Publication Cover
New Genetics and Society
Critical Studies of Contemporary Biosciences
Volume 43, 2024 - Issue 1
73
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Are we ready for the genomic era? Insights from judges and lawyers

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Article: e2367210 | Received 17 Jul 2023, Accepted 29 May 2024, Published online: 07 Jul 2024

Abstract

Genetic advances have brought new opportunities to society, with new powers of polygenic prediction, genetic engineering and gene-based environmental interventions. Judges and lawyers influence interpretations and attitudes towards complex societal issues and develop regulation. Therefore, their genetic literacy and views are an important part of society’s readiness for the genomic era. The study explored judges’ and lawyers’ (N = 117) genetic literacy, as well as views on the use of genetic advances. Quantitative and qualitative analyses showed insufficient knowledge of essential genetic concepts, including striking cases of over- and under-estimation of genetic and environmental influences on behavior. Participants’ views on every issue were widely varied, from strong agreement to strong disagreement. The majority of the participants thought that current laws are not sufficient to protect individuals from misuses of genetic data. The results suggest that society is not ready for the genomic era and call for multidisciplinary efforts to increase the readiness.

1. Introduction

Genomic advances have brought enormous potential benefits but also risks for individuals and societies. This is because applications of genomic advances stretch across many aspects of life, including health, sports, insurance, education, and criminal justice; as well as from pre-birth to future generations. Progressing towards societal readiness for successful applications of genomic advances relies on knowledge and views of key stakeholders, including judges and lawyers.

Judges and lawyers are guardians in the process of implementation and regulation of scientific advances. As key justice stakeholders, they have the power and duty to protect people from potential injustices associated with the use of new technologies. Genetics is an area where sophisticated regulation of current and future advances is paramount – as genetic applications have many unaddressed practical, ethical, philosophical and other societal implications.

For example, existing laws are not designed to protect from missuses of information extracted from sequenced DNA data (Andrews, Mehlman, and Rothstein Citation2015; Furrow et al. Citation2013; Selita Citation2019b). Information extracted from sequenced DNA can be used for polygenic prediction, such as estimating one’s health risk or educational potential. This prediction can be used for disease prevention and personalized education, but also brings risks of discriminatory use (Andrews, Mehlman, and Rothstein Citation2015; Selita Citation2019b). Another area of concern is gene editing, including the germline. It brings great promise for new treatments, but it also presents risks for irreversible damage to an organism, population genetic pool, and societal organization (Bosley et al. Citation2015; Kovas and Selita Citation2021; Raposo, Citation2019). Addressing such issues, including via sophisticated legal regulation, require informed deliberations of the relevant stakeholders.

A growing body of research has identified genetic literacy – sufficient understanding of the key genetic concepts – as a prerequisite to the ability to assess and appreciate risks associated with the use of genetic advances. Genetic literacy is also foundational for the drafting of legislation and legal procedures that can prevent harm. Several studies to date explored genetic literacy of judges and lawyers, as well as their views on applications of genetic advances. The current paper provides an overview of this literature from the limited number of explored jurisdictions (US, Germany, Romania); and extends this research in another jurisdiction – Russia.

In 2012 and 2015, Aspinwall et al. (181 judges in the US) and Fuss et al. (372 judges in Germany), using the same vignette assessed the extent to which biological evidence concerning psychopathy mitigated, aggravated, or had no effect on the punishment judges would issue (Aspinwall, Brown, and Tabery Citation2012; Fuss, Dressing, and Briken Citation2015). The results showed that within the same jurisdiction judges viewed the biological evidence as mitigating and aggravating. Cross-national differences also emerged: the presentation of genetic information led to a shorter sentence in the US sample, but to an increased likelihood of involuntary commitment in a forensic psychiatric hospital in Germany. The results indirectly indicate that judges do not have sufficient genetic knowledge, and also that genetic literacy interacts with societal setups and general attitudes (e.g. misconceptions about immutability of traits; and assumed purpose of the prison sentence – punishment, keeping offender off the streets, deterrence). In 2016, Berryessa CM, using a sample of 21 US judges, examined how judges would react if they were told or were presented evidence that an offender has a mental disorder with genetic influences (Berryessa Citation2016). Different judges viewed the same evidence as mitigating or aggravating, some were sceptical about such evidence and some judges expressed reliance on experts interpreting such evidence. These results also indirectly indicate lack of relevant genetic knowledge.

In 2019, Berryessa CM reported results of a study where 59 US judges were asked to describe their thoughts about sentencing if an offender’s mental disorder was known to be genetically influenced (Berryessa Citation2019). The study reported that a knowledge of genetic aetiology of a mental disorder was associated with more restrictive sentence, likely driven by essentialist beliefs and stigmatization biases. These results, again, indirectly suggested a lack of nuanced understanding of genetics, such as importance of gene-environment interplay. A further paper in 2019 provided indirect evidence for lack of understanding of contemporary genetics among judges. The paper examined paternity cases in UK and Australian courts in relation to judges’ “biotruth” rhetoric, whereby genetic parentage is viewed as a simple scientific reality (Robert Citation2019). The author discusses how such rhetoric conflates precision of genetic identification with establishing legal parentage, child’s identity and reflecting child’s interests. Such simplistic and reductionist approach contrasts with findings from contemporary genetics of complex gene-gene and gene-environment interplay affecting phenotypic expression.

Another study highlighted concerns about lack of legal stakeholders’ understanding of genetics and related perceptions of genetic science as a “truth machine” (Amelung, Granja, and Machado Citation2020). The paper presented analyses of 9 forensic geneticists’ evaluation of the views on genetics by publics-in-particular – active members of the criminal justice system, including judges, lawyers and police force. These views were described as often overly enthusiastic and unrealistic in regards to applications of DNA technologies in criminal cases. Another study in 2019 explored views on the use of genetic advances of an opportunistic sample of lawyers and law students from different countries (Selita, Chapman, and Kovas Citation2019). The results showed strong endorsement for the State using genetic information on propensity for violence for prevention of crime; and for considering genetic information in sentencing. Such strong endorsement of controversial uses is again an indication of lack of genetic literacy. Higher genetic literacy would include understanding of limitations of DNA analyses, potential risks and unintended consequences of overstating DNA evidence, and misuses that may stem from predictive use of probabilistic information.

