3,845
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Introduction

Orchestrating Multiple Goals Across Adulthood: From Solo to Tutti

Abstract

Rather than focusing on single goals, people usually strive for multiple goals in different domains of life at the same time. To do so successfully, people need to match their multiple goals to their opportunity structures, maintain sufficient diversity in their goal system, and consider the positive and negative tradeoffs between particular goals. This issue brings together five original research articles addressing goals in the domains of work, family, leisure, and academic achievement. They draw on longitudinal data from the United States, Germany, Finland and Switzerland and on samples ranging from adolescence to adulthood.

GOALS AS BUILDING BLOCKS OF DEVELOPMENT IN ADULTHOOD

Goals are defined as “desired states that people seek to obtain, maintain or avoid” (Emmons, Citation1996, p. 314) and can be considered as “building blocks of personality and development in adulthood” (Freund & Riediger, Citation2006, p. 353). They give direction and meaning to a person’s life (Klinger, Citation1977; Little, Citation1989), guide attention and behavior (e.g., Bargh & Ferguson, Citation2000), and represent the standard for evaluating performance (e.g., Bandura, Citation1989), which in turn can affect subjective well-being (e.g., Brunstein, Citation1993). One of the most important dimensions for understanding developmental regulation across the life span is learning how individuals commit to, engage with, and abandon goals vis-à-vis their personal resources and the contextual opportunities and constraints (e.g., Heckhausen, Citation1999).

The most prominent theories of developmental regulation (Baltes & Baltes, Citation1990; Schulz & Heckhausen, Citation1996) argue that because humans are endowed with “open behavior programs” (see Mayr, Citation1974) and simultaneously face finitude and limits to their resources (see Kahneman, Citation1973; Kruglanski, Bélanger, Chen, & Köpetz, Citation2012), adaptive developmental regulation must encompass some form of “selectivity” to optimize their development (e.g., Freund & Baltes, Citation1998). In other words, being selective is not only necessary to give direction to life but is also a good strategy for the efficient investment of limited resources.

CRITERIA OF ADAPTIVE GOAL SETTING

But which goals should individuals select and which not? Schulz and Heckhausen (Citation1996; see also Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, Citation2010) have proposed three criteria by which the adaptive value of goal selection can be judged. All three are closely related to the concept and content of this issue.

First, individuals should take into account the opportunity structures for goal attainment and select goals for which opportunities are sufficiently favorable. Otherwise, they risk wasting their resources by striving for goals that are futile. They are also likely to encounter repeated experiences of failure, which can threaten their motivational and emotional capacities. As opportunities wax and wane across time (e.g., Heckhausen et al., Citation2001) and place (e.g., Tomasik & Silbereisen, Citation2012), adaptive goal selection requires constant and careful adjustment, making it a process rather than a static entity. Methodologically, this fact requires longitudinal studies, and one of the eligibility criteria for the inclusion of articles in this issue was that results be based on longitudinal data.

Second, selectivity should not be absolute in the sense that one goal is selected and all others are abandoned. Rather than “putting all one’s eggs in one basket,” it is more adaptive to maintain some diversity in one’s goal system. This not only maximizes the prospects for attaining some of the goals selected but also minimizes the risk of a “total loss” if the attainment of single goals becomes no longer possible. With this criterion, we arrive at the core of the articles in this issue. Indeed, scientific evidence and everyday experience tell us that most people pursue multiple goals at the same time, some pertaining to multiple life domains such as work or education, and some to the same life domain (e.g., Riediger & Freund, Citation2006). However, theoretical and empirical research on this issue is limited, which was one of the primary reasons to compile the studies contained in this issue.

The third criterion is that goal selection should take into account the positive and negative trade-offs between single goals. On one hand, when people are pursuing multiple goals, they may encounter goal conflicts because either goal-related resources are limited or attainment strategies for some goals are incompatible with the attainment strategies for others (Riediger & Freund, Citation2004). On the other hand, one goal or a set of goals can also facilitate others (Riediger, Citation2007). This might result either from instrumental relations among goals or from overlapping goal attainment strategies (Riediger & Freund, Citation2004). These intergoal relations together with their consequences for psychological adjustment and development are the central process investigated in the following articles.

