9,113
Views
22
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Ostracism in childhood and adolescence: Emotional, cognitive, and behavioral effects of social exclusion

&
Pages 217-236 | Received 06 Apr 2012, Accepted 19 Jun 2012, Published online: 20 Jul 2012

Abstract

Drawing on theories of development, motivation, and personality we examined children's and adolescents’ emotional and cognitive perception of, and explained their behavioral reactions to, ostracism in two experimental studies. In Study one, 93 fourth- and eighth-graders (49 girls) were either socially included or excluded within a virtual ball-tossing game (cyberball). Results demonstrated that ostracism causes negative emotions and a selective memory for social events, similarly for children and adolescents, which verifies the usefulness of cyberball beyond self-reports. In Study two, 97 fourth- to ninth-graders (43 girls) behaviorally reacted to the previously induced ostracism episode within a modified paradigm (cyberball-R). Multinomial logistic regression demonstrated that psychosocial differences between participants displaying prosocial, avoidant, and antisocial reactions followed the expected pattern, which provides initial evidence concerning moderators that prevent children and adolescents from receiving further aggression.

Ostracism is defined as “[…] acts of ignoring and excluding of an individual or groups by an individual or group” (Williams, Citation2001, p. ix). Literature indicates two main functions of why individuals are ignored within or excluded from social contexts. On the one hand, ostracism clearly represents a strategy for acting aggressively and gaining control over social relationships (Williams, Citation2001). Representing a subset of relational aggression, ostracism is experienced as aversive, because it contradicts the fundamental need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, Citation1995) and consequently causes detrimental effects for the victims, as empirically supported by surveys in the field of relational aggression (Crick & Grotpeter, Citation1996; Werner & Crick, Citation2004) as well as by experimental studies utilizing rejection paradigms (Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister Citation2003; Williams, Citation2001; Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, Citation2004). On the other hand, ostracism also has a constructive character. In accordance with this perspective, it can be used to control contra-normative behavior (Stormshak, Bierman, Bruschi, Dodge, & Coie, Citation1999) and to regulate or protect the group (Brewer, Citation2005).

Both functions imply a regular occurrence of ostracism and, in fact, this form of interpersonal behavior is part of our everyday social life. Based on a diary study, Williams, Wheeler, and Harvey (Citation2001) estimated the frequency of becoming ostracized in our lifetime to amount to about 25,000 times, which is on average one ostracism episode per day. Moreover, research suggests that ostracism is ubiquitous in social contexts, because it similarly occurs in private interactions, such as in romantic relationships (Faulkner, Williams, Sherman, & Williams, Citation1997), as well as public interactions, such as in school (Rigby, Citation1999) or at work (Masclet, Citation2003). Hence ostracism should not necessarily be considered a pathological or non-normative behavior, but rather a common event challenging people to correctly perceive and adaptively react to it.

Developmental Aspects of Ostracism

Based on the variety of research in the field of ostracism, much progress has been made in improving our understanding of social exclusion. Recently, literature has also started to focus on the effects of ostracism in young age. This developmental perspective is of vital importance, because functioning peer relationships are crucial for an adaptive development in childhood and adolescence (McDougall, Hymel, Vaillancourt, & Mercer, Citation2001), while at the same time ostracism—as part of relational aggression or bullying—is a frequently occurring, developmental risk factor within the school setting (Rigby, Citation1999; Scheithauer, Hayer, Petermann, & Jugert, Citation2006; Werner & Crick, Citation2004).

Research demonstrates that ostracism causes negative affects and threatens basic psychological needs in children and adolescents (Abrams, Weick, Thomas, Colbe, & Franklin, Citation2011) with an age-related hypersensitivity to ostracism in adolescence (Pharo, Gross, Richardson, & Hayne, Citation2011; Sebastian, Viding, Williams, & Blakemore, Citation2010), which, in turn, was found to be related to an increased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (Masten et al., Citation2009; Moor et al., Citation2012), a neural region which seems to process social pain (cf. Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, Citation2003).

Literature suggests developmental mechanisms, which may explain the hypersensitivity to ostracism in adolescence (cf. Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernberg, Citation2001): First, with the transition to adolescence the amount of time that children spend with their peers rises considerably and the importance of peer groups reaches its peak (Larson & Richards, Citation1991). Second, throughout adolescence, physical aggression becomes less appropriate, but non-physical forms of aggression, such as ostracism, seem to increase with age (Björkqvist, Österman, & Kaukiainen, Citation1992). Third, a development of social cognitions takes place covering increasing abilities such as perspective taking, information processing, and reasoning (Steinberg, Citation2005).

These developmentally based social-cognitive factors may differentiate children's and adolescents’ understanding and interpretation of ostracism, as suggested by Abrams and colleagues (Citation2011), who found that ostracism threatens specific needs differentially in childhood and adolescence. While these initial studies started to provide valuable insights concerning social exclusion in children and adolescents, little is known about ostracism effects beyond self-reports. In the present paper we, therefore, focus on the perception of and reaction to ostracism.

