1,268
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Effectiveness of Intergenerational Exchange Programs Between Adolescents and Older Adults: A Systematic Review

, MPhilORCID Icon, , PhD, , PhD & , PhD

ABSTRACT

Communities are aging and becoming more segregated, leading to fractured relationships between generations. Intergenerational exchange has improved cohesion, particularly when different generations engage as equal partners. This paper presents a systematic review of intergenerational studies between adolescents and older adults. Thirteen papers were reviewed using PRISMA guidelines, and outcomes, methodological quality, facilitators, and barriers identified, to better understand effectiveness and inform recommendations for future practice. The framework informed quality assessment, and the papers were rated moderate or high quality. Unfortunately, heterogeneity across studies rendered comparison challenging. Further attention is required to elucidate guidelines for implementing and reporting intergenerational studies.

Contribution to the Field

  1. This review demonstrated how non-familial intergenerational programs involving adolescents and older adults provided benefits to both. Benefits for older adults included improved wellbeing, cognitive, and social engagement.

  2. Benefits for adolescents were identity formation and skill development. Shared outcomes for both generations were improved attitudes and stereotypes, reduced generational gap, and solidarity.

  3. High variability in program design, methodology, and sample size was evident across studies. However, it highlighted the suitability of IG engagement across differing contexts.

  4. Future recommendations included facilitator training, diverse samples, and longitudinal methodological designs.

The overall pattern of an aging population has occurred globally, with predicted numbers of older people (aged 65+) to double by 2050 (United Nations, Citation2019). Communities have changed as many services have become digitized, disadvantaging those who were not technologically proficient. The COVID-19 pandemic brought about immediate and rapid transfers of essential services online and impacted opportunities for intergenerational (IG) contact (Drury et al., Citation2022). Collectively, these factors may have placed older individuals at risk of social isolation and loneliness, especially those who were least mobile or connected. Similarly, higher incidences of loneliness were reported among younger people (NSPCC, Citation2021) and suggestions have been made for mixed age neighborhoods to improve service access for younger and older residents (Sabater et al., Citation2019). Reduced contact between generations and inaccurate perceptions or attitudes may have developed to produce the concept of “other” (Lepianka, Citation2015; Tajfel & Turner, Citation1986). Consequently, many of the positive attributes of integration, such as democracy, trust, and positive social cohesion, were jeopardized (Sabater et al., Citation2018; Social Integration Commission, Citation2014).

Altered social demographics highlighted a shift from traditional cultural practices to knowledge transfer between generations. Older adults experienced fewer opportunities to adopt previously held roles, such as carer or educator, and were unable to impart wisdom or achieve generativity (Erikson & Erikson, Citation1998). Consequently, younger generations were less likely to learn skills, history, and heritage from older family or community members. Older adults also lost access to the social and technological expertises of younger people (Korupp & Szydlik, Citation2005; UNESCO, Citation2011), which impacted their ability to remain active and engaged, particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Intergroup contact theory (Allport, Citation1954) purports that prejudice between distanced groups may be reduced via increased contact, especially when the groups have equal status, common goals, intergroup cooperation, and institutional support. The quality of contact was more effective in improving attitudes and stereotypes compared to frequency of contact (Drury et al., Citation2016; Pettigrew & Tropp, Citation2006). Positive effects were further mediated when friendship development and heightened empathy occurred between groups, and when anxiety related to interacting with outgroup members decreased (Page-Gould et al., Citation2008; Pettigrew & Tropp, Citation2006, Citation2008).

IG programs have connected older and younger generations to facilitate social exchange, knowledge transfer, and promote active participation in developing meaningful relationships (Beynon & Lang, Citation2018; MacCallum et al., Citation2010). The IG methods have been transferred into the learning paradigm as a guided exploration of differences between groups (Franz & Scheunpflug, Citation2016) and placed both generations as equal partners in the process. They have supported lifelong learning and improved wellbeing in older members of society and improved connectedness between groups (Beynon & Lang, Citation2018; Burnes et al., Citation2019; MacCallum et al., Citation2010; Mannion, Citation2012; Newman & Hatton-Yeo, Citation2008). Nevertheless, challenges have existed among IG studies, including small, homogeneous samples and recruitment difficulties (Bertram et al., Citation2018; Fair & Delaplane, Citation2015; Gallagher & Fitzpatrick, Citation2018; Norouzi et al., Citation2015), and the impact of IG projects required further evaluation (Jarrott et al., Citation2021; Martins et al., Citation2019).

The IG studies have predominantly recruited children (aged 5–12 years) or university students (aged 18+) (Jarrott et al., Citation2021; Martins et al., Citation2019; Pstross et al., Citation2017). Adolescent participants were underrepresented within the IG literature – this is important as it has been suggested that adolescents and older adults possess similarities that render them suitable for collaboration. Specifically, they have parallel and complimentary developmental needs that create a cohesive synergy (Newman & Hatton-Yeo, Citation2008). Older adults desire to achieve generativity (Erikson & Erikson, Citation1998), accomplishment, and wisdom, whereas adolescents typically strive for identity formation via evaluation of morals, beliefs, and social connection (Erikson, Citation1959; Erikson & Erikson, Citation1998). Under circumstances that promote positive mutual engagement, age-specific needs may be reciprocally addressed. Age is an important factor when planning and implementing IG programs, as this can affect the appropriateness of content, engagement of participants, and program impact (Gerritzen et al., Citation2020).