STRATEGIES OF ADAPTIVE GOAL PURSUIT

Observing opportunities and constraints for goal attainment, maintaining sufficient diversity in one’s goal system, and considering positive and negative trade-offs between multiple goals represent developmental challenges—challenges for which individuals have developed various regulation strategies. For instance, individuals can negotiate multiple goals by employing strategies of concurrent or strategies of sequential goal pursuit (Orehek & Vazeou-Nieuwenhuis, Citation2013). Concurrent strategies usually comprise using single multifinal means to purse multiple goals at the same time (Köpetz, Faber, Fishbach, & Kruglanski, Citation2011), whereas sequential strategies require prioritizing the allocation of one’s resources to one goal at a time (Riediger & Freund, Citation2008).

In our own research (for an overview, see Tomasik & Freund, Citation2015), we focused on strategies of prioritization and ran a series of lab studies to investigate how individuals mastered a goal conflict that was only possible to solve by engaging in one goal and disengaging from the other. Our new work is revealing that not only is prioritization an effective means to solve goal conflicts, but also that older people are better in prioritizing. The age effect we found suggests that the management of trade-offs might be closely linked to personal resources for goal attainment that usually follow a distinct trajectory across the life span and tend to deteriorate with age. This example therefore shows that the three criteria suggested are not independent from each other but rather moderate each other in complex ways. This makes the study of multiple goals across the life span a fascinating but complicated endeavor.

CURRENT RESEARCH ON MULTIPLE GOALS

Sharing this fascination, the authors of the articles focus on the topic of multiple goals, leveraging different theoretical approaches and samples that differ in age and by countries, regions, and other characteristics. The first article by Shane and Heckhausen used data from two waves of the Midlife in the United States National Longitudinal Study of Health and Well-Being (MIDUS). It applied the framework of the motivational theory of life-span development (Heckhausen, Wrosch, & Schulz, Citation2010) and explicitly or implicitly tested all three of the criteria proposed above. Concerning the match of personal goals with opportunities and constraints, the authors found that adults indeed matched their engagement with shifting opportunities as they age. They also found that most adults were engaged in simultaneous goals in multiple domains, and that their goals systems were highly diverse. Most importantly, however, Shane and Heckhausen showed that adults can be quite efficient in coordinating the positive and negative trade-offs of multiple goal engagement across different domains of life. In all domains examined, higher engagement in one domain was predictive of a more positive correlation between engagement, quality, and perceived control in other domains. In other words, engagement with multiple goals was on average associated with increased synergies between these goals. This is an intriguing result that opens the question of whether individuals actually perceive their multiple goal-striving as synergistic or not.

The second article by Knecht and Freund explicitly addresses this issue by studying the consequences of multiple goal striving for subjective well-being and perceived goal relations. The authors used data from a sample that was of similar age as the MIDUS sample but lived and worked in Switzerland. The methodological feature of this study was its measurement burst design with more than 120 assessments on 20 consecutive days in actual everyday situations. The starting point of this research was the observation that contemporary work place arrangements and the spread of modern communication technologies increasingly blurs the boundaries between work, family, and leisure (e.g., Allen, Cho, & Meier, Citation2014). Would this have rather positive or negative consequences on subjective well-being and perceived goal relations? The answer to this question seems to depend on whether the different goal contexts and contexts for goals striving were incongruent or whether they were integrated. Results showed that incongruity was negatively related to indicators of subjective well-being, to lower perceived facilitation between the respective goals, and to higher perceived conflict. The opposite was true for instances of integration. Despite the consistent picture portrayed by Knecht and Freund, one question remains open. By design, the authors did not explicitly measure the different demands encountered by the individuals in the different domains of life, which probably influences how much integration is possible.

The third article by Tomasik and Silbereisen focusses exactly on such demands in the domains of work and family life. The authors capitalized on four waves of data collected in Germany right before the outset of the global financial and economic crisis in 2007 (see Silbereisen, Tomasik, & Reitzle, Citation2012). The demands investigated in a sample of adults ages 16 to 43 years in part referred to economic uncertainty related to the crisis but also to uncertainty concerning one intimate relationship and family life as well as demands related to excessive expectations at the work place. All three clusters of demands were modeled as individual latent trajectories over time, and the authors analyzed the interrelations between these trajectories as well as their sociodemographic correlates. Two findings are particularly relevant in the present context. First, the authors found some evidence of a cumulative disadvantage effect in the sense that those who already reported a high load of demands at the outset of the study increasingly did so across the course of the following four years. Second, however, these participants appeared at the same time to be inoculated against the surge of uncertainty related to the global financial crisis whereas those initially reporting low demands were struck quite markedly in terms of perceived uncertainty. Taken together, these findings do not only highlight the importance to consider the broader social and economic context when studying multiple goals but also point to the need to explicitly measure demands in the different domains of life.