Study 1: Emotional and Cognitive Perception of Ostracism

Previous research predominantly examined people's perception of ostracism with the utilization of self-reports assessing feelings of inclusion (Zadro et al., Citation2004) or indirect indicators such as anger (Buckley, Winkel, & Leary, Citation2004) or affected need states (Williams, Cheung, & Choi, Citation2000). Unfortunately, this economical approach depends on participants’ willingness to report about aversive feelings and may reveal an incorrect estimation of ostracism effects given its transparent test intention.

Therefore, to obtain objective information regarding the perception of ostracism, the first study investigated an implicit cognitive mechanism based on the following theoretical background: In accordance with Williams’ model (Citation1997), the most obviously threatened need during situations of ostracism is the need to belong, which is a fundamental human motivation (Baumeister & Leary, Citation1995; Maslow, Citation1943), and, therefore, drives cognitive responses that are influenced by its current state of satiation. With respect to the “needs-threat-repair prediction” (Williams, Citation2001) short-term ostracism will lead people to attempts at socially reconnecting themselves.Footnote1 The consequential cognitive mechanism implies that ostracized people should implicitly pay greater attention to social events in their environment in order to create opportunities for fulfilling their thwarted need to belong. With a sample of undergraduates utilizing a chat-room rejection paradigm, Gardner, Pickett, and Brewer (Citation2000) showed that “social hunger”, similar to physical hunger, biases the memory for goal-relevant events.

Hence, in addition to self-reported emotions, we apply the described cognitive mechanism to investigate the age-related perception of ostracism. Drawing on the theoretical background, we hypothesize that ostracized children and adolescents will report negative emotions and will display a selective memory for social events. Moreover, given the outlined developmental mechanisms, we expect that adolescents will experience ostracism as more hurtful and threatening compared to children due to the increasing importance of peers as well as the rising experience of and sensitivity to ostracism with age.

Method

Sample

Elementary and secondary school students from two federal states of Germany participated voluntarily in this study. The overall sample size amounts to 93 students, of which 53 were fourth graders (29 girls) with an average age of 10.0 years (SD = 0.6) and 40 were eighth graders (20 girls) with an average age of 14.4 years (SD = 0.5).Footnote2 All participants were randomly distributed to the control group (n = 47) and experimental group (n = 46).

Instruments

Cyberball paradigm

Utilizing the cyberball paradigm (Williams et al., Citation2000) we experimentally manipulated the independent variable, namely ostracism in the experimental group and social inclusion in the control group.

As can be seen in , this ball-tossing game involves three players: one real participant and two computer players which are perceived as real due to an introductory cover story. While the participant in the control group gets the ball as much as the other players, the participant in the experimental group becomes ostracized. This standardized paradigm lasts about 2 minutes in each of the two conditions and starts with the ostracism episode after about 15 seconds in the experimental group.

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm to manipulate ostracism (Cyberball; Williams et al., Citation2000). Arrows and text blocks are shown for reasons of description and were not presented within the experiment.

Figure 1. Experimental paradigm to manipulate ostracism (Cyberball; Williams et al., Citation2000). Arrows and text blocks are shown for reasons of description and were not presented within the experiment.
Emotional indicators

Two instruments analyzed the emotional effects of ostracism. First, we utilized a scale measuring feelings of amusement. Besides the assessment of an emotional indicator, this self-constructed instrument was used to raise the credibility of the cover story (see procedure) and mask the real measurement intention. Hence, from the total of 14 items, 6 were used to generate the scale (e.g., “It was fun to play the computer game”), whereas the remaining 8 items represented filler items. Participants rated their felt amusement on a 4-point Likert scale (from 1 = not at all to 4 = absolutely) and the mean score presented the indicator feelings of amusement.

Second, we assessed self-reported mood by using the PANAS (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, Citation1988) in a validated German version (Krohne, Egloff, Kohlmann, & Tausch, Citation1996). The PANAS measures positive and negative mood by using 10 adjectives, respectively, which present emotional feelings (e.g., “interested” or “upset”). Participants were asked to rate the extent to which the respective adjective described their current emotional state by using a 5-point scale (from 1 = very slightly or not at all to 5 = extremely). The mean score of the 10 positive and 10 negative items presented the indicators positive and negative mood, respectively.

Cognitive indicator

Based on the validated instrument by Gardner and colleagues (Citation2000), a cognitive test assessed the selective memory for social events by asking participants to recall previously read diary events of an unknown person. For the purpose of the present study, we applied an adapted version of this diary, which had been adjusted in quantity, content, and complexity for the participating age group. Within a booklet, 16 events were presented, ranging between 22 and 26 words. These events were counterbalanced with regard to their social character, covering social events (e.g., “Today, I received a package in the mail from my uncle. He sent me the funny pictures from our last vacation together in Spain”) and individual events (e.g., “Today, I got a new haircut that I absolutely can’t stand; I hope that my hair grows back quickly”), as well as their emotional valence (positive and negative events). Finally, with respect to these two dimensions, the order of the respective events was rotated within the diary. The cognitive indicator was computed as an individual ratio for each participant by dividing the number of recalled social events through the number of total events. The resulting score represents the selective memory for social events and ranged from 0 (no social recalls) to 1 (only social recalls).

Background information

As control variables, participants’ computer use and diary use were assessed. On a 4-point Likert scale (from 1 = never to 4 = often), participants were asked to report the frequency by which they use the respective media.

