM crops are slightly more acceptable than GM meats, probably because the former are perceived to be safer (Magnusson and Koivisto Hursti Citation2002). In contrast to GM food, respondents show a mild acceptance for the transfer of human DNA to microorganisms for medicinal production. This reflects the Chinese people's general pragmatic approach in decision-making since GM medicine is seen as more beneficial than GM food. A Taiwanese study also concludes that Chinese consumers are more positive about using GST to produce medicine than food (Chen and Li Citation2007). Chinese people are well known for their obsession with eating and food, and their concern with GM food probably reflects the cultural idiosyncrasies about food as well as a general reservation regarding GST.

Genetic enhancement and tampering with nature

Given the general reserved attitude towards GST, it is not surprising to find our respondents strongly rejecting the use of GST for human enhancement. GST can serve the purpose of therapy and enhancement: genetic therapy attempts to prevent or treat diseases by correcting the genes with harmful mutations and genetic enhancement attempts to alter the genetic material to improve non-pathological physical and behavioral characteristics. Despite the argument that it may be impossible to distinguish between the two (Miller Citation1994, Marteau et al. Citation1995), there is more support for genetic therapy than enhancement in the West based mainly on safety considerations (Frewer et al. Citation1997, Kerr et al. Citation1998, Milner et al. Citation1998, Calnan et al. Citation2005). But our study indicates that there may be another important underlying reason for Hong Kong people's conservatism regarding GST in general and rejection of genetic enhancement in particular. Our respondents hold the strong belief that GST ultimately tampers with nature. This corroborates the finding of another Mainland Chinese study that GM food threatens the natural order of things and is fundamentally unnatural (Lu Citation2006). This sentiment is not unique among Chinese since some consumers in the West, particularly Christians, also feel that GM foods are unnatural (Hill et al. Citation1998) and eating GM foods or taking gene tests gives rise to a sense of guilt and immorality for disturbing the order of nature (Hietala et al. Citation1995, Magnusson Citation2002). The reason why the concept of tampering with nature is shared by the culturally diverse Westerners and Chinese people is probably because they share a common worldview of a “natural order”: for Westerners the order of nature is ordained by the Christian God, and for the Chinese prescribed by Dao (path) – a concept shared by the two most influential philosophical/religious schools in China: Confucianism and Daoism.

The concern that GST tampers with nature may also affect public policy related to future development of GST. Respondents in this study agree that limited healthcare resources should be used to solve other healthcare problems in Hong Kong rather than developing GST and that GST should be prohibited before strict regulatory and oversight mechanisms are in place. Between the two stereotypes of either “genetic optimism” or “discourse of concern” towards a new technology such as GST (Durant et al. 1996, Conrad Citation2001), how people approach it is largely influenced by beliefs and attitudes (Catz et al. Citation2005). Until one's inherited beliefs are reconciled with the scientific merits of GST, people remain conservative or even skeptical about the innovation. For this reason, in order to improve people's attitude towards GST, education programs must be designed to tailor-fit the ethnic, cultural and linguistic contexts of the local community for them to be effective.

Conclusion

The present study of a sample of the general public of Hong Kong reveals a strong conservative attitude in this community about GST. Respondents have a limited agreement to the use of GST for prevention of serious inheritable diseases, but they disagree with most of the other applications including genetic testing for chronic diseases, production of GM foods and genetic enhancement of natural traits. They are also concerned about the negative implications of GST, particularly about consumers' increased desire for “designer babies” and choices for abortion. They believe that limited resources are better spent to provide other needed medical services than developing GST. It is possible that the overall negative response of our respondents may have been influenced by the fact that about half of our questions are cast in a “negative” tone; but more probably, this conservative attitude is related to the Chinese belief that GST basically tampers with nature. With regard to management of GI, our respondents show little concern about potential abuses by employers or schools, although they distrust insurance companies. Respondents are lukewarm in their willingness to share GI with other family members, and university students are even less willing to commit themselves; these responses contradict the strong Confucian familism that underpins the cultural ethos of the local community and requires further studies for their clarification.