In 2019, a study directly assessed genetic literacy of an opportunistic sample of 5405 participants from different countries (Chapman et al. Citation2019). The analyses by profession indicated that genetic knowledge of lawyers (N = 80) was similarly low to that of representatives of other professions. In 2020, Selita et al. examined genetic literacy of 73 Supreme Court judges and 94 lawyers in Romania, using a 17-item survey (Selita et al. Citation2020). The results showed that judges’ and lawyers’ knowledge of genetics was inconsistent: some items, that could be answered using general reasoning, were answered correctly by ∼90% of the judges (e.g. the approximate number of genes in the genome). Other items, requiring knowledge of basic genetic concepts, were answered correctly by only ∼24% of the judges. The Romanian judges and lawyers also shared their views on the use of genetic information in different life contexts, and on application of genetic advances (Selita et al. Citation2023). The results showed a high agreement among judges and other lawyers endorsing the use of gene editing for prevention and treatment of disease; as well as for other purposes (“improving self or children”). The judges and lawyers also reported strong endorsement for use of genetic information by the State for crime prevention, but much weaker endorsement for the use of genetic information by insurers, schools or employers.

Overall, the research to date uncovered important limitations in legal stakeholders’ genetic knowledge. Possessing some knowledge may provide a false sense of security and form a basis for misinformed decisions. Legal practitioners themselves acknowledge the need for improved genetic literacy. For example, judges in the Romanian study almost unanimously welcomed training in gene-environment processes for judges (Selita et al. Citation2020). The research to date also suggested specific patterns in legal stakeholders’ endorsement of different applications of genetic advances. Further research is needed to understand what these results mean for our genomic readiness.

As genetics has no borders, further research is also needed to examine whether these patterns of results can be replicated in other jurisdictions. Establishing universality vs. specificity of the results is important, as developing appropriate genetic regulation is a global concern. For example, gene editing has cross-generational implications and has the power to affect the future of humankind. Effective regulation of germline editing in just a few countries would not protect from potential harm, if other jurisdictions were to allow such a practice. Moreover, different jurisdictions have different legal reasoning, application of laws and method of developing laws. Therefore, it is important to see whether views match or differ across jurisdictions. Cross-national research may also help uncover factors that influence knowledge and views on these matters.

1.1. The present study

This study explores genetic literacy and opinions of a representative group of judges and lawyers from Russia. The study uses largely the same measures as those used in the two previous studies with Romanian participants (Selita et al. Citation2020, Citation2023). This allows for a systematic exploration of whether the same pattern of results can be observed in a different jurisdiction. Based on the pattern of results found with Romanian legal professionals, we explored whether the Russian legal professionals would show:

  1. Uneven knowledge across different genetic concepts.

  2. High endorsement of gene editing for medical purposes, for use of genetic information by the State for prevention of crime and for using genetic information to adapt environments to people's needs; and low endorsement for allowing insurance companies to access clients’ genetic information.

  3. High agreement that laws in place are insufficient to protect individuals from misuses of genetic information; and in recognizing the urgent need for updating relevant laws and procedure; as well as for training of judges in gene-environment processes.

2. Materials and methods

The study was approved by the Goldsmiths Department of Psychology Ethics Committee (PSY10.10.2016). Informed consent was obtained before the beginning of data collection.

2.1. Participants

The 102 participants were 56 judges (commercial court, N = 36; and common court, N = 20); and 46 lawyers (27 advocates and 19 prosecutors). Participants were recruited by personal invitation by academics and advocates – Russian members of the international Working Group on Legal, Ethical and Societal Implications of Genetics (LESIG, 2016-2020). Participation rate of judges was 90.3% (62 judges were approached, 56 provided full data). Participation rate of lawyers was 92% (50 lawyers were approached, 46 provided full data). Participants returned completed questionnaires in-person. Therefore, no demographic information (age, sex or gender) was collected to preserve anonymity. The number of responses varied for different analyses, as participants could skip any items they did not wish to answer.

2.2. Measures and procedure

This study used 30 items in total. Twenty nine items were drawn from the International Genetic Literacy and Attitudes Survey (iGLAS) (Chapman et al. Citation2017). The iGLAS was first created and validated in English (Chapman et al. Citation2017), and has been adapted to 9 other languages. The adaptations were created following established procedures (Fenn, Tan, and George Citation2020). This included forward translation by 2 translators working separately; backwards translations by 2 translators working separately; followed by checks by a committee of experts – members of the iGLAS team. More than 14,000 participants from different countries completed the survey. Detailed information on validation, construction, translation and use of iGLAS can be found in a previous publication (Chapman et al. Citation2017). In the current study, we used the Russian adaptation of iGLAS – the first language of the participants. Additionally, one item (item 5, ) was developed specifically for this study by the iGLAS research team.

Table 1. Frequencies (N participants and proportions) of correct responses for each of the multiple-choice options for the 5 genetic knowledge items.

2.2.1. Judges’ and lawyers’ genetic literacy

Judges’ and lawyers’ genetic knowledge was explored with 18 iGLAS items and 1 additional item (item 5 in ). Five knowledge items required participants to select a response from the provided list: 3 items had yes/no or true/false responses; 2 items had 1 correct and 3 incorrect response options (see ). These responses were combined into an overall knowledge score. The remaining 14 knowledge items examined participants’ knowledge of heritability for 14 complex traits (see ). Heritability expresses the extent to which individual differences in a particular trait in a particular population are due to people’s genetic differences (e.g. Polderman et al. Citation2015; Selita and Kovas Citation2018). Participants were asked to provide responses to: “On a scale of 0–100 how important are genetic differences between people in explaining individual differences in the following traits”.

Table 2. Participants’ estimates of heritability (genetic influence) for 14 complex traits.