Procedure

Each participant was taken out of class and accompanied to an extra room in school. In the following cover story, the respective participant was told that the research team was interested in the way people get an impression of other people and to what extent different media (computer vs diary) influence this process. Therefore, participants started to interact via a computer game (cyberball) with “other people”, after which they were instructed to read diary entries of another unknown person, and finally they were asked to answer a questionnaire concerning these interactions covering three aspects: (a) the amusement while playing cyberball and reading the diary; (b) the recall of the previously read diary events; and (c) the felt mood. The final debriefing was given in front of the entire class after all students had participated in the experiment for the purpose of minimizing informational exchange between participants and maintaining the cover story throughout the testing period.

Results and discussion

With regard to the emotional indicators, all scales displayed sufficient reliabilities with an internal consistency of Cronbach's α = .72 for feelings of amusement, .87 for positive mood, and .73 for negative mood, respectively. Concerning the psychometric properties of the cognitive instrument, both age groups were recalling a satisfying number of total events to rule out a floor effect for children (M = 4.28, SD = 1.80) and a ceiling effect for adolescents (M = 7.32, SD = 2.50). Moreover, there were no group differences between the control and the experimental group regarding the total number of recalled events, neither for children nor for adolescents (each with p > .5).

presents the emotional and cognitive effects broken down for the two experimental conditions. Group differences followed the hypothesized patterns: Ostracized participants reported to have less fun while playing cyberball, a lower positive mood, a higher negative mood, and showed a selective memory for social events. These differences were examined using a 2 × 2 (experimental condition, age group) multivariate analysis of covariance with computer use and diary use as covariates. In accordance with the first hypothesis, differences between the experimental conditions were statistical significant and showed moderate to high effect sizes (Cohen's d). With respect to the second hypothesis, the condition × age-group interactions revealed no significant results.

Table 1 Emotional and cognitive outcomes for control group (CG) and experimental group (EG)

On the one hand, experiencing ostracism in childhood and adolescence causes a negative emotional state; on the other hand, it triggers cognitive processes in order to change this unsatisfactory state of belongingness: When becoming acquainted with a novel person, ostracized persons display a selective attention for goal-relevant events as a necessary precondition to refill their thwarted need to belong. In this respect, Study 1 provided novel insights concerning children's and adolescents’ cognitive perception of ostracism and a strict manipulation check of cyberball beyond self-reports, which verifies the applicability of this experimental paradigm in Study 2.

Study 2: Explaining Behavioral Reactions to Ostracism

Given the aforementioned frequency of ostracism episodes over the life course (Williams et al., Citation2001), there is obviously a need for children and adolescents to learn how adaptively deal with social exclusion during their development. Behavioral reactions to ostracism differ across people and research consistently finds a considerable interindividual variability in this regard (Roecker Phelps, Citation2001; Williams, Citation2001). As reviewed by Smart Richman and Leary (Citation2009), the main types of behavioral reactions to ostracism are prosocial, antisocial, and avoidant by nature.

However, especially for children and adolescents, little is known concerning the underlying mechanisms that are responsible for variations in behavioral reactions to ostracism (Williams, Citation2007). We assume that the ability to adaptively react to ostracism is influenced by personal moderators, which present abilities that protect children and adolescents from receiving further aggression. A few initial studies already shed light on the question which moderators may play a role in this regard (Ayduk et al., Citation2000; Nezlek, Kowalski, Leary, Blevins, & Holgate, Citation1997), but the empirical evidence is deficient for children and adolescents. Therefore in the second study we aim to explain behavioral reactions to ostracism in childhood and adolescence by identifying their moderating factors.

A framework that theoretically systematizes this research is the cognitive-affective personality system (CAPS; Mischel & Shoda, Citation1995). This model postulates that behavior is influenced by a set of cognitive-affective units (CAUs) which, in turn, interact dynamically within an intra-individually stable activation network of a person. Five different types of CAUs are responsible for behavioral differences due to their activation, organization, and interrelation: encodings, expectancies and beliefs, affects, goals and values, as well as competencies and self-regulatory plans. Within the present study, all of these CAUs serve as predictors for differences in ostracism reactions and are, therefore, operationalized below with regard to this specific context.

The first CAU within the CAPS model represents “Encodings”, which is operationalized as perspective-taking skills, because this ability helps people to encode the world (Galinsky & Moskowitz, Citation2000) and particularly facilitates the understanding of complex and ambiguous ostracism episodes (Abrams, Rutland, Pelletier, & Ferrell, Citation2009). “Expectancies and Beliefs” is the second type of CAUs, which is operationalized as withdrawal because especially anxious expectations and the consequential tendency of social avoidance are evidential person variables explaining inter-individual differences in reactions to ostracism (Downey, Mougios, Ayduk, London, & Shoda, Citation2004). “Affects”, the third component of the CAPS model, is operationalized as physical aggression, because anger- and hostility-related affects are important ostracism-related facets, which correspond with physical aggression on the response level (Anderson & Bushman, Citation2002). The fourth CAU, “Goals and Values”, is operationalized as need to belong, which is an important personal value responsible for behavioral differences in reactions to experienced ostracism (Baumeister & Leary, Citation1995). The final unit of the CAPS model represents “Competencies and Self-regulatory Plans”, which is operationalized as anger regulation, because ostracism elicits anger (Buckley et al., Citation2004), which, in turn, determines behavioral reactions to exclusion (Chow, Tiedens, & Govan, Citation2008). illustrates the application of the CAPS model for the domain of ostracism by mapping the above described and operationalized CAUs between the ostracism–reaction association.