Collaborative activities, such as skill development or engaged story telling (Cohen-Mansfield & Muff, Citation2022), are key to IG programs, and outcomes show improved attitudes, knowledge, or wellbeing (DeVore et al., Citation2016). However, communication between participants is often one-way, i.e., one group has interviewed or coached the other in learning a skill, and consequentially, measured outcomes have been unilateral (Barnard, Citation2014). Content analysis of four decades of IG research revealed that only 38% of the studies involved non-familial participants and measured the experiences of both groups (Jarrott, Citation2011).

Reciprocity is the equal exchange of support or information between individuals or groups (Antonucci & Jackson, Citation1989) and sets IG programs apart from others that simply included more than one age group (Mannion, Citation2012). When relationships are reciprocal, successful adaptation and response to aging is facilitated, as is learning and communication, and improved wellbeing (Antonucci & Jackson, Citation1989; Litwin, Citation2004; McKee & Heydon, Citation2015). As such, reciprocity has been significant in promoting benefits for both groups involved in IG exchange and an essential aspect of IG programs (Knight et al., Citation2014).

Diversity exists across many aspects of IG programs (Martins et al., Citation2019), yet some key features remain universal. Specifically, inclusion of participants from older and younger generations, who engaged in shared activities toward a mutual goal, with the aim of improving social, psychological, or health outcomes, such as relationships or attitudes between groups (Drury et al., Citation2017; Springate et al., Citation2008). Nevertheless, evidence highlighting the benefits of IG programs has been inconsistently reported, and examination of the effectiveness of activities could be further explored (Drury et al., Citation2017; Martins et al., Citation2019). Robust claims regarding the value of such projects have been challenging to assert (Knight et al., Citation2014), and there remains a need to improve the implementation and evaluation of IG practice (Kaplan, Citation2002; Martins et al., Citation2019). Giraudeau and Bailly (Citation2019) recommended key conditions to promote successful exchange; specifically, meaningful activities and understanding of the other generation. Yet, the reported samples included children aged 5–12 years and failed to address the dearth of literature including adolescents. The current review aimed to complement and extend current IG research by identifying the benefits of IG exchange and recommending means of improving the quality and effectiveness of programs.

The objectives of this study were to (1) identify outcomes of IG programs for adolescents and older adults; (2) examine the methodological quality of IG programs; (3) identify barriers and facilitators for delivering IG studies; and (4) recommend best practices for future programs of IG engagement.

Method

This review was conducted and reported in accordance with PRISMA guidelines (Page et al., Citation2021) and the protocol was registered on PROSPERO (CRD42019145405).

Search strategy

A systematic literature search was conducted on the databases Medline, Embase, PsycInfo, CINAHL, and ProQuest Complete using the following search terms: (Intergeneration* OR inter-generation* OR multigeneration* OR multi-generation* OR “skipped generation*” OR “split generation*” OR “mixed generation*” OR transgeneration* OR trans-generation* OR cross-generation* OR “cross generation*” OR “age integration” OR “generation* gap”) AND (“skill* exchange*” OR “skill* transfer*” OR “skill* shar*” OR “knowledge* exchange*” OR “knowledge* transfer*” OR “knowledge* shar*” OR learn*). The reference lists of key texts were also searched.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

This review included full-text, English language, scholarly articles published between 1995 and 2022. Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed-method studies were accepted; review papers or program profiles were not. Adolescent participants aged 11–18 years were included; studies with participants younger than 11 or older than 18 were considered if the mean age was between 11 and 18 years. Familial samples were not included. This review sought studies that actively engaged both younger participants (YP) and older participants (OP) as equal contributors in IG engagement and excluded those that did not report measured outcomes of IG engagement for both groups.

Screening process

One reviewer (MW) screened titles and abstracts of results to assess against the eligibility criteria of this review. The full texts of selected citations were assessed independently by MW and KN. Disagreements were resolved by discussion; a third independent reviewer was not necessary.

Appraisal strategy and scoring

An adapted version of the Caldwell et al. (Citation2011) framework assessed the methodological quality of papers as it was appropriate for use in assessing both qualitative and quantitative research. Sixteen critical appraisal questions were selected from this framework to assess study features including rationale, design, and methodology. Scores of 1–3 were assigned for each question, with 1 indicating low quality or little to no detail provided and 3 indicating highest quality or very detailed. Total scores ranged from 16 to 48: scores between 16 and 26 indicated a low-quality paper; 27–37 was moderate quality; and 38–48 was high quality. MW and KN independently appraised each study, and where scoring differences occurred, an average score was applied to achieve consensus. Please see Appendix 1 for the appraisal template used.

Analysis

The reported outcomes of IG programs were coded and categorized into overarching themes by MW according to the research objectives. Themes were then discussed and agreed between MW and KN. Reported outcomes were grouped into outcomes of IG programs that were exclusive to YP, outcomes exclusive to OP, and shared outcomes.

Results

A total of 9013 papers were identified through database searching and hand searching reference lists of key texts. Initial screening of paper titles eliminated 8393 irrelevant papers, bringing database results to 620. The screening process at this stage was to read titles and abstracts to assess if results matched the inclusion criteria for this review; 114 papers were selected for further reading. Full-text articles were read by MW and KN, 101 were excluded, and 13 included in the final review. See for searches and screening results, including reasons for excluding full-text papers.

Figure 1. Flow diagram of search and screening processes. Adapted from Page et al. (Citation2021).

Figure 1. Flow diagram of search and screening processes. Adapted from Page et al. (Citation2021).