2.2.2. Judges’ and lawyers’ views on genetics

Judges’ and lawyers’ views were explored with 11 iGLAS items, of which 10 were presented on 7-point Likert scales from 1 (strongly disagree), through 4 (neither agree nor disagree), to 7 (strongly agree); and 1 item required participants to choose 1 of 4 responses. For the present paper, views are grouped into the following two themes ( and Figure 2): views on gene editing and use of genetic information (4 items); and views on updating key genomics-related laws and on the need for training of judges (7 items).

Table 3. Means, Standard Deviations and Frequency responses for the 10 opinion items presented on Likert scales.

2.2.3. Supplementary report from another sample of Russian lawyers

The report contains results from a seminar with 15 lawyers and judges, including practitioners from a number of areas and Law School academics – organized as part of the LESIG Working Group’s activities. The session lasted 2.5 h. During the session, participants completed questionnaires, received information (2 presentations on genetics and law), and engaged in discussions. The discussions were recorded with permission from participants and then transcribed, preserving full anonymity of the participants.

Fourteen of the 15 participants also completed questionnaire booklets at the beginning of the session. The questions were taken from the earlier versions of iGLAS, including 16 genetic knowledge items, of which 4 overlapped with the ones used in the main analyses of this paper. The participants also provided heritability estimates of 8 traits; as well as responses to 4 vignettes. Following this, participants discussed the answers to the knowledge questions and related issues; these data were qualitatively analysed (see SOM).

3. Results

First, we compared responses of judges and lawyers on each of the 30 items. Inferential analyses revealed no significant differences between judges and lawyers in variability or average values for 29 out of the 30 items. The exception was a small but significant (t (75.73) = 2.48, p = .016) difference in heritability estimate of sexual orientation: judges’ estimate was lower (M = 29.63, SD = 28.81) than that of lawyers (M = 37.38, SD = 24.20). As the groups did not otherwise differ significantly, we re-ran analyses on a combined sample of judges and lawyers. The following sections describe the results from this combined sample.

3.1. Genetic knowledge

First, we calculated total genetic knowledge for each participant by summing their correct responses for the 5 questions (possible score range 0–5). These scores were then converted into an average correct score (raw genetic knowledge score, divided by 5, multiplied by 100). The average knowledge score was relatively low at 2.56 (or 51.18% correct). As can be seen from the frequencies in , only 24.8% of the participants knew the correct definition of a genome; and only 19.4% of the participants knew that the DNA sequence in two different cells is identical. Most of the participants (64–79%), however, answered correctly that we cannot fully predict a person’s behavior from examining their DNA, that most traits are influenced by genes, and that in Russia new born infants are tested for genetic mutations.

Next, we examined participants’ knowledge of heritability for 14 complex traits. The results, presented in , show a wide variability in participants’ responses for each trait, with substantial proportions of participants estimating 0 or 100% heritability (see ).

Figure 1. Proportion of participants responding that traits are either 0% or 100% heritable.

Figure 1. Proportion of participants responding that traits are either 0% or 100% heritable.

3.2. Opinions on the use of genetic information and regulation of genomic advances

We assessed views on several key matters, grouped into: (1) views on use of gene editing and use of genetic information; and (2) updating of relevant laws, procedure and training of judges. The results for the 10 Likert scale opinion items are presented in . For all items, a large variability was observed, with the whole range of response options (1–7) used. To simplify the discussion in this section, we created two groups of responses: “Disagree” – combining scores 1–3 (strongly disagree, disagree, and somewhat disagree); and “Agree” – combining scores 5–7 (somewhat agree, agree, and strongly agree).

3.2.1. Views on gene editing and use of genetic information

Around 60% of participants agreed with each of the following uses of genetic advances: that gene editing should be used for prevention and treatment of disease (Item 1, ); that genetic information should be used to adapt environments to people's needs, e.g. individualized health advice (Item 2, ); and that the State should use genetic information about aggressiveness for prevention of crime (Item 4, ). In contrast, only 25.7% of participants agreed that insurance companies should be allowed access to genetic data prior to issuing health and/or life insurance (Item 3, ).

3.2.2. Views on updating the relevant laws and training

Only around 20% of the participants felt that current laws are sufficient to protect individuals from misuses of genetic data by insurance companies, employers, and schools (Items 5–7 in ). As to when the relevant laws should be amended, the two most common answers were “now (ASAP)” (35.8%); and “after we are certain of the scale of the risk” (39.6%) ().

Figure 2. Frequency and proportion responses to “when the relevant laws should be amended”.

Note: The question asked: From a DNA sample taken at birth we already can predict, with a degree of probability, future behavior (such as school performance). The precision of prediction is continuously increasing. Moreover, sequencing is already routinely conducted for medical research and other purposes. When should relevant laws (e.g. data protection, privacy, non-discrimination, insurance, employment) be updated/introduced accordingly?

Figure 2. Frequency and proportion responses to “when the relevant laws should be amended”.Note: The question asked: From a DNA sample taken at birth we already can predict, with a degree of probability, future behavior (such as school performance). The precision of prediction is continuously increasing. Moreover, sequencing is already routinely conducted for medical research and other purposes. When should relevant laws (e.g. data protection, privacy, non-discrimination, insurance, employment) be updated/introduced accordingly?

Participants were divided in their views, with substantial proportions agreeing, disagreeing and neither agreeing nor disagreeing, on whether the legal system should accommodate the large variability among the people, including in terms of ability, personality and level of education (Item 9, ); and on whether information about gene-environment processes should be included in judges’ training (Item 10, ).

3.3. Results from the seminar (Supplementary report)

The quantitative analyses of the data on genetic knowledge and views of 14 lawyers indicated overall low genetic knowledge in this group, with similarly uneven knowledge across items as shown in the main study. For example, similar to the main analyses in this study, only about 20% of the seminar participants selected a correct response for “DNA in different cells is identical”. The detailed results are reported in SOM.

Qualitative analyses of participants’ comments and discussions, reported in SOM, produced the following two broad conclusions: (1) The lawyers and judges thought that genetic information is difficult and they did not possess the necessary genetic knowledge; and that they would like to know more in this field; and (2) The lawyers and judges are aware of multiple legal issues associated with genetic advances, including: data misuse and eugenics; ownership of genetic data; gene editing; national security; lack of legal protection and of uniform enforcement of laws.