Figure 2. CAPS in situations of ostracism (based on Mischel & Shoda, Citation1995). Arrows between the CAUs are for reasons of illustrating their complex interrelation and do not present specific associations.

Figure 2. CAPS in situations of ostracism (based on Mischel & Shoda, Citation1995). Arrows between the CAUs are for reasons of illustrating their complex interrelation and do not present specific associations.

The system's consideration of behavior as the accessibility of these units and their dynamic interplay is compatible with current cognitive, biological, and neuropsychological models of social behavior. Even though the complexity of human behavior can never be represented by a simple arithmetic equation, the application of this model provides a starting point that guides the examination of interindividual differences in behavioral reactions to ostracism. For this purpose, the second study investigates these personal moderators of ostracism reactions. In accordance with the literature, we expect to uncover all main types of behavioral reactions to ostracism covering prosocial, antisocial, and avoidant reactions. In addition, based on the outlined developmental mechanisms, we expect that prosocial reactions will increase with age due to the improvement of social cognitions in general and the increasing ostracism experiences in particular. Furthermore, we hypothesize that psychosocial variables in the form of CAUs explain behavioral reactions to ostracism demonstrated by a significant overall model fit. More specifically, we expect both non-prosocial reactions (reactive ostracism and aggression) to be predictable by low encodings, low exclusion expectancies, high affects, high inclusion values, and low competencies.

Method

Sample

Students from elementary and secondary schools of a large city in Germany were invited to participate in an online study entitled “Social Interactions in Virtual Space”. A total of 97 fourth to ninth graders (43 girls) with an average age of 11.7 years (SD = 1.6) voluntarily responded and took part in the online experiment.

Instruments

Cyberball-R

The current version of cyberball merely provides researchers with the opportunity to uncover the harmful effects of ostracism. Hence we designed a modification of this paradigm, which goes beyond the experimental initiation of ostracism and allows the research question of behavioral reactions to it to be addressed. After ostracizing the participants, a pop-up window appears in cyberball-R containing the message that an administrator has recognized that one player was being socially excluded, so he encourages the others to include this player and provides two auxiliary functions for the ostracized participant. In accordance with the literature (Smart Richman & Leary, Citation2009), these extra functions enable participants to display the three main types of behavioral reactions to ostracism: Throwing the ball (a) very hard to the other players (antisocial reaction = aggression), (b) to a wall, from which the ball bounces back (avoidant reaction = reactive ostracism), and (c) in a normal manner to the other players (prosocial reaction = social reintegration).

After this message cyberball-R continues with the partly inclusion of the real participant and the possible utilization of these extra functions. Because this second level lasts for a total of 25 additional throws, the number of behavioral reactions depends on the course of the game and specifically on participants’ use of the wall. The total number of resulted behavioral reactions ranged between 5 and 11 (M = 7.38, SD = 1.34).

Psychosocial predictors (cognitive-affective units)

Perspective-taking skills (encodings)

Participants rated their perspective-taking skills utilizing the respective subscale from the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI; Davis, Citation1983; German translation: Lamsfuss, Silbereisen, & Boehnke, Citation1990). On eight items (e.g., “I sometimes try to understand my friends better by imagining how things look from their perspective”) participants rated the extent to which these statements apply to them using a 5-point scale (from 1 = never true to 5 = almost always true). The mean score represents the predictor perspective-taking skills (M = 3.41, SD = 0.67) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .76.

Withdrawal (expectancies and beliefs)

On a syndrome subscale taken from the Youth Self Report (YSR; Achenbach, Citation1991; German translation: Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist, Citation1998) participants rated their withdrawn behavior on seven items (e.g., “I am secretive or keep things to myself”) using a 3-point scale (from 1 = not correct to 3 = completely correct). The mean score represents the predictor withdrawal (M = 1.48, SD = .40) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .75.

Physical aggression (affects)

Utilizing the respective subscale of the aggression questionnaire from Buss and Perry (Citation1992; German translation: Amelang & Bartussek, Citation1997) participants rated their degree of and attitude toward physical aggression on eight items (e.g., “Given enough provocation, I may hit another person”) by using a 5-point scale (from 1 = extremely uncharacteristic of me to 5 = extremely characteristic of me). The mean score represents the predictor physical aggression (M = 2.11, SD = 1.01) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .87.

Need to belong (goals and values)

On 10 items of the need to belong scale (e.g., “I want other people to accept me”) participants reported their individual desire for social inclusion (Leary, Kelly, Cottrell, & Schreindorfer, Citation2006; German translation: Renner, Citation2006) by rating the extent to which each respective statement applies to them on a 4-point Likert scale (from 1 = not correct to 4 = completely correct). The mean score represents the predictor need to belong (M = 2.89, SD = 0.44) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .58.