Study characteristics

Of the 13 papers, six presented qualitative data (DeSouza, Citation2007; Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Jones et al., Citation2004; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021; Öberg, Citation2007; Portman et al., Citation2010), three presented quantitative (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004), and four were mixed method (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021; Orte et al., Citation2018). Qualitative study designs included grounded theory (n = 3), phenomenology, descriptive, and collective case design. Quantitative studies were quasi-experimental pre-post design, experimental pre-post, and experimental post-only. All mixed-method studies employed a pre-post design. No randomized controlled trials (RCTs) were included, and three studies specified inclusion of control groups (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). However, control samples were not always available for both groups of participants (Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). Total sample sizes varied across studies: three papers had fewer than 20 participants; three had between 20 and 50; two had between 50 and 100, and five papers had 100+ participants. Of the papers that provided a gender breakdown of their sample, seven had mostly female participants and two had female participants only. Four papers provided either an incomplete gender breakdown, i.e., gender disclosed for YP but not older, or failed to include this information. Participants engaged in various activities, including shared life or storytelling; engaged reading; information and communications technology (ICT), educational, or language competency activities; collaborative cognitive or experience activities; and other interactive activities, e.g., talent show or games. All IG activities were scheduled within specific time periods, which ranged from 90 minutes to 1 year. One study employed a single intervention session, nine employed multiple sessions ranging from 3 weeks to 1 year, and three papers provided incomplete information. Eight papers generated measurement instruments and one paper did not specify the details of the questionnaire used. Further information on study characteristics is detailed in Appendix 3.

Outcomes for older participants

Improved wellbeing

IG programs provided older participants opportunities to impart wisdom and guide a younger generation. Older adults modeled positive social behavior to YP, engendering pride, fulfillment, and appreciation of feeling useful via meaningful interactions and experiences (DeSouza, Citation2007; Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Jones et al., Citation2004; Portman et al., Citation2010). In an experimental condition that required YP and OP to consider a life challenge that placed OP as the expert, stronger generativity was reported among OP compared to control conditions that either had single generation groups or placed the YP as the expert (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007). Through open communication and shared life experiences, OP felt they contributed to the development and success of a younger generation (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021).

Physical and mental health

Perceived current life satisfaction significantly improved among OP following IG engagement, as did empowerment, self-efficacy, and confidence (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). Structured activities offered respite from monotonous daily routines, facilitating a sense of purpose, hope, rejuvenation, and reduced isolation (Portman et al., Citation2010). Mental and physical health markers improved following IG engagement (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021), including self-value (DeSouza, Citation2007), and observed affect among OP who had experienced chronic depression (Jones et al., Citation2004).

Cognitive and social engagement

IG programs provided opportunities for OP to participate in engaged cognitive activities (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004), instigating improved mental alertness (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021) and increased engagement with activities they deemed challenging, e.g., Internet searching (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014). Cognitive performance and cognitive-affective complexity, i.e., “the ability to view events and persons in an open, tolerant, and complex fashion by focusing on the negative as well as the positive side of the self and others” (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007, p. 695), were significantly improved in OP in experimental conditions compared to control groups (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007). Social interactivity and feeling connected to society improved among OP who previously felt disengaged and lacking confidence in their social skills (DeSouza, Citation2007; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Jones et al., Citation2004; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021).

Outcomes for younger participants

Identity formation

IG programs offered adolescents opportunities to develop self-awareness and experience personal growth via cooperative engagement with older adults who shared a wealth of knowledge and wisdom (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015). Prosocial behavior, i.e., “voluntary behavior intended to benefit another” (Eisenberg et al., Citation2006, p. 646), was observed in significantly more adolescents in an IG experimental condition that facilitated identity formation compared to control conditions (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007). The YP reported heightened confidence in their abilities to negotiate life’s challenges and enhanced comfort within themselves (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Portman et al., Citation2010).

Skill development

YP enjoyed adopting management and leadership roles during the IG activities (DeSouza, Citation2007; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014) as it offered them control over their educational materials (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021) and increased confidence in their teaching abilities and achievements (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014). Pre-engagement training prepared them for their new roles and engaging with their IG partners (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014), and benefits extended into their traditional learning endeavors (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021). Social contact and perceived acceptance from OP facilitated improved social and practical life skills among the YP (DeSouza, Citation2007; Jones et al., Citation2004). This included English language speaking and writing (Lai & Burchett, Citation2021) and adaptation of Internet usage for practical means, such as booking medical appointments (Leek & Rojek, Citation2021).

Shared outcomes

Attitudes and stereotypes

Baseline preconceptions held by each generation toward the other were heterogeneous. Öberg (Citation2007, p. 36) found that both generations held stereotypical assumptions about the other prior to IG interaction, including negative perceptions of older adults as “old-fashioned” and younger people as “disrespectful”, yet Meshel and McGlynn (Citation2004) reported generally positive attitudes and stereotypes. YP believed that both OP and society held negative perceptions about adolescents, labeling them as “lazy, irresponsible” (DeSouza, Citation2007, p. 51), which some OP admitted to (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004). However, Kranz et al. (Citation2021) found that both generations held more favorable stereotypes of youth than of older age. OP expressed concerns that YP held negative attitudes toward them and would not be engaged in IG work; however, these were overcome (DeSouza, Citation2007).

Adolescents rated the OP’s learning skills significantly lower following collaborative engagement using a computer but did not negatively affect their attitudes toward them (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014). Comparatively, older adults improved their attitudes of adolescents’ knowledge and teaching skills following IG engagement (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). Post-program and follow-up stereotyping improved for both generations in an experimental group that discussed existential life questions compared to a control condition, with more positive ratings recorded on the bipolar adjectives unfriendly-friendly, pessimistic-optimistic, idle-busy, and dependent-independent (Kranz et al., Citation2021).