Acknowledgement

The authors wish to thank Professor Chad Hansen of the Department of Philosophy, University of Hong Kong for his contribution in the conception phase of this project. This study was supported in part by a grant from the University Strategic Research Theme: Genomics, Proteomics, Bioinformatics & ELSI, The University of Hong Kong, grant No.10206152.11222.21700.302.01. The authors declare no conflict of interest of any kind.

References

  • Calnan, M., Montaner, D., and Horne, R., 2005. How acceptable are innovative health-care technologies? A survey of public beliefs and attitudes in England and Wales, Social Science & Medicine 60 (9) (2005), pp. 1937–1948.
  • Catz, D. S., et al., 2005. Attitudes about genetics in underserved, culturally diverse populations, Community Genetics 8 (3) (2005), pp. 161–172.
  • Chen, M., and Li, H., 2007. The consumer's attitude toward genetically modified foods in Taiwan, Food Quality and Preference 18 (2007), pp. 662–674.
  • Conrad, P., 2001. Genetic optimism: framing genes and mental illness in the news, Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 25 (2001), pp. 225–247.
  • Darlington, R. B., 1990. Regression and linear models. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1990.
  • Durant, J., Hansen, A., and Bauer, M., 1999. Public understanding of the new genetics. In: T. Marteau and M. Richards, eds. The troubled helix: Social and psychological implications of the new human genetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 235–248..
  • FEHD (Food and Environmental Hygiene Department), 2001. Report on food safety survey (2001), [online]. Available from: http://www.fehd.gov.hk/publications/foodsurvey/foodsurvey.PDF [Accessed 6 January 2009].
  • Frewer, L. J., Howards, C., and Shepherd, R., 1997. Public concerns in the United Kingdom about general and specific applications of genetic engineering: risk, benefits and ethics, Science, Technology and Human Values 22 (1997), pp. 98–124.
  • Furu, T., et al., 1993. Attitudes towards prenatal diagnosis and selective abortion among patients with retinitis pigmentosa or choroideremia as well as among their relatives, Clinical Genetics 43 (3) (1993), pp. 160–165.
  • Gaskell, G., et al., 1999. Worlds apart? The reception of genetically modified foods in Europe and the US, Science 285 (1999), pp. 384–387.
  • Gaskell, G., Allum, N., and Stares, S., 2003. Europeans and biotechnology in 2002: Eurobarometer 58.0 (2003), A report to the EC Directorate General for research from the project “Life Sciences in European Society” [online]. Available from: http://web.archive.org/web/20070613013746/http://www.europa.en.int/comm/public_opinion/archives/eb/ebs_177_en.pdf [Accessed 6 January 2009].
  • Greenpeace, 2004. Opinion survey on public attitudes toward genetically modified food (2004), [online]. Available from: http://www.greenpeace.org/raw/content/china/ch/press/reports/119199.pdf [Accessed 6 January 2009].
  • Henneman, L., Timmermans, D. R., and van der Wal, G., 2004. Public experiences, knowledge and expectations about medical genetics and the use of genetic information, Community Genetics 7 (1) (2004), pp. 33–43.
  • Hietala, M., et al., 1995. Attitudes toward genetic testing among the general population and relatives of patients with a severe genetic disease: a survey from Finland, American Journal of Human Genetics 56 (1995), pp. 1493–1500.
  • Hill, R., et al., 1998. Reactions to a new technology: students' ideas about genetically engineered foodstuffs, Research in Science & Technological Education 16 (1998), pp. 203–216.
  • Human Genetics Commission, 2001. Public attitudes to human genetic information: people's panel quantitative study conducted for the Human Genetics Commission. London: Human Genetics Commission; 2001.
  • Hocking, R. R., 1996. Methods and applications of linear models: regression and the analysis of variance. New York: Wiley; 1996.