Anger regulation (competencies and self-regulatory plans)

Utilizing the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI; Spielberger, Citation1988; German translation: Schwenkmezger, Hodapp, & Spielberger, Citation1992) participants rated on three subscales with eight items each their tendency to direct anger inwards (e.g., “I boil inside, but I don’t show it”), to direct anger outwards (e.g., “I make sarcastic remarks to others”), and to control anger (e.g., “I can stop myself from losing my temper”) on a 4-point Likert scale (from 1 = almost never to 4 = almost always). The mean score of each subscale represents the predictors anger in (M = 2.28, SD = 0.65) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .77, anger out (M = 2.01, SD = 0.64) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .81, and anger control (M = 2.77, SD = 0.61) with a reliability of Cronbach's α = .80, respectively.

Procedure

Data assessment was realized in an online study. Students from surrounding schools received a written call of participation including a password for our website, which was password protected to control its accessibility. Each participant was given the cover story that the research team was interested in the way children and adolescents interact with unknown peers over the Internet. The study started with cyberball-R, which had an overall length of about 5 minutes. Afterwards, participants were asked to answer a questionnaire including all psychosocial predictors that took about 10 minutes.

Results and discussion

In accordance with the first hypothesis, participants showed the complete variety of behavioral reactions and differed considerably concerning their behavioral reactions between each other. Given these inter-individually varying reactions, we grouped participants into categories based on their predominant type of behavioral reaction. Thereby, we revealed three groups, which distribute as follows: (a) prosocial (55%); (b) avoidant (34%); antisocial (11%).

In order to compare these groups and explain the behavioral reactions to ostracism, we performed multinomial logistic regression analysis including two logit models (avoidant and antisocial reactions) contrasting the reference category (prosocial reactions). In the model we entered demographics (age, gender, and computer use) as controls and psychosocial variables (perspective-taking skills, withdrawal, physical aggression, need to belong, anger in, anger out, and anger control) as theory-based predictors. Continuous variables were z-standardized to facilitate the comparability and interpretation of all regression weights. It is noteworthy, that the intercorrelations between all predictors are at best moderate in their absolute values. Results of the multinomial logistic regression analysis are summarized in .

Table 2 Multinomial logistic regression predicting behavioral reactions to ostracism

In contrast to the second hypothesis, no demographic variable yielded a significant effect, which suggests that behavioral reactions to ostracism seem to be independent of age. In accordance with the third hypothesis, the utility of psychosocial predictors for predicting ostracism reactions was demonstrated by a significant overall model fit, LR χ2(20, N = 97) = 32.07, p < .05. More specifically, although many effects missed the level of significance due to the lacking statistical power of the small subsamples, the avoidant and antisocial groups are characterized by the expected psychosocial differences in contrast to the prosocially reacting group: Avoidant participants who display reactive ostracism have low exclusion expectancies (social withdrawal) and high affects (physical aggression), while antisocial participants who display aggression have low competencies (anger regulation) as well as tending to have low encodings (perspective-taking skills) and high inclusion values (need to belong).

These findings support the usefulness of the CAPS model for identifying factors that moderate the reaction to ostracism. In this respect, Study 2 provides initial insights concerning psychosocial variables, which may reflect the (in)ability of children and adolescents to adaptively react to ostracism and serve as risk or protective factors for receiving further aggression.

General Discussion

The primary purpose of this paper was to examine the emotional, cognitive, and behavioral effects of ostracism in childhood and adolescence. The main findings of the two experimental studies refer to the universality of ostracism effects and to the initial explanation of behavioral reactions to ostracism.

Universality of ostracism effects

Both studies revealed the hypothesized ostracism effects in childhood and adolescence: Social exclusion is perceived as a fundamental threat because it negatively affects children's and adolescents’ emotions and immediately drives their cognitive state to fulfill their thwarted need to belong. Moreover, for almost every second participant (45%), ostracism initiates socially incompetent reactions, either relationally aggressive in the form of reactive ostracism or physically aggressive in the form of hurting the source of ostracism. In accordance with the existing literature, these findings support the highly unpleasant character of ostracism (cf. Williams, Citation2001, Citation2007). Most interestingly, across both studies, the resulted ostracism effects were found to affect children and adolescents similarly, because no age differences appeared with regard to the emotional and cognitive perception of and the behavioral reactions to ostracism. Concerning the emotional effects, the missing age difference seems to contradict previous research that revealed a hypersensitivity to ostracism in adolescence, but these developmental comparisons were contrasting adults (Pharo et al., Citation2011; Sebastian et al., Citation2010). In fact, in accordance with the present findings, studies that compared children and adolescents found no age difference concerning emotional ostracism effects (Abrams et al., Citation2011; Moor et al., Citation2012). In combination with the age-independent non-self-report measures (cognitive and behavioral variables), these findings strongly suggest that the general hurtfulness of ostracism applies similarly to both children and adolescents.