The YP realized how similar the OPs’ outlooks and abilities were to their own, which diminished judgmental attitudes and previously held stereotypes (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Orte et al., Citation2018; Portman et al., Citation2010). Direct engagement with YP largely challenged negative preconceptions held by OP (DeSouza, Citation2007; Jones et al., Citation2004; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004) but some older men remained unsettled at the informal way YP interacted with them (DeSouza, Citation2007). Younger and older participants welcomed the opportunity to debunk stereotypical assumptions of their own generation, and as such, provided a reliable source to base their perceptions on each other, facilitating improved understanding and attitudes (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Jones et al., Citation2004; Öberg, Citation2007; Orte et al., Citation2018).

Closing the generational gap

Younger and older participants admitted to feeling anticipatory nerves before introductions (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Orte et al., Citation2018), and OP expressed disbelief that adolescents would be interested in hearing about their lives (Portman et al., Citation2010). Program activities, including cooperative learning training, were proactive in easing nerves and bridging the generational gap (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; DeSouza, Citation2007; Orte et al., Citation2018). IG interactions facilitated meaningful and unifying experiences (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Orte et al., Citation2018; Portman et al., Citation2010) that allowed both generations to learn about each other’s values and re-examine their own (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; Öberg, Citation2007). As such, mutual benefits were experienced, including reciprocal curiosity, understanding, and group cohesiveness (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Jones et al., Citation2004; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021), which improved IG familial relations and social capital (DeSouza, Citation2007; Jones et al., Citation2004; Öberg, Citation2007; Orte et al., Citation2018).

Solidarity

IG activities permitted both generations to work collaboratively and discuss morals, family, physical ability, and media representations of IG relationships. Participants identified several shared experiences, which included identification of unfavorable perceptions of the other as perpetuated via the media (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004) and a shared sense of vulnerability within society (Öberg, Citation2007). Consequently, any perceived initial distance between the groups gave way to solidarity. It was not exclusively age-related factors that participants felt connected on. For example, discussions on gender equality reduced the generational gap, as women expressed more contemporary, liberal opinions compared to the male participants (Öberg, Citation2007). Leek and Rojek (Citation2021) found that collaborative working using ICT facilitated rapprochement between generations in terms of shared attitudes toward ICT. Participants at a female retreat found solidarity via their life experiences as women and both generations emerged feeling closer; the OP expressed “pride in the progress of women” following their time with the YP (Portman et al., Citation2010, p. 100). Closeness between generations was reported in several studies, and participants developed friendships with members of the other group (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021; Öberg, Citation2007).

Methodological quality

Assessment rated two papers as moderate quality (Öberg, Citation2007; Orte et al., Citation2018) and eleven of high quality (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; DeSouza, Citation2007; Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Jones et al., Citation2004; Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004; Portman et al., Citation2010). No papers were rated as low quality. Quality appraisal scoring is reported for each study in Appendix 2, column one.

Facilitators and barriers for effective IG engagement

Methodology and design

Data collection periods and the time that participants spent in direct contact with each other differed between studies, including solitary IG sessions and extended year-long engagement. High design variability indicated the suitability of IG programs across frameworks and research questions. Participants often expressed the desire to continue with their study partners long term, and authors considered the potential benefits of longitudinal programs (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; DeSouza, Citation2007; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Jones et al., Citation2004; Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021).

Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed-method studies were included in this review and only three studies specified inclusion of control groups (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). Some papers employed an experimental design, and randomization was evident in selection and design efforts (DeSouza, Citation2007; Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Meshel & McGlynn, Citation2004). Nevertheless, high heterogeneity was evident across included studies, thus rendering comparisons challenging.

Staffing or scheduling issues

During program implementation, Jones et al. (Citation2004) had trouble establishing cohesion between groups, which reduced the time spent in productive engagement, and a lack of funding and available facilitators negated program longevity. Other practical issues, such as transportation for participants and adverse weather, required adaptation and flexibility from facilitators (Jones et al., Citation2004; Orte et al., Citation2018). Conversely, coordinators who provided inconsistent guidance were perceived by participants as interrupting the natural flow of interaction and stifling spontaneous exchanges (DeSouza, Citation2007). As such, the necessity for efficient and engaged facilitators was highlighted.

Reciprocity

Reciprocity has been important in facilitating active learning and improved outcomes between generations (Mannion, Citation2012), and this review included studies that engaged both YP and OP as equal participants. This was reported as both groups adopting and switching roles of teacher and learner or both learning from a third party in an exchange triad. The equal status afforded to all participants improved understanding, challenged preconceptions, and enhanced skill development.

Sample

Total sample sizes varied, from <20 to 100+. Many studies reported homogenous samples for both generations, consisting of mainly female and self-selecting participants. The motivation of participant engagement was pivotal in the success of IG activities (Leek & Rojek, Citation2021). Positive outcomes were reported in highly educated samples (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021) and marginalized or vulnerable groups (Jones et al., Citation2004; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021). Small sample size was often reported, recruitment rarely randomized, and descriptions of the wider population infrequently provided, therefore diminishing the ability to generalize findings (DeMichelis et al., Citation2015; DeSouza, Citation2007; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021).

Summary

Overall, high variability was evident across studies, including design, methodology, and sample characteristics. Evidence supported benefits for both YP and OP engaged in IG activities across settings, including response to developmental requirements, physical and mental health, and skill development. Heterogeneity rendered comparison challenging yet highlighted the adaptability of IG programs.