4. Discussion

In this study we collected data from a representative sample of judges and lawyers in the Russian Federation – extending previous research into genomic era readiness with judges and lawyers in other jurisdictions.

Comparing the results with those reported in two recent papers exploring knowledge and views of Romanian judges and lawyers (Selita et al. Citation2020; Citation2023), suggested some potentially universal as well as country-specific trends. For example, in both Russia and Romania, the genetic literacy of judges and lawyers was relatively low; the majority of participants endorsed gene editing for medical purposes, as well as the use of genetic information for adapting environments to people' needs and for crime prevention. At the same time most participants disagreed with use of genetic information by insurers. In addition, in both countries, the majority of the judges and lawyers thought that current laws are not sufficient to protect individuals from misuses of genetic data in insurance, education and employment contexts; and that relevant laws need to be amended. However, the views of the Russian judges and lawyers were overall more varied than those of Romanian judges and lawyers. In the following sections we provide a detailed discussion of the results.

4.1. Genetic knowledge

The knowledge of genetic concepts and heritability tested is essential to understanding risks and benefits associated with use of genetic data. The results showed that the average knowledge of judges and lawyers was relatively low and not evenly distributed across the items. The 5 multiple-choice knowledge items had either 2 or 4 response options, with 35% being chance level. This means that the average number of correct answers (53.94% for judges and 47.83% for lawyers) was close to chance level. Similarly low genetic knowledge was observed in the second group of Russian judges and lawyers (reported in SOM). Most participants chose incorrect answers to the questions about DNA: what is a genome; and DNA sequence in two different cells. In contrast, most participants gave correct answers to the other questions: that new borns undergo genetic testing; that most traits are influenced by genes; and that behavior cannot be fully predicted from examining DNA.

For the 14 heritability items, mean estimates for the traits ranged from about 30% to 70%, suggesting that most participants thought that these traits are moderately to substantially influenced by genetic factors. These means are consistent with the pattern of heritability estimates obtained in scientific studies (e.g. Polderman et al. Citation2015), although some means were somewhat lower than established heritability estimates (eye color, height, and especially weight). The underestimation of weight heritability is consistent with results from Romanian judges and lawyers (Selita et al. Citation2020), and with an unselected international sample (Chapman et al. Citation2019). This underestimation of weight heritability may stem from the fact that weight is more malleable than some other traits (e.g. height), and is viewed as being under greater individual control, which is erroneously equated with absence of genetic effects.

Further examination of the results showed that the relatively accurate means for most traits masked the great variability in responses across individuals, ranging from 0% to 100% for most traits. For example, approximately 9% of the participants erroneously believed that ADHD is 100% environmental; and approximately 6% of the participants believed it was 100% genetic. Similarly, 13.5% of participants felt that IQ was fully determined by genes, with no environmental influences. These results are consistent with those from previous studies with judges and lawyers, as well as general population, which also showed wide variability in responses (Chapman et al. Citation2019; Selita et al. Citation2020).

The gaps in genetic knowledge shown by many of the judges and lawyers in this study may have practical consequences. For example, if lawyers do not know that genetic testing on new borns is carried out in the country, they will not see a need for developing measures to protect children’s genetic data. Such protection, however, is necessary, considering that breaches of children’s genetic data have already been reported (Bailey et al. Citation2019; Supreme Court of Minnesota Citation2011a). Similarly, the lack of understanding of what a genome is, and that DNA is identical in different cells, may lead to underestimation of the risks posed by sequencing of the genome. Once a person’s DNA from any cell is sequenced, it presents a life-long wealth of information about the person, with risks for discrimination across life aspects (Selita Citation2019b).

Incorrect knowledge of heritability of traits may also interfere with justice as it is erroneously associated by many people with malleability of traits (“more heritable – less malleable” fallacy). Previous research found that people’s estimates of heritability of traits were linked to people’s views and attitudes more broadly. For example, people tend to think (erroneously) that, when treating more heritable traits, medication is more effective than psychological interventions (Madrid-Valero et al. Citation2021; Pope et al., Citationin press). In the justice context, under- and over-estimation of genetic and environmental effects on behavior – may serve as foundation for a coherent but erroneous pattern of views regarding blame, determinism, immutability, high risk of recidivism, etc.

4.2. Opinions of judges and lawyers on use of genetic information and regulation of genomic advances

4.2.1. Views on gene editing and use of genetic information

Participants’ views were varied, with substantial proportions of participants agreeing, disagreeing and undecided on gene editing for prevention and treatment of disease; using genetic information to adapt environments to people's needs; and the State using genetic information for crime prevention. Around 60% of participants endorsed such use.

This lack of agreement among legal professionals from the same jurisdiction reflects the complexity of decisions in this area, particularly given the many issues that are yet to be resolved including: technical (e.g. specificity of effects in gene editing); practical (e.g. accessibility); and ethical (e.g. effects on future generations, human rights violations) (Godard et al. Citation2003; Government Office for Science Citation2022a; Sperber et al. Citation2017). For example, although the use of gene editing in medicine is viewed by many positively, gene engineering to enhance human capacity more broadly is much more controversial (Government Office for Science Citation2022b). Similarly, although some benefits (e.g. personalized medication or screening regimes) are obvious, gene-based treatments may be prohibitively expensive, which may further exacerbate existing inequalities in access to medical care (Naddaf Citation2022). Moreover, advice and treatments based on probabilistic polygenic information may be inaccurate and even harmful; and some uses of genetic information may be linked to discrimination, such as gene-based streaming in education or job selection.

Similarly, the use of genetic information for crime prevention can lead to violations of individuals’ rights. For example, the State could take actions such as surveillance, compulsory education or therapy programmes – based on people’s polygenic information. However, such use is not warranted by the current state of knowledge: it is not known how many genetic markers for aggression exist; whether the same markers operate in different populations; and how many “risk” variants one should have before one is classified “high risk”. Moreover, genetic markers interact with each other and with environments – making predictions probabilistic and requiring many pieces of information to be put into the equation. It is therefore clear that gene-based crime prevention is an ethical minefield, requiring extensive evaluation and wider discussion.