Behavioral reactions to ostracism

To our knowledge, Study 2 is the first examination which integrates both prosocial and antisocial behavioral reactions in the same study. Building on previous studies that changed aspects of the cyberball environment (Van Beest, & Williams, Citation2006; Van Beest, Williams, & Van Dijk, Citation2011), we similarly designed a modified version of this experimental paradigm (cyberball-R) in order to investigate an innovative research question. With this, we began to explain behavioral reactions to ostracism and revealed several novel insights. First, behavioral reactions to ostracism differ inter-individually. Second, the extent to which children and adolescents display behavioral reactions was predictable with the use of psychosocial variables, which were operationalized based on the CAPS model (Mischel & Shoda, Citation1995). Third, the elements of this model (encodings, expectancies and beliefs, affects, goals and values, competencies and self-regulatory plans) differentiate between prosocial and non-prosocial reactions in the expected manner.

In particular the finding that low perspective-taking and anger control skills are associated with aggressive reactions supports research on social behavior in young age (Dodge, Pettit, McClaskey, & Brown, Citation1986) and suggests a vicious circle of ostracism and aggression: Children and adolescents with information-processing deficits, in particular, misinterpret ambiguous situations and assume hostile intentions, which causes anger. In combination with low anger control skills, these children and adolescents will most likely show aggressive behavior which, in turn, increases the likelihood of experiencing further ostracism, rejection, or aggression by their peers as a reaction to their maladaptive behavior. When we successfully translate these finding into applied fields and enable further projects to develop suitable and effective prevention-intervention programs, we may help children and adolescents facing serious long-term episodes of ostracism.

Limitations and outlook

It is worth noting that Study 2 is quasi-experimental in nature, because we merely realized the exclusion condition for examining behavioral reactions to ostracism. Nonetheless, there are several reasons for focusing the exclusion option in Study 2. First of all, this design matches our research question, because cyberball-R provides the dependent variable, which we aimed to explain, and not the independent variable, which needs to be experimentally manipulated. Second, the previous experimental validation of cyberball in Study 1 provided the necessary knowledge concerning the occurrence of ostracism within the exclusion condition, so that Study 2 could build on this by exploring behavioral reactions to it. Finally, to our knowledge, a theoretical and empirical rationale for antisocial reactions within the inclusion condition is not to be expected, and, therefore, the subsequent statistical analyses merely covering prosocial reactions would be unnecessary. However, although Study 1 ensures that cyberball initiates ostracism, upcoming validations of cyberball-R in general and its types of throws in particular are required and will advance this important line of research.

Another noteworthy point one needs to consider when interpreting the data of Study 2 is the limitation to self-reports for the assessment of psychosocial predictors. Even though the online study was anonymous, so that self-reports are free of social desirability, additional ratings by teachers, peers, or adults concerning children's and adolescents’ psychosocial skills would have certainly enriched the explanatory power of Study 2. However, self-reports may not only be more conservative, but considering a person's perception of the world might also be more useful and informative in explaining and predicting human behavior than measuring the objective world itself.

The questionnaire elements in Study 1 were not counterbalanced, so that the feelings of exclusion may have been less present for later items. However, as the questionnaire was very short, mood-recovering processes after the exclusion seem to be unlikely; but even if they exist, it would rather support our findings, as this design effect makes the hypothesis more conservative to test. Similarly, while CAUs are expected to present rather stable person variables (cf. Mischel & Shoda, 1995), one might correctly expect a potential confirmation bias in Study 2, in which self-reported CAUs were measured after cyberball-R. Hence children's and adolescents’ behavior might have caused their self-reports, because being rejected impacts emotion which, in turn, influences how subsequent information are processed and interpreted. Although in both cases the sequence effects are rather minor, we encourage further studies to counterbalance the elements of their study design in order to avoid possible confounding effects.

Together with the few initial studies that addressed a developmental perspective on ostracism, our findings suggest that the ability to cope with ostracism seems to develop between adolescence and young adulthood (cf. Sebastian et al., Citation2010). In order to identify those critical periods, future studies need to cover a broader age range, ideally over the entire course of life and including longitudinal designs for examining developmental trajectories.

While previous research hardly considered ostracism in childhood and adolescence, the present studies provided valuable insights regarding the emotional, cognitive, and behavioral effects of ostracism. With these findings we have started to generate evidence concerning moderating risk and protective factors concerning ostracism reactions. As the CAPS model proved a valuable framework to guide and systematize this line of research, the expansion of cognitive-affective units with the use of additional predictors offers a theory-based and structured agenda, which promises to improve our understanding of ostracism in childhood and adolescence.

Notes

1 The underlying logic of this exclusion–reconnection prediction implies that rejected people should only be favorably interested in people who present a realistic option of a renewed social connection (Maner, DeWall, Baumeister, & Schaller, Citation2007). In contrast, the sources of short-term ostracism, who have previously rejected a person are also likely to receive reactive aggression (Buckley et al., Citation2004; Twenge, Baumeister, Tice, & Stucke, Citation2001), as investigated in Study 2 of this paper.

2 These two homogeneous age groups of 10- and 14-year-olds were chosen because they are both embedded within a comparably stable composition of classmates due to class rearrangements within the German school track system. Therefore, social conditions of the school system do not affect the comparison of ostracism effects between children and adolescents.