Discussion

This paper reviewed studies of IG exchange between non-familial adolescents and older adults, identifying outcomes, facilitators, and barriers of effective IG exchange, to inform recommendations for future programs. Engagement between generations elucidated the developmental challenges that each experienced and revealed how they complemented and responded to each other. Development of personal values and identity formation in YP was addressed by older adults who had life experience and wisdom. In turn, curiosity and awareness from YP toward OP reinvigorated a sense of purpose among OP and permitted them to relive and share their own experiences and anecdotes, further encouraging validation and generativity. Post-intervention attitudes toward the other were not universally improved; nevertheless, resounding feedback was that IG engagement facilitated mutually beneficial interactions between generations, stimulated relationship development and improved understanding. The participants found solidarity via shared experiences and feelings of comparability and affinity.

Both generations were equal contributors, which eliminated power imbalances and facilitated cooperation toward achieving common goals. Improved attitudes and understanding occurred, particularly where friendships developed, as purported by intergroup contact theory (Allport, Citation1954; Pettigrew & Tropp, Citation2008). This supports Galbraith et al. (Citation2015) who found that positive outcomes of IG programs were more contingent on a setting that facilitated meaningful interactions and relationship development than the type of activity performed. Pre-engagement education on the other generation or training relating to study activities reduced anxiety and boosted confidence among participants (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Orte et al., Citation2018). Similar findings have been reported with recommendations to engage participants and facilitators prior to IG activities commencing to facilitate engagement and study success (Galbraith et al., Citation2015; Gerritzen et al., Citation2020).

The effectiveness of the IG interventions was positively influenced by participant demographics, including high literacy and educational achievements (Canedo-García et al., Citation2017). Potential bias toward engaged and studious female participants may be evident in this review, as OP were recruited who enjoyed engaged reading and volunteering with YP (Dipardo & Schnack, Citation2004), as were adolescents who attended the highest academically achieving schools (Kessler & Staudinger, Citation2007; Kranz et al., Citation2021; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021). Nonetheless, vulnerable groups were also represented in this review and feedback was similarly positive (Jones et al., Citation2004; Leek & Rojek, Citation2021), indicating the transferability of IG programs across contexts and populations. An imbalance of mostly female participants has been reported in the IG literature (Canedo-García et al., Citation2017; Krzeczkowska et al., Citation2021), suggesting that they may be more open to participating in voluntary or research activities. Engaging in activities that benefit others has been associated with healthy development of autonomy and agency in adolescents (Fulgini, Citation2019) and improved wellbeing and quality of life in older adults (Owen et al., Citation2022).

The findings of this review indicated that male adolescents and older adults may miss out on these benefits. Cohen-Mansfield (Citation2022) found that initial motivations to participate in IG activities were altruism or gaining valuable experiences for YP and receiving support or filling spare time for OP. These motivations were shown to align once IG engagement commenced, and both generations experienced satisfaction and IG friendships as motivational factors to continue the activities. Men’s Sheds have facilitated mutual personal growth for IG groups of men via mentorship and skill development activities (Wilson et al., Citation2018). Further advancement of IG programs that attract male participants would benefit the literature.

Echoed throughout the studies in this review were participants’ desires for IG engagement to continue long term, but practical and methodological challenges affected the sustainability of programs. Martins et al. (Citation2019) purported that the primary predictor of program effectiveness was the length of time that participants were in contact, which highlighted a need within IG research for empirically sound long-term projects. Future studies may consider extending the interactions between participants using digital platforms, as proposed by Canedo-García et al. (Citation2017). This will assist in overcoming practical challenges, permit program sustainability, and encourage empowerment, self-confidence, and skill acquisition (Peterat & Mayersmith, Citation2006).

Integral to a successful and sustainable program is a skilled facilitator who possesses good communication skills, enthusiasm, commitment, and knowledge of both generations (Jarrott, Citation2011; Jarrott et al., Citation2019). Facilitators must carefully shape the program with intent and purpose, incorporating short – long-term goals and maintain communication between participants, facilitators, and researchers, so that expectations align (Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Orte et al., Citation2018). Ineffective management of sessions hindered spontaneous interactions (DeSouza, Citation2007), and YP thrived when offered responsibility and control (DeSouza, Citation2007; Gamliel & Gabay, Citation2014; Lai & Burchett, Citation2021). As such, facilitators and researchers should strive to achieve a balance between structured and hands-off management. Training for IG facilitators that is flexible and tailored to the research objectives and the participants’ abilities will facilitate improved engagement and individual outcomes (Galbraith et al., Citation2015).

Pertinent issues affecting adolescent and older adult populations were highlighted in this review. Variable study design and assessment alluded to a lack of continuity regarding impact measurement, which was reported elsewhere (Houghton et al., Citation2022). Nonetheless, the adaptability and provision of high-quality IG programs is highlighted, echoing findings of Jarrott et al. (Citation2021) that identified evidence-based IG practice across methodological frameworks. It is recommended that IG activities are voluntary and flexible (Jarrott et al., Citation2019); possibly rendering RCTs as unsuitable. Improved research rigor conducted at a community level would be feasible and advantageous. Frameworks and guidelines for planning, implementing, and assessing IG programs will add value and create persuasive power to inform policy and practice. Participatory research methods offer a flexible, iterative framework that promotes inclusive exploration of pertinent social issues (Israel et al., Citation1998) and have facilitated IG benefits, including improved IG relations, wellbeing, and physical health (Anderson et al., Citation2016; Schroeder et al., Citation2017). Recommendations for best practice of IG programs are summarized in .