  • Ikeda, W., 2008. The public's attitudes towards the use of genetic information for medical purposes and its related factors in Japan, Community Genetics 11 (2008), pp. 18–25.
  • Iredale, R., and Longley, M. J., 2000. Reflections on citizen's juries: the case of the citizen's jury on genetic testing for common disorders, Journal of Consumer Studies and Home Economics 24 (2000), pp. 41–47.
  • Jallinoja, P., et al., 1998. Attitudes towards genetic testing: analysis of contradictions, Social Science & Medicine 46 (1998), pp. 1367–1374.
  • Julian-Reynier, C., et al., 1996. Attitudes towards cancer predictive testing and transmission of information to the family, Journal of Medical Genetics 33 (9) (1996), pp. 731–736.
  • Kerr, A., Cunningham-Barley, J., and Amos, D., 1998. Drawing the line: analysis of lay people's discussions about the new genetics, Public Understanding of Science 7 (1) (1998), pp. 41–60.
  • Lehmann, L. S., et al., 2000. Disclosure of familial genetic information: perceptions of the duty to inform, American Journal of Medicine 109 (9) (2000), pp. 705–711.
  • Li, Q., et al., 2002. Consumer attitudes toward genetically modified foods in Beijing, China, AgBioForum 5 (4) (2002), pp. 145–152.
  • Lu, L., 2006. Chinese public understanding of the use of agricultural biotechnology – a case study from Zhejiang Province of China, Journal of Zhejiang University Science B 7 (4) (2006), pp. 257–266.
  • Macer, D. R.J., et al., 1997. Attitudes to biotechnology in Japan and New Zealand in 1997, Eubios Journal of Asian and International Bioethics 7 (1997), pp. 137–151.
  • Macer, D. R.J., Azariah, J., and Srinives, P., 2000. Attitudes to biotechnology in Asia, International Journal of Biotechnology 2 (4) (2000), pp. 313–332.
  • Magnusson, M. K., and Koivisto Hursti, U. K., 2002. Consumer attitudes towards genetically modified foods, Appetite 39 (1) (2002), pp. 9–24.
  • Mao, X., and Wertz, D. C., 1997. China's genetic services providers' attitudes towards several ethical issues: a cross-cultural study, Clinical Genetics 52 (2) (1997), pp. 100–109.
  • Marteau, T., et al., 1995. Public attitudes towards the selection of desirable characteristics in children, Journal of Medical Genetics 32 (10) (1995), pp. 796–798.
  • Miller, H. I., 1994. Gene therapy for enhancement, Lancet 334 (1994), pp. 316–317.
  • Milner, K. K., et al., 1998. Attitudes of young adults to prenatal screening and genetic correction for human attributes and psychiatric conditions, American Journal of Medical Genetics 76 (2) (1998), pp. 111–119.
  • Morrison, D. F., 1990. Multivariate statistical methods. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1990.
  • Pentz, R. D., et al., 2005. Hereditary nonpolyposis colorectal cancer family members' perceptions about the duty to inform and health professionals' role in disseminating genetic information, Genetic Testing 9 (3) (2005), pp. 261–268.
  • Peterson, S. K., et al., 2003. How families communicate about HNPCC genetic testing: findings from a qualitative study, American Journal of Medical Genetics 119C (2003), pp. 78–86.
  • Sturgis, P., et al., 2004. "Genomic science: emerging public opinion". In: Curtice, J., et al., eds. British social attitudes: the 21st report. London: Sage; 2004. pp. 119–145.
  • Subrahmanyan, S., and Cheng, P. S., 2000. Perceptions and attitudes of Singaporeans toward genetically modified food, Journal of Consumer Affairs 34 (2) (2000), pp. 269–290.
  • Wang, Z., 2003. Knowledge of food safety and consumption decision: an empirical study on consumer in Tianjing, China, China Rural Economy 4 (2003), pp. 41–48.
  • Zhang, X., 2002. "Chinese consumers' concerns over food safety [online]". 2002, Working Paper, Agricultural Economics Institute, The Hague, The Netherlands. Available from: http://mailman.greenpeace.org/mailman/listinfo/press-releases [Accessed 6 January 2009].

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.