REFERENCES

  • Abrams , D , Rutland , A , Pelletier , J and Ferrell , J . 2009 . Group nous and social exclusion: The role of theory of social mind, multiple classification skill and social experience of peer relations within groups . Child Development , 80 : 224 – 243 .
  • Abrams , D , Weick , M , Thomas , D , Colbe , H and Franklin , KM . 2011 . On-line ostracism affects children differentially from adolescents and adults . British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 29 : 110 – 123 .
  • Achenbach , TM . 1991 . Manual of the youth self-report and (1991) profile , Burlington , VT : University of Vermont .
  • Amelang , M and Bartussek , D . 1997 . Differentielle Psychologie und Persönlichkeitsforschung [Differential psychology and personality research] , Kohlhammer : Stuttgart .
  • Anderson , CA and Bushman , BJ . 2002 . Human aggression . Annual Review of Psychology , 53 : 27 – 51 .
  • Arbeitsgruppe Deutsche Child Behavior Checklist . 1998 . Fragebogen für Jugendliche; deutsche Bearbeitung der Youth Self-Report Form der Child Behavior Checklist (YSR) [Questionnaire for adolescents; German adaptation of the youth self-report form from the child behavior checklist (YSR)] , Köln : Arbeitsgruppe Kinder-, Jugend- und Familiendiagnostik .
  • Ayduk , Ö , Mendoza-Denton , R , Mischel , W , Downey , G , Peake , PK and Rodriguez , M . 2000 . Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 29 : 776 – 792 .
  • Baumeister , RF and Leary , MR . 1995 . The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation . Psychological Bulletin , 117 : 497 – 529 .
  • Björkqvist , K , Österman , K and Kaukiainen , A . 1992 . “ The development of direct and indirect aggressive strategies in males and females ” . In Of mice and women: Aspects of female aggression , Edited by: Björkqvist , K and Niemela , P . 51 – 64 . San Diego , CA : Academic .
  • Brewer , MB . 2005 . “ The psychological impact of social isolation ” . In The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying , Edited by: Williams , K , Forgas , J and von Hippel , W . 333 – 345 . New York : Psychology Press .
  • Buckley , K , Winkel , R and Leary , M . 2004 . Reactions to acceptance and rejection: Effects of level and sequence of relational evaluation . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 40 : 14 – 28 .
  • Buss , AH and Perry , M . 1992 . The aggression questionnaire . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 63 : 452 – 459 .
  • Chow , RM , Tiedens , LZ and Govan , CL . 2008 . Excluded emotions: The role of anger in antisocial responses to ostracism . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 44 : 896 – 903 .
  • Crick , NR and Grotpeter , JK . 1996 . Children's treatment by peers: Victims of relational aggression and overt aggression . Development and Psychopathology , 8 : 367 – 380 .
  • Davis , MH . 1983 . Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 44 : 113 – 126 .
  • Dodge , KA , Pettit , GS , McClaskey , CL and Brown , MM . 1986 . Social competence in children . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 51 ( 2 ) Serial No. 213
  • Downey , G , Mougios , V , Ayduk , O , London , B and Shoda , Y . 2004 . Rejection sensitivity and the defensive motivational system: Insights from the startle response to rejection cues . Psychological Science , 15 : 668 – 673 .
  • Eisenberger , NI , Lieberman , MD and Williams , KD . 2003 . Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion . Science , 302 : 290 – 292 .
  • Faulkner , SJ . Williams, K. D., Sherman, B., & Williams, E. (1997). The “silent treatment”: Its incidence and impact. Paper presented at the 69th annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago
  • Galinsky , AD and Moskowitz , GB . 2000 . Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 78 : 708 – 724 .
  • Gardner , WL , Pickett , CL and Brewer , MB . 2000 . Social exclusion and selective memory: How the need to belong influences memory for social events . Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 26 : 486 – 496 .
  • Krohne , HW , Egloff , B , Kohlmann , C-W and Tausch , A . 1996 . Untersuchung mit einer deutschen Form der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [Investigating a German form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS)] . Diagnostica , 42 : 139 – 156 .
  • Lamsfuss , SM , Silbereisen , RK and Boehnke , K . 1990 . Empathie und Motive prosozialen Handelns [Empathy and motives of prosocial behavior] , Berlin : Freie Universität .
  • Larson , R and Richards , MH . 1991 . Daily companionship in late childhood and early adolescence: Changing developmental contexts . Child Development , 62 : 284 – 300 .
  • Leary , MR . Kelly, K. M., Cottrell, C. A., & Schreindorfer, L. S. (2006). Individual differences in the need to belonging: Mapping the nomological network. Unpublished manuscript, Wake Forest University, NC, USA
  • Maner , JK , DeWall , CN , Baumeister , RF and Schaller , M . 2007 . Does social exclusion motivate interpersonal reconnection? Resolving the “porcupine problem” . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 92 : 42 – 55 .
  • Masclet , D . 2003 . Ostracism in work teams: A public good experiment . International Journal of Manpower , 24 : 867 – 887 .
  • Maslow , AH . 1943 . A theory of human motivation . Psychological Review , 50 : 370 – 396 .