Table 1. Recommendations for best practice of intergenerational exchange programs.

Strengths and limitations

This review is not without limitations. The search criteria identified IG programs that included adolescent and older adult participants and reported intervention effects for both groups. These specifications may have excluded high-quality studies that were conducted with samples beyond these parameters or that focused outcomes on one generation. Furthermore, this review reported only on studies that were published in the English language, which may have excluded relevant papers.

High variability was evident across almost all features of IG studies in this review and throughout the IG literature (Canedo-García et al., Citation2017; Martins et al., Citation2019). The lack of uniformity between papers made comparison difficult; however, this highlighted the suitability of IG engagement across contexts. Challenges obscured the longevity of programs and samples were often small or gender-specific, thus diminishing the generalizability of findings. Nevertheless, methodologically strong studies were found, and reported outcomes were relatively homogenous, indicating universal benefits.

Conclusion

This review has provided a synthesis of the literature documenting non-familial IG programs between adolescents and older adults. Benefits were evident: improved physical, psychosocial, and cognitive status for older adults, and heightened self-awareness and skill acquisition by adolescents. Improved ability to respond to age-specific developmental concerns, including generativity and identity formation, was evident in older adults and adolescents, respectively, as was improved social cohesion and attitudes.

IG programs can promote improved social connectedness between generations by bringing together groups who infrequently interact in modern society. Age-friendly community policy programs and education systems will promote IG and lifelong learning. Without this contact, the benefits of social capital will be lost, and groups will display underdeveloped social and practical skills, creating a disharmonious society. As communities continue to age and generations become more distant, the value of IG programs becomes more prominent.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Northern Periphery and Arctic Program 2014-2018 (project number 179).

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Correction Statement

This article has been republished with minor changes. These changes do not impact the academic content of the article.