Participants’ views on whether insurance companies should be allowed access to genetic data prior to issuing health and/or life insurance were less varied – with almost 75% disagreeing with such use. This result suggests that judges and lawyers recognize injustices associated with insurance premiums being determined on genetic information – potentially making insurance inaccessible for people with high polygenic risks. However, simply denying insurance companies access to genetic information may not be a viable option for societies in the genomic era, primarily because the insurance industry relies on monies collected from the healthy to cover the costs of those less healthy (Pugh Citation2021; Rodriguez-Rincon et al. Citation2022; Selita et al. Citation2023; UK Department of Health Citation2022). Therefore, society needs to evaluate and implement alternative solutions, such as different models of State insurance provision (Chapman et al. Citation2020; Selita Citation2020).

4.2.2. Views on updating the relevant laws and need for training

We assessed judges’ and lawyers’ views on whether, when and which genetics related laws should be updated; and if they believe that judges should receive some training in genetics.

Around 56% of the judges and lawyers thought that current laws in Russia are insufficient to protect individuals from misuses of genetic advances in insurance, education and employment. The results of content analysis from the Seminar (reported in SOM) identified specific genetics related concerns, including misuse of genetic data/eugenics, ownership of genetic data, gene editing, national security, lack of legal protection and uniform enforcement of laws. Indeed, pre-genomic era laws are not sufficient in the genomic era, where DNA sequencing is becoming routine. Similarly, the use of data resulting from whole genome sequencing is not effectively covered by new specific laws passed in some jurisdictions, such as the U.S. (Andrews, Mehlman, and Rothstein Citation2015; Furrow et al. Citation2013; Selita Citation2019b).

As to when the relevant laws should be amended, ∼30% of the participants responded that the relevant laws (e.g. data protection, privacy, non-discrimination, insurance, employment) should be updated “now (ASAP)” and ∼20% – that the laws should be updated “after some cases in these areas have been brought to courts”. Interestingly, ∼40% thought that the laws should be amended “after we are certain of the scale of the risk”. However, calculating with certainty the scale of harm from misuses of predictive information of genetic data is not possible. This is because continuous genetic advances mean that an individual's DNA sequence, once extracted, can become a source of increasing amount of information in the course of the person's life across different life aspects – from education, to insurance, to health, to employment, to influences on decisions (Selita Citation2019b).

In relation to how laws should be amended, 74% of the participants agreed that breaches of genetic data should be made a criminal offence, although it was not clear whether participants endorsed a fine only or also imprisonment. The fact that the majority of the participants endorsed criminalization of genetic data breaches may reflect their awareness that breaches of genetic data have life-long and intergenerational consequences for individuals and their families. At present, in many jurisdictions (e.g. Russia, UK), genetic data receives the same protection as other sensitive personal data, such as medical data – with no custodial sentence or power of arrest e.g. (Data Protection Act 2018, Citationn.d.; General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) Citation2016; The Crown Prosecution Service Citation2018). Offences are punishable only by a fine, often small (ICO Citation2006; Citation2018; Supreme Court of Minnesota Citation2011a; WiredGov Citation2018). This may not be sufficient to deter data breaches, which are common (e.g. Cyentia Institute Citation2016; Lintern Citation2021; Merrick Citation2017; Selita Citation2019b; Supreme Court of Minnesota Citation2011b).

The views of participants on whether the legal system should accommodate the population variability were split, with approximately one third – agreeing; one third – disagreeing and one third – neither agreeing nor disagreeing. This split in opinions may reflect the uncertainty about how exactly the individual differences can be accommodated in the legal system (Selita Citation2018). Some accommodations have already been implemented in some jurisdictions, for example providing lawyers with detailed guidelines on accommodating people’s learning disabilities and other personal difficulties (Bowden, Douds, and Simpson Citation2011; UK Government, Citationn.d.). Similar provisions will have to take place in regard to accommodating individual differences more broadly, for example by reducing legal jargon; and making the legal system more accessible in terms of cost and more representative of the population it represents (Civil Justice Council Citation2011; Moorhead and Sefton Citation2005; Selita Citation2019a).

Finally, we explored judges’ and lawyers’ views on whether training of judges should include fundamentals of gene-environment processes involved in individual differences in behavior. Around 50% agreed and around 35% disagreed. We performed additional analyses to test whether those participants who think that existing laws are sufficient are also more likely to think that training in gene-environment processes is not needed. The results of correlational and ANOVA analyses (available from the authors) revealed no significant association between the two variables.

It is possible that endorsement for gene-environment training depends on the area of practice. For example, many participants in the present study were commercial court judges. For them, information on gene-environment processes may seem too far removed to be relevant, as they rarely need to judge people’s behavior in their day-to-day work. Indeed, previous research found much greater endorsement for such training in a sample of Romanian Supreme Court judges whose work includes criminal cases where knowledge about individual differences may be particularly relevant (Selita et al. Citation2020)

In the present study we further explored potential reasons for not endorsing gene-environment training by analysing responses of the Seminar participants (reported in SOM). Participants commented that the field of genetics “is highly complex” and “technical” and that they have “never considered genetics”. Some also expressed that they do not know anything about genes”, but this “is nothing to be ashamed of – as the field is complex and new”. In light of these complexities, participants may feel that one would need a prohibitively long time to acquire the necessary knowledge.

However, the danger of not including genetic knowledge in lawyers’ training is that without such knowledge they are not able to identify and protect against the risks of misuses of genetic information. Without training, understanding of genetic concepts is often superficial and misses the ethical complexities; potential consequences; details of how technologies (e.g. gene editing) work; and how such technologies can be used (O’Keefe et al. Citation2015). For example, various metaphors and simple descriptions used for gene editing techniques present them in a way that may lead to wrong decisions across the public, political, and scientific sectors (O’Keefe et al. Citation2015). The nuanced presentation of information must capture the limitations of the current knowledge and challenges of using predictive polygenic information; as well as dispel some of the deterministic myths, and warn against over-optimistic views of genetic information as bio-truth (Robert Citation2019). Evidence suggests that incorporating modules on gene-environment processes in medical and social sciences degrees, such as psychology, may lead to less biased views (Jacko et al. Citation2023; Jamieson and Radick Citation2017).