  • Masten , CL , Eisenberger , NI , Borofsky , LA , Pfeifer , JH , McNealy , K Mazziotta , JC . 2009 . Neural correlates of social exclusion during adolescence: Understanding the distress of peer rejection . Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience , 4 : 143 – 157 .
  • McDougall , P , Hymel , S , Vaillancourt , T and Mercer , L . 2001 . “ The consequences of early childhood rejection ” . In Interpersonal rejection , Edited by: Leary , M . 213 – 247 . New York : Oxford University Press .
  • Mischel , W and Shoda , Y . 1995 . A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure . Psychological Review , 102 : 246 – 268 .
  • Moor , BG , Güroğlu , B , Op de Macks , ZA , Rombouts , SA , Van der Molen , MW and Crone , EA . 2012 . Social exclusion and punishment of excluders: Neural correlates and developmental trajectories . Neuroimage , 59 : 708 – 717 .
  • Nezlek , JB , Kowalski , RM , Leary , MR , Blevins , T and Holgate , S . 1997 . Personality moderators of reactions to interpersonal rejection: Depression and trait self-esteem . Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 23 : 1235 – 1244 .
  • Pharo , H , Gross , J , Richardson , R and Hayne , H . 2011 . Age-related changes in the effect of ostracism . Social Influence , 6 : 22 – 38 .
  • Prinstein , M , Boergers , J and Vernberg , E . 2001 . Overt and relational aggression in adolescents: Social-psychological adjustment of aggressors and victims . Journal of Clinical Child Psychology , 30 : 479 – 491 .
  • Renner , B . (2006). Need to Belong Scale. Unpublished instrument. Retrieved from: http://www.uni-konstanz.de/diagnostik
  • Rigby , K . 1999 . Peer victimization at school and the health of secondary school students . British Journal of Educational Psychology , 69 : 95 – 104 .
  • Roecker Phelps , CE . 2001 . Children's responses to overt and relational aggression . Journal of Clinical Child Psychology , 30 : 240 – 252 .
  • Scheithauer , H , Hayer , T , Petermann , F and Jugert , G . 2006 . Physical, verbal and relational forms of bullying among students from Germany: Gender-, age-differences and correlates . Aggressive Behavior , 32 : 261 – 275 .
  • Schwenkmezger , P . Hodapp, V., & Spielberger C. D. (1992). Das State-Trait-Ärger-Ausdrucks-Inventar – STAXI. [The State-Trait-Anger-Expression-Inventory – STAXI]. Bern: Huber
  • Sebastian , C , Viding , E , Williams , KD and Blakemore , SJ . 2010 . Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence . Brain and Cognition , 72 : 134 – 145 .
  • Smart Richman , L and Leary , MR . 2009 . Reactions to discrimination, stigmatization, ostracism, and other forms of interpersonal rejection: A multimotive model . Psychological Review , 116 : 365 – 383 .
  • Spielberger , CD . 1988 . Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) , Odessa , FL : Psychological Assessment Resources .
  • Steinberg , L . 2005 . Cognitive and affective development in adolescence . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 9 : 69 – 74 .
  • Stormshak , EA , Bierman , KL , Bruschi , C , Dodge , KA and Coie , JD . 1999 . The relation between behaviour problems and peer preference in different classroom contexts . Child Development , 70 : 169 – 182 .
  • Twenge , J , Baumeister , R , Tice , D and Stucke , T . 2001 . If you can't join them, beat them: Effects of social exclusion on aggressive behavior . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 81 : 1058 – 1069 .
  • Twenge , J , Catanese , K and Baumeister , R . 2003 . Social exclusion and the deconstructed state: Time perception, meaninglessness, lethargy, lack of emotion, and self-awareness . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 85 : 409 – 423 .
  • Van Beest , I and Williams , KD . 2006 . When inclusion costs and ostracism pays, ostracism still hurts . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 91 : 918 – 928 .
  • Van Beest , I , Williams , KD and Van Dijk , E . 2011 . Cyberbomb: Effects of being ostracized from a death game . Group Processes and Intergroup Relations , 14 : 581 – 596 .
  • Watson , D , Clark , LA and Tellegen , A . 1988 . Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 54 : 1063 – 1070 .
  • Werner , NE and Crick , NR . 2004 . Maladaptive peer relationships and the development of relational and physical aggression during middle childhood . Social Development , 13 : 495 – 514 .
  • Williams , KD . 1997 . “ Social ostracism ” . In Aversive interpersonal behaviors , Edited by: Kowalski , RM . 130 – 170 . New York : Plenum Press .
  • Williams , KD . 2001 . Ostracism: The power of silence , New York : Guilford Press .
  • Williams , KD . 2007 . Ostracism . Annual Review of Psychology , 58 : 425 – 452 .
  • Williams , KD , Cheung , C and Choi , W . 2000 . Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignored over the Internet . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 79 : 748 – 762 .
  • Williams , KD , Wheeler , L and Harvey , J . 2001 . “ Inside the social mind of the ostracizer ” . In The social mind: Cognitive and motivational aspects of interpersonal behavior , Edited by: Forgas , J , Williams , K and Wheeler , L . 294 – 320 . New York : Cambridge University Press .
  • Zadro , L , Williams , KD and Richardson , R . 2004 . How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence . Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 40 : 560 – 567 .

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.