References

  • Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Addison Wesley.
  • Anderson, S., Fast, J., Keating, N., Eales, J., Chivers, S., & Barnet, D. (2016). Translating knowledge: Promoting health through intergenerational community arts programming. Health Promotion Practice, 18(1), 15–25. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524839915625037
  • Antonucci, T. C., & Jackson, J. S. (1989). Successful aging and life course reciprocity. In A. M. Warnes (Ed.), Human aging and later life (pp. 83–95). Edward Arnold.
  • Barnard, D. (2014). Through our eyes: A pastoral care bridge linking the generations. Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging, 26(2–3), 122–135. https://doi.org/10.1080/15528030.2013.826161
  • Bertram, A. G., Burr, B. K., Sears, K., Powers, M., Atkins, L., Holmes, T., Kambour, T., & Kuns, J. B. (2018). Generations learning together: Pilot study for a multigenerational program. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 16(3), 243–255. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1477402
  • Beynon, C., & Lang, J. (2018). The more we get together, the more we learn: Focus on intergenerational and collaborative learning through singing. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 16(1–2), 45–63. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1404405
  • Burnes, D., Sheppard, C., Henderson, C. R., Jr., Wassel, M., Cope, R., Barber, C., & Pillemer, K. (2019). Interventions to reduce ageism against older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 109(8), e1–e9. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2019.305123
  • Caldwell, K., Henshaw, L., & Taylor, G. (2011). Developing a framework for critiquing health research: An early evaluation. Nurse Education Today, 31(8), e1–e7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nedt.2010.11.025
  • Canedo-García, A., García-Sánchez, J. N., & Pacheco-Sanz, D. I. (2017). A systematic review of the effectiveness of intergenerational programs. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(1882), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01882
  • Cohen-Mansfield, J. (2022). Motivation to participate in intergenerational programs: A comparison across different program types and generations. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(6), 3554. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063554
  • Cohen-Mansfield, J., & Muff, A. (2022). Comparing community-based intergenerational activities in Israel: Participants, programs, and perceived outcomes. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 65(5), 495–511. https://doi.org/10.1080/01634372.2021.1983683
  • DeMichelis, C., Ferrari, M., Rozin, T., & Stern, B. (2015). Teaching for wisdom in an intergenerational high-school-English class. Educational Gerontology, 41(8), 551–566. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2014.994355
  • DeSouza, E. (2007). Intergenerational interaction through reminiscence processes. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 5(1), 39–56. https://doi.org/10.1300/J194v05n01_04
  • DeVore, S., Winchell, B., & Rowe, J. M. (2016). Intergenerational programming for young children and older adults: An overview of needs, approaches, and outcomes in the United States. Childhood Education, 92(3), 216–225. https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2016.1180895
  • Dipardo, A., & Schnack, P. (2004). Expanding the web of meaning: Thought and emotion in an intergenerational reading and writing program. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(1), 14–37. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.39.1.3
  • Drury, L., Abrams, D., & Swift, H. J. (2017). Making intergenerational connections: What are they, why do they matter and how to make more of them. Age UK. https://www.ageuk.org.uk/globalassets/age-uk/documents/reports-and-publications/reports-and-briefings/active-communities/rb_2017_making_intergenerational_connections.pdf
  • Drury, L., Abrams, D., & Swift, H. J. (2022). Intergenerational contact during and beyond COVID-19. Journal of Social Issues, 78(4), 860–882. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12551
  • Drury, L., Hutchison, P., & Abrams, D. (2016). Direct and extended intergenerational contact and young people’s attitudes towards older adults. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(3), 522–543. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12146
  • Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646–718). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle: Selected papers. International Universities Press.
  • Erikson, E. H., & Erikson, J. M. (1998). The life cycle completed (extended version). W. W. Norton.
  • Fair, C. D., & Delaplane, E. (2015). “It is good to spend time with older adults. You can teach them, they can teach you”: Second grade students reflect on intergenerational service learning. Early Childhood Education Journal, 43(1), 19–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-014-0634-9
  • Franz, J., & Scheunpflug, A. (2016). A systematic perspective on intergenerational learning: Theoretical and empirical findings. Studia Paedagogica, 21(2), 25–41. https://doi.org/10.5817/SP2016-2-3
  • Fulgini, A. J. (2019). The need to contribute during adolescence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(3), 331–343. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437
  • Galbraith, B., Larkin, H., Moorhouse, A., & Oomen, T. (2015). Intergenerational programs for persons with dementia: A scoping review. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 58(4), 357–378. https://doi.org/10.1080/01634372.2015.1008166
  • Gallagher, C., & Fitzpatrick, A. (2018). “It’s a win-win situation” - intergenerational learning in preschool and elder care settings: An Irish perspective. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 16(1–2), 26–44. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1404403
  • Gamliel, T., & Gabay, N. (2014). Knowledge exchange, social interactions, and empowerment in an intergenerational technology program at school. Educational Gerontology, 40(8), 597–617. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601277.2013.863097
  • Gerritzen, E. V., Hull, M. J., Verbeek, H., Smith, A. E., & de Boer, B. (2020). Successful elements of intergenerational dementia programs: A scoping review. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 18(2), 214–245. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2019.1670770
  • Giraudeau, C., & Bailly, N. (2019). Intergenerational programs: What can school-age children and older people expect from them? A systematic review. European Journal of Ageing, 16(3), 363–376. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10433-018-00497-4
  • Houghton, C., Hennessy, M., Smyth, S., Hennelly, N., Smalle, M., Jordan, F., Jones, C. H., Quinn, M., Casey, D., & Teahan, Á. (2022). The experiences and perceptions of young people and older people living with dementia of participating in intergenerational programmes: A qualitative evidence synthesis. Dementia (London, England), 21(7), 2144–2171. https://doi.org/10.1177/14713012221112385
  • Israel, B. A., Schulz, A. J., Parker, E. A., & Becker, A. B. (1998). Review of community-based research: Assessing partnership approaches to improve public health. Annual Review of Public Health, 19(1), 173–202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.19.1.173
  • Jarrott, S. E. (2011). Where have we been and where are we going? Content analysis of evaluation research of intergenerational programs. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 9(1), 37–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2011.544594
  • Jarrott, S. E., Scrivano, R. M., Park, C., & Mendoza, A. N. (2021). Implementation of evidence-based practices in intergenerational programming: A scoping review. Research on Aging, 43(7–8), 283–293. https://doi.org/10.1177/0164027521996191
  • Jarrott, S. E., Stremmel, A. J., & Naar, J. J. (2019). Practice that transforms intergenerational programs: A model of theory- and evidence informed principles. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 17(4), 488–504. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2019.1579154
  • Jones, E. D., Herrick, C., & York, R. F. (2004). An intergenerational group benefits both emotionally disturbed youth and older adults. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 25(8), 753–767. https://doi.org/10.1080/01612840490506329
  • Kaplan, M. S. (2002). Intergenerational programs in schools: Considerations of form and function. International Review of Education, 48, 305–334. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021231713392
  • Kessler, E. M., & Staudinger, U. M. (2007). Intergenerational potential: Effects of social interaction between older adults and adolescents. Psychology and Aging, 22(4), 690–704. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.690