The fourth article by Segerstrom et al. addresses the issue of possible excessive demands that has not been addressed by the first three studies. The authors drew on a study of women in middle and older age in which those reporting the experience of mild to moderate chronic pain were deliberately oversampled. The rationale of the article was that the experience of pain would limit resources available for goal striving in other domains of life and probably also affect the perceived conflict between these goals. This might or might not compromise subjective well-being. On the one hand, previous studies have shown that goal conflict can have adverse consequences for subjective well-being (e.g., Riediger & Freund, Citation2008) and psychosomatic health (e.g., Freund, Knecht, & Wiese, Citation2014). On the other hand, avoiding goal conflict by disengaging from goals can be negative from a control theoretical perspective (e.g., Carver & Scheier, Citation1990) as it deprives the individual from the potential benefits of goal striving and eventual goal attainment. Contrary to their expectations, Segerstrom et al. did not find a main effect of goal conflict on subjective well-being. However, they found that for women with pain higher goal conflict was positively correlated with subjective well-being whereas for women without pain this correlation was negative. This finding is striking and urges us to reconsider the issue of causality in the study of multiple goals.

The fifth article by Flunger, Marttinen, Tuominen-Soini, and Salmela-Aro drew on data of a sample of young adults from Finland and focused exclusively on achievement-related goals. The authors conceptualized the multiplicity of goals in terms of different achievement-related orientations that may or may not occur in combination. Young people could, for instance, be motivated to acquire new knowledge and/or to do better than others and/or to put forth as little effort as possible when striving for academic goals (see Niemivirta, Citation2002). Flunger and her colleagues argued that these multiple orientations have meaningful associations with the youth’ identity development status and with various aspects of goal striving such as commitment, effort, and progress. Using latent change score models, they found significant positive associations of mastery-related goal orientations (i.e., motivation to acquire new knowledge and motivation to succeed at studying) with identity exploration and identity commitment as well as with goal effort. Consequently, they argued that high exploration and commitment as well as high effort may serve young adults as a resource for orchestrating multiple goal orientations towards the usually most adaptive mastery-related style. Although limited to the academic domain, this study points to possible developmental precursors of how multiple goals might be approached.

CONCLUSION

Taken together, the articles compiled answer important questions related to studying multiple goal pursuit and illustrate the possible directions that the study on multiple goals could take in the future. Shane and Heckhausen convincingly show that maintaining diversity in one’s goal structure is not necessarily associated with costs and trade-offs but rather can bring about synergies for all respective goals. From a theoretical perspective, maintaining diversity while observing the various intergoal relations thus may not only be considered a developmental challenge but also as a good strategy to optimize goal striving in the respective life domains. One may now ask about interindividual differences or who is better in constructing his or her goal system in a way that maximizes these synergies and why.

Knecht and Freund provide evidence for everyday processes related to a successful negotiation of multiple goals in different domains of life. They point to the beneficial aspects of integrating goals between multiple domains. This integration might be a key strategy to manage diversity and at the same time to profit from positive trade-offs between different goals. Future research should investigate in more details whether a successful integration is more a question of individual variables such as self-efficacy, for instance, or structural variables such as family-related policies at the work place.

Tomasik and Silbereisen emphasize the role of contexts and how changes in the contextual opportunities might refer to multiple goals striving. This directly refers to the first criterion of adaptive goal setting. They show that cumulative disadvantage and inoculation effects may occur when multiple demands are examined over time. Are these complementary processes that result in a set point in the long run or does one process overlap the other, and if yes, under what contextual circumstances does that happen?

Segerstrom and her collaborators demonstrate that an apparent limitation in goal-related resources does not necessarily need to be associated with more conflict between multiple goals. If these results cannot be explained by reverse causality, they might point to some undiscovered strategies of how people regulate negative trade-offs between multiple goals. Future research might examine the exact strategies that are relevant for this effect and whether these strategies could also be successfully applied outside the clinical context?

Finally, Flunger and her colleagues elucidate possible developmental precursors of an adaptive orientation toward goals. Emerging work is revealing how goal orientations play important roles in negotiating conflicting goals outside the academic context, but we do not know very much about how these goal orientations develop in adolescence. Could the precursors suggested by Flunger and her collaborators also be associated with a more successful mastery of multiple goals later in the life span?