5. Conclusions and future directions

The study found overall low genetic literacy and wide variability in views on genetics among legal professionals in the same jurisdiction. Further research is needed to understand the reasons and practical implications of this variability, as well as to clarify the sources of differences found across jurisdictions. For example, the agreement among Russian judges and lawyers in the current study was overall lower than the agreement found in the Romanian sample; and Romanian participants showed overall stronger endorsement for genetic applications in medicine, crime prevention, personalization of the legal system, as well as amending existing laws and incorporating genetics in judges’ training. With information available only from two jurisdictions, explanations for these differences are currently speculative.

For example, differences in opinions may stem from media coverage of the ongoing genetic research, including focus of the coverage on potential benefits vs. ethical dilemmas and issues of access to advances. Views may also be influenced by popular culture, such as books, movies and games that often portray an anti-utopian image of gene editing. The differences may also stem from the way information is presented to participants in research, as previous research has demonstrated that the way genetic information is presented may impact important justice related decisions (Selita, Chapman, and Kovas Citation2019). For example, presentation of the question about the “use of genetic information for crime prevention by the State” may have led to the observed difference: endorsement by 60% of the Russian vs. 91% of the Romanian judges and lawyers. The introductory information for participants in the Romanian sample read: “If we find that people with certain genetic mutations have a propensity for violence … ”. Therefore, the information was hypothetical (“if we find”) and vague (“people with certain mutations have a propensity for violence”). In contrast, in our study the item read: “Over 40 markers in the human genome have been linked to aggressiveness. The State should use this information for prevention of crime”. These facts may have appeared more salient (markers already linked to aggressiveness) and more nuanced (many genes are involved), but at the same time potentially less informative or usable. The observed difference may therefore reflect complex processes through which opinions are influenced by interaction between genetic knowledge, how information is presented, personal values and societal influences.

Other factors, such as participants’ jurisdiction, area of practice and experience may have also contributed to the observed differences across the studies. Further research is also needed to explore any potential differences in views of professionals from civil law vs. common law jurisdictions. Both Russia and Romania are civil law jurisdictions, where the laws are codified; views may be different in common law jurisdictions, where laws include case/judge-made law. Research is also needed to establish to what extent the views of judges and lawyers correspond to those of the general public and other groups of professionals. Several studies to date have suggested similar trends in views of judges, lawyers, and other groups e.g. (McCaughey et al. Citation2016; Selita et al. Citation2023; Selita, Chapman, and Kovas Citation2019).

To conclude, genetic advances have brought many new opportunities to societies, with new powers of polygenic prediction, genetic engineering and gene-based environmental interventions. Judges and lawyers have the power to influence society through influencing interpretations and attitudes towards complex societal issues (Amsterdam and Bruner Citation2011). Therefore, their understanding and views towards genetics can shape how societies view and use genetic advances. The results of our study highlight the need for improving readiness of the legal workforce for the genomic era. This is a shared responsibility and will require efforts of legal professionals, geneticists, educators, forensic scientists, psychologists and other key stakeholders to how the relevant knowledge can be communicated more effectively. Moreover, reducing knowledge “deficit” is only one of the many steps that need to be taken, including increasing opportunities for cross-disciplinary research and discussions, debates, sharing values, perspectives and views (Nadkarni et al. Citation2019; National Academy of Sciences et al. Citation2017).

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (33.1 KB)

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to the judges and lawyers for taking time from their busy schedules to complete a lengthy survey. Their contribution is invaluable for development of justice in this complex area.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Data availability statement