  • Knight, T., Skouteris, H., Townsend, M., & Hooley, M. (2014). The act of giving: A systematic review of nonfamilial intergenerational interaction. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 12(3), 257–278. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2014.929913
  • Korupp, S. E., & Szydlik, M. (2005). Causes and trends of the digital divide. European Sociological Review, 21(4), 409–422. https://doi.org/10.1093/esr/jci030
  • Kranz, D., Thomas, N. M., & Hofer, J. (2021). Changes in age stereotypes in adolescent and older participants of an intergenerational encounter program. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.658797
  • Krzeczkowska, A., Spalding, D. M., McGeown, W. J., Gow, A. J., Carlson, M. C., & Nicholls, L. A. B. (2021). A systematic review of the impacts of intergenerational engagement on older adults’ cognitive, social, and health outcomes. Ageing Research Reviews, 71, 101400. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2021.101400
  • Lai, A., & Burchett, R. (2021). Involving retired citizens in ESL education: Case study of a secondary school program. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 19(2), 249–271. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2020.1739588
  • Leek, J., & Rojek, M. (2021). ICT tools in breaking down social polarization and supporting intergenerational learning: Cases of youth and senior citizens. Interactive Learning Environments, 31, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/10494820.2021.1940214
  • Lepianka, D. (2015). How similar, how different? On Dutch media depictions of older and younger people. Ageing and Society, 35(5), 1095–1113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X14000142
  • Litwin, H. (2004). Intergenerational exchange and mental health in later life – the case of older Jewish Israelis. Aging and Mental Health, 8(3), 196–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860410001669723
  • MacCallum, J., Palmer, D., Wright, P., Cumming-Potvin, W., Brooker, M., & Tero, C. (2010). Australian perspectives: Community building through intergenerational exchange programs. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 8(2), 113–127. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350771003741899
  • Mannion, G. (2012). Intergenerational education: The significance of reciprocity and place. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 10(4), 386–399. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2012.726601
  • Martins, T., Midão, L., Martínez Veiga, S. M., Dequech, L., Busse, G., Bertram, M., McDonald, A., Gilliland, G., Orte, C., Vives, M., & Costa, E. (2019). Intergenerational programs review: Study design and characteristics of intervention, outcomes, and effectiveness. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 17(1), 93–109. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1500333
  • McKee, L. L., & Heydon, R. M. (2015). Orchestrating literacies: Print literacy learning opportunities within multimodal intergenerational ensembles. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 15(2), 227–255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798414533562
  • Meshel, D. S., & McGlynn, R. P. (2004). Intergenerational contact, attitudes, and stereotypes of adolescents and older people. Educational Gerontology, 30(6), 457–479. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270490445078
  • Newman, S., & Hatton-Yeo, A. (2008). Intergenerational learning and the contributions of older people. Ageing Horizons, 8(10), 31–39. https://www.ageing.ox.ac.uk/files/ageing_horizons_8_newmanetal_ll.pdf
  • Norouzi, N., Chen, J. C., & Jarrott, S. E. (2015). Intergenerational explorations: Where everyone has a purpose. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 13(3), 260–265. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2015.1058282
  • NSPCC. (2021). Childline Raise Concerns About Mental Health as Counselling Sessions Delivered to Children Passes 50,000. https://www.nspcc.org.uk/about-us/news-opinion/2021/childline-press-release/
  • Öberg, B. M. (2007). Meeting the other: A way of fighting age discrimination? A discussion circle with young and old participants in Sweden. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 5(2), 27–44. https://doi.org/10.1300/J194v05n02_03
  • Orte, C., Vives, M., Amer, J., Ballester, L., Pascual, B., Gomila, M. A., & Pozo, R. (2018). Sharing intergenerational relationships in educational contexts: The experience of an international program in three countries (Spain, Poland and Turkey). Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 16(1–2), 86–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/15350770.2018.1404414
  • Owen, R., Berry, K., Brown, L. J. E., & Heyn, P. C. (2022). Enhancing older adults’ well-being and quality of life through purposeful activity: A systematic review of intervention studies. The Gerontologist, 62(6), e317–e327. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnab017
  • Page-Gould, E., Mendoza-Denton, R., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). With a little help from my cross-group friend: Reducing anxiety in intergroup contexts through cross-group friendship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1080–1094. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.5.1080
  • Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J. M., Hróbjartsson, A., Lalu, M. M., Li, T., Loder, E. W., Mayo-Wilson, E., McDonald, S. … Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ, 372, n71. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71
  • Peterat, L., & Mayersmith, J. (2006). Farm friends: Exploring intergenerational environmental learning. Journal of Intergenerational Relationships, 4(1), 107–116. https://doi.org/10.1300/J194v04n01_12
  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.5.751
  • Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 922–934. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.504
  • Portman, T. A. A., Bartlett, J. R., & Carlson, L. A. (2010). Relational theory and intergenerational connectedness: A qualitative study. Adultspan Journal, 9(2), 88–102. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0029.2010.tb00074.x
  • Pstross, M., Corrigan, T., Knopf, R. C., Sung, H., Talmage, C. A., Conroy, C., & Fowley, C. (2017). The benefits of intergenerational learning in higher education: Lessons learned from two age friendly university programs. Innovative Higher Education, 42(2), 157–171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10755-016-9371-x
  • Sabater, A., Finney, N., & Graham, E. (2019). House Prices Push Old and Young into Separate Neighbourhoods. Economic & Social Research Council Evidence Briefing. Economic & Social Research Council. https://esrc.ukri.org/files/news-events-and-publications/evidence-briefings/house-prices-push-old-and-young-into-separate-neighbourhoods/
  • Sabater, A., Graham, E., & Finney, N. (2018). (Un)affordable housing and the residential separation of age groups. ESRC Centre for Population Change. (Briefing 45). http://www.cpc.ac.uk/docs/BP45_UnAffordable_housing_and_the_residential_separation_of_age_groups.pdf
  • Schroeder, K., Ratcliffe, S. J., Perez, A., Earley, D., Bowman, C., & Lipman, T. H. (2017). Dance for health: An intergenerational program to increase access to physical activity. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 37, 29–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2017.07.004
  • Social Integration Commission. (2014). Social Integration: A Wake-Up Call. Social Integration Commission. https://the-challenge.org/cms/uploads/a-wake-up-call-social-integration-commission.pdf
  • Springate, I., Atkinson, M., & Martin, K. (2008). Intergenerational practice: A review of the literature. (LGA Research Report F/SR262). NFER. https://www.nfer.ac.uk/media/1968/lig01.pdf
  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24). Nelson-Hall.
  • UNESCO. (2011). What is Intangible Cultural Heritage?. https://ich.unesco.org/en/what-is-intangible-heritage-00003
  • United Nations. (2019). World Population Ageing 2019: Highlights. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. (ST/ESA/SER.A/430). https://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/publications/pdf/ageing/WorldPopulationAgeing2019-Highlights.pdf
  • Wilson, N. J., Cordier, R., Ciccarelli, M., MacCallum, J., Milbourn, B., Vaz, S., Joosten, A., Buchanan, A., McAuliffe, T., & Stancliffe, R. J. (2018). Intergenerational mentoring at Men’s sheds: A feasibility study. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 31(1), e105–e117. https://doi.org/10.1111/jar.12338

Appendix 1

Caldwell et al. (Citation2011) adapted quality appraisal checklist.

Appendix 2

Additional information on the included studies

Appendix 3

Characteristics of the included studies (n = 13)