These and other questions await further scientific investigation by those who are inclined to study the complexity of multiple goal striving in the future. This complexity requires further theoretical elaboration on what makes an individuals’ goal system comprising multiple goals more adaptive as compared to another. To be successful, this work should probably take the theoretical standpoint of a conductor of an orchestra that ought to be more interested in the interplay of all instruments rather than the fine tuning of single ones: that is, from solo to tutti.

REFERENCES

  • Allen, T. D., Cho, E., & Meier, L. L. (2014). Work-family boundary dynamics. Annual Review of Organizational Psychology and Organizational Behavior, 1, 99–121. doi:10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-031413-091330
  • Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspective on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bandura, A. (1989). Self-regulation of motivation and action through internal standards and goal systems. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 19–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: On the automacity of higher mental processes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 925–945. doi:10.1037/033-2909.126.6.925
  • Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.5.1061
  • Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97, 19–35. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19
  • Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and subjective well-being. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.). The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 313–337). New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and compensation as strategies of life management: Correlations with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Aging, 13, 531–543. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.13.4.531
  • Freund, A. M., Knecht, M., & Wiese, B. (2014). Multidomain engagement and self-reported psychosomatic symptoms in middle-aged women and men. Gerontology, 60, 255–262. doi:10.1158/000358756
  • Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2006). Goals as building blocks of personality and development in adulthood. In D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of personality development (pp. 353–372). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood: Age-normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive challenges. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Fleeson, W. (2001). Developmental regulation before and after a developmental deadline: The sample case of “biological clock” for childbearing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 400–413. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.16.3.400
  • Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motivational theory of life-span development. Psychological Review, 117, 32–60. doi:10.1037/a0017668
  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  • Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis, MA: University of Minnesota Press.
  • Köpetz, C., Faber, T., Fiushbach, A., & Kruglanski, A. (2011). The multifinality constraints effect: How goal multiplicity narrows the means set to a focal end. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 810–826. doi:10.1037/a0022980
  • Kruglanski, A., Bélanger, J. J., Chen, X., & Köpetz, C. (2012). The energetics of motivated cognition: A force-field analysis. Psychological Review, 119, 1–20. doi:10.1037/a0025488
  • Little, B. R. (1989). Personal projects analysis: Trivial pursuits, magnificent obsessions, and the search for coherence. In D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psychology: Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 15–31). New York, NY: Springer.
  • Mayr, E. (1974). Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. American Scientist, 62, 650–659.
  • Niemivirta, M. (2002). Motivation and performance in context: The influence of goal orientations and instructional setting on situational appraisals and task performance. Psychologia, 45, 250–270. doi:10.2117/psysoc.2002.250
  • Orehek, E., & Vazeou-Niuewenhuis, A. (2013). Sequential and concurrent strategies of multiple goal pursuit. Review of General Psychology, 17, 339–349. doi:10.1037/a0032584
  • Riediger, M. (2007). Interference and facilitation among personal goals: Age-group differences and associations with well-being and behavior. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, J.-E. Nurmi, & S. D. Philipps (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action, and human flourishing (pp. 119–143). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2004). Interference and facilitation among personal goals: Differential associations with subjective well-being and persistent goal pursuit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1511–1523. doi:10.1177/0146167204271184
  • Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2006). Focusing and restricting: two aspects of motivational selectivity in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 21, 173–185. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.173
  • Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2008). Me against myself: Motivational conflicts and emotional development in adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 23, 479–494. doi:10.1037/a0013302
  • Schulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1996). A life span model of successful aging. American Psychologist, 51, 702–714. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.7.702
  • Silbereisen, R. K., Tomasik, M. J., & Reitzle, M. (2012). Sozialer Wandel und subjektives Wohlbefinden: Die Rolle von Anforderungen, Bewältigung, Ressourcen und Kontexten [Social change and subjective well-being: The role of demands, coping, resources, and contexts]. In E. Holtmann & H. Best (Eds.), Aufbruch der entsicherten Gesellschaft: Deutschland nach der Wiedervereinigung (pp. 305–327). Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Campus.
  • Tomasik, M. J., & Freund, A. M. (2015). You cannot spend the same dollar twice: On a series of studies on resolving goal conflicts. European Health Psychologist, 17, 85–88.
  • Tomasik, M. J., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2012). Beneficial effects of disengagement from futile struggles with occupational uncertainty: A contextualist-motivational approach. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1785–1796. doi:10.1037/a0026952

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.