All data are available upon request from the corresponding author ([email protected]). The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to being part of a larger dataset, but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  • Amelung, Nina, Rafaela Granja, and Helena Machado. 2020. “Communicating Forensic Genetics: “Enthusiastic” Publics and the Management of Expectations.” In Exploring Science Communication: A Science and Technology Studies Approach, 209–226. London: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781529721256
  • Amsterdam, Anthony G., and Jerome S. Bruner. 2011. Minding the Law. 1. paperback ed. 2. print., [Nachdr.]. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press.
  • Andrews, Lori B., Maxwell J. Mehlman, and Mark A Rothstein, eds. 2015. Genetics: Ethics, Law and Policy. 4th ed. St Paul, MN: West Academic. https://faculty.westacademic.com/Book/Detail?id=31475.
  • Aspinwall, Lisa G., Teneille R. Brown, and James Tabery. 2012. “The Double-Edged Sword: Does Biomechanism Increase or Decrease Judges’ Sentencing of Psychopaths?” Science 337 (6096): 846–849. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1219569.
  • Bailey, Donald B., Lisa M. Gehtland, Megan A. Lewis, Holly Peay, Melissa Raspa, Scott M. Shone, Jennifer L. Taylor, et al. 2019. “Early Check: Translational Science at the Intersection of Public Health and Newborn Screening.” BMC Pediatrics 19 (1): 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-019-1606-4.
  • Berryessa, Colleen M. 2016. “‘Judges’ Views on Evidence of Genetic Contributions to Mental Disorders in Court.” The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology 27 (4): 586–600. https://doi.org/10.1080/14789949.2016.1173718.
  • Berryessa, Colleen M. 2019. “Judicial Stereotyping Associated with Genetic Essentialist Biases toward Mental Disorders and Potential Negative Effects on Sentencing.” Law & Society Review 53 (1): 202–238. https://doi.org/10.1111/lasr.12382.
  • Bosley, Katrine S., Michael Botchan, Annelien L. Bredenoord, Dana Carroll, R. Alta Charo, Emmanuelle Charpentier, Ron Cohen, et al. 2015. “CRISPR Germline Engineering—The Community Speaks.” Nature Biotechnology 33 (5): 478–486. https://doi.org/10.1038/nbt.3227.
  • Bowden, Keith, Fergus Douds, Yasmin Simpson and Scotland, and Scottish Government. 2011. People with Learning Disabilities and the Criminal Justice System. http://www.scotland.gov.uk/Publications/2011/03/21142925/0.
  • Chapman, Robert, Maxim Likhanov, Fatos Selita, Emily Smith-Woolley, and Yulia Kovas. 2017. “Genetic Literacy and Attitudes Survey (Iglas): International Population-Wide Assessment Instrument.” European Proceedings of Social and Behavioural Sciences 45–66. https://doi.org/10.15405/epsbs.2017.12.6.
  • Chapman, Robert, Maxim Likhanov, Fatos Selita, Ilya Zakharov, Emily Smith-Woolley, and Yulia Kovas. 2019. “New Literacy Challenge for the Twenty-First Century: Genetic Knowledge Is Poor Even among Well Educated.” Journal of Community Genetics 10 (1): 73–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12687-018-0363-7.
  • Chapman, Carolyn Riley, Kripa Sanjay Mehta, Brendan Parent, and Arthur L. Caplan. 2020. “Genetic Discrimination: Emerging Ethical Challenges in the Context of Advancing Technology.” Journal of Law and the Biosciences 7 (1): 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1093/jlb/lsz016.
  • Civil Justice Council. 2011. “Access to Justice for Litigants in Person (or Self-Represented Litigants).” https://www.judiciary.uk/wp-content/uploads/2014/05/report-on-access-to-justice-for-litigants-in-person-nov2011.pdf.
  • The Crown Prosecution Service. 2018. “Data Protection Act 2018 - Criminal Offences.” https://www.cps.gov.uk/legal-guidance/data-protection-act-2018-criminal-offences.
  • Cyentia Institute. 2016. “Mining for Database Gold: Findings from 2016 Breach Level Index.” https://cyentia.com/library-item/2016-mining-for-database-gold-findings-from-2016-breach-level-index/.
  • Data Protection Act 2018. n.d. Queen’s Printer of Acts of Parliament. Accessed September 11, 2021. https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/2018/12/contents/enacted.
  • Fenn, Jessy, Chee-Seng Tan, and Sanju George. 2020. “Development, Validation and Translation of Psychological Tests.” BJPsych Advances 26 (5): 306–315. https://doi.org/10.1192/bja.2020.33.
  • Furrow, Barry, Thomas Greaney, Sandra Johnson, Timothy J. Stoltzfus, and Robert L. Schwartz. 2013. Health Law, Cases, Materials and Problems. 7th ed. Minnesota: West Academic Publishing. https://faculty.westacademic.com/Book/Detail?id=2028.
  • Fuss, Johannes, Harald Dressing, and Peer Briken. 2015. “Neurogenetic Evidence in the Courtroom: A Randomised Controlled Trial with German Judges.” Journal of Medical Genetics 52 (11): 730–737. https://doi.org/10.1136/jmedgenet-2015-103284.
  • GDPR (General Data Protection Regulation). 2016. https://eur-lex.europa.eu/eli/reg/2016/679/oj.
  • Godard, Béatrice, Leo ten Kate, Gerry Evers-Kiebooms, and Ségolène Aymé. 2003. “Population Genetic Screening Programmes: Principles, Techniques, Practices, and Policies.” European Journal of Human Genetics 11 (S2): S49–S87. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201113.
  • Government Office for Science. 2022a. “Genomics Beyond Health.” https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/genomics-beyond-health.
  • Government Office for Science. 2022b. “Genomics Beyond Health.” Government. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/genomics-beyond-health.
  • ICO. 2006. “What Price Privacy? The Unlawful Trade in Confidential Personal Information.” http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/shared/bsp/hi/pdfs/21_07_11_icomotorman.pdf.
  • ICO. 2018. “Enforcement Action.” Government. ICO. 10 September 2018. https://ico.org.uk/action-weve-taken/enforcement/.
  • Jacko, Anastasia M., Andrea L. Durst, Karen L. Niemchick, Stephen M. Modell, and Amy H. Ponte. 2023. “Public Health Genetics: Surveying Preparedness for the Next Generation of Public Health Professionals.” Genes 14 (2): 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3390/genes14020317.
  • Jamieson, Annie, and Gregory Radick. 2017. “Genetic Determinism in the Genetics Curriculum: An Exploratory Study of the Effects of Mendelian and Weldonian Emphases.” Science & Education 26 (10): 1261–1290. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11191-017-9900-8.
  • Kovas, Yulia, and Fatos Selita. 2021. “DNA: The Greatest Text of All.” In Oedipus Rex in the Genomic Era: Human Behaviour, Law and Society, edited by Yulia Kovas and Fatos Selita, 111–172. London: Palgrave Macmillan UK. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-349-96048-4_5.
  • Lintern, Shaun. 2021. “Thousands of Patients Hit by NHS Data Breaches.” The Independent, July 3, 2021, sec. News. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/health/data-nhs-patient-breaches-privacy-b1877154.html.
  • Madrid-Valero, Juan J., Robert Chapman, Evangelina Bailo, Juan R. Ordoñana, Fatos Selita, Yulia Kovas, and Alice M. Gregory. 2021. “What Do People Know About the Heritability of Sleep?” Behavior Genetics 51 (2): 144–153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10041-3.
  • McCaughey, Tristan, Paul G. Sanfilippo, George E.C. Gooden, David M. Budden, Li Fan, Eva Fenwick, Gwyneth Rees, et al. 2016. “A Global Social Media Survey of Attitudes to Human Genome Editing.” Cell Stem Cell 18 (5): 569–572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stem.2016.04.011.
  • Merrick, Rob. 2017. “GPs Urgently Examine 173 Cases of Patients Who May Have Been Harmed after Massive NHS Data Loss.” February 27, 2017. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/uk/politics/nhs-data-loss-gps-examine-cases-jeremy-hunt-health-department-a7602531.html.
  • Moorhead, Richard, Mark Sefton, Great Britain, and Department for Constitutional Affairs. 2005. Litigants in Person: Unrepresented Litigants in First Instance Proceedings. London: DCA Research Unit.
  • Naddaf, Miryam. 2022. “Researchers Welcome $3.5-Million Haemophilia Gene Therapy — but Questions Remain.” Nature 612 (7940): 388–389. https://doi.org/10.1038/d41586-022-04327-7.
  • Nadkarni, Nalini M., Caitlin Q. Weber, Shelley V. Goldman, Dennis L. Schatz, Sue Allen, and Rebecca Menlove. 2019. “Beyond the Deficit Model: The Ambassador Approach to Public Engagement.” BioScience 69 (4): 305–313. https://doi.org/10.1093/biosci/biz018.
  • National Academy of Sciences, Committee on Human Gene Editing: Scientific, Medical, and Ethical Considerations, National Academy of Medicine, and National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2017. Human Genome Editing: Science, Ethics, and Governance. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/24623.
  • O’Keefe, Meaghan, Sarah Perrault, Jodi Halpern, Lisa Ikemoto, Mark Yarborough, and UC North Bioethics Collaboratory for Life & Health Sciences. 2015. ““Editing” Genes: A Case Study About How Language Matters in Bioethics.” The American Journal of Bioethics 15 (12): 3–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/15265161.2015.1103804.
  • Polderman, Tinca J. C., Beben Benyamin, Christiaan A. de Leeuw, Patrick F. Sullivan, Arjen van Bochoven, Peter M. Visscher, and Danielle Posthuma. 2015. “Meta-Analysis of the Heritability of Human Traits Based on Fifty Years of Twin Studies.” Nature Genetics 47 (7): 702–709. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3285.
  • Pope, Matilda, Juan Jose Valero, Fatos Selita, Yulia Kovas, and Alice M. Gregory. in press. “What the Public Believe about the Aetiology of Anxiety Disorders and Why It Matters.”
  • Pugh, Jonathan. 2021. “Genetic Information, Insurance and a Pluralistic Approach to Justice.” Journal of Medical Ethics 47 (7): 473–479. https://doi.org/10.1136/medethics-2020-106913.
  • Raposo, Vera. 2019. “The First Chinese Edited Babies: A Leap of Faith in Science.” JBRA Assisted Reproduction 23 (3): 197–199. https://doi.org/10.5935/1518-0557.20190042.
  • Robert, Hannah. 2019. “Genetic Bodily Fragments and Relational Embodiment: Judicial Rhetoric about “Biological Truth” in Paternity Disputes in the Family Courts.” Australian Feminist Law Journal 45 (1): 63–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/13200968.2019.1646111.
  • Rodriguez-Rincon, Daniela, Sarah Parkinson, Lucy Hocking, Hamish Evans, Emma Hudson, and Katherine I. Morley. 2022. “Assessing the Impact of Developments in Genetic Testing on Insurers’ Risk Exposure.” Rand Health Quarterly 9 (4): 1–17.
  • Selita, Fatos. 2018. “Unrepresented Litigants in Modern Courts – Ordeal by Combat.” Legal Issues Journal 6 (1): 1–35.
  • Selita, Fatos. 2019a. “Improving Access to Justice: Community-Based Solutions.” Asian Journal of Legal Education 6 (1–2): 83–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/2322005819855863.
  • Selita, Fatos. 2019b. “Genetic Data Misuse: Risk to Fundamental Human Rights in Developed Economies.” Legal Issues Journal 7 (1): 53–95. https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.11423724.
  • Selita, Fatos. 2020. “Justice in the Genomic and Digital Era: A “Different World” Requiring “Different Law”.” Legal Issues Journal 8 (1): 55–70. https://www.legalissuesjournal.com/article/814/.
  • Selita, Fatos, Robert Chapman, and Yulia Kovas. 2019. “To Use or Not to Use: No Consensus on Whether and How to Apply Genetic Information in the Justice System.” Behavioral Sciences 9 (12): 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120149.
  • Selita, Fatos, Robert Chapman, Yulia Kovas, Vanessa Smereczynska, Maxim Likhanov, and Teemu Toivainen. 2023. “Consensus Too Soon: Judges’ and Lawyers’ Views on Genetic Information Use.” New Genetics and Society 42 (1): 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/14636778.2023.2197583.
  • Selita, Fatos, and Yulia Kovas. 2018. “Genes and Gini: What Inequality Means for Heritability.” Journal of Biosocial Science 51 (1): 18–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021932017000645.
  • Selita, Fatos, Vanessa Smereczynska, Robert Chapman, Teemu Toivainen, and Yulia Kovas. 2020. “Judging in the Genomic Era: Judges’ Genetic Knowledge, Confidence and Need for Training.” European Journal of Human Genetics 28: 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41431-020-0650-8.
  • Sperber, Nina R., Janet S. Carpenter, Larisa H. Cavallari, Laura J. Damschroder, Rhonda M. Cooper-DeHoff, Joshua C. Denny, Geoffrey S. Ginsburg, et al. 2017. “Challenges and Strategies for Implementing Genomic Services in Diverse Settings: Experiences from the Implementing GeNomics In pracTicE (IGNITE) Network.” BMC Medical Genomics 10 (1): 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12920-017-0273-2.
  • Supreme Court of Minnesota. 2011a. Bearder v. State JUSTIA. Supreme Court of Minnesota.
  • Supreme Court of Minnesota. 2011b. Bearder v. State FindLaw. Supreme Court of Minnesota.
  • UK Department of Health. 2022. “Code on Genetic Testing and Insurance: 3-Year Review 2022.” Corporate report. UK. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/code-on-genetic-testing-and-insurance-3-year-review-2022/code-on-genetic-testing-and-insurance-3-year-review-2022.
  • UK Government. n.d. “Equality and Diversity.” GOV.UK. Accessed 29 April 2022. https://www.gov.uk/government/organisations/hm-courts-and-tribunals-service/about/equality-and-diversity.
  • WiredGov. 2018. “Medical Centre Fined for Abandoning Sensitive Information in Empty Building | Information Commissioner’s Office.” May 25, 2018. https://www.wired-gov.net/wg/news.nsf/articles/Medical+centre+fined+for+abandoning+sensitive+information+in+empty+building+25052018162000?open.