4,301
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Articles

The potential of biomarkers for diagnosing insomnia: Consensus statement of the WFSBP Task Force on Sleep Disorders

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, , ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon show all
Pages 614-642 | Received 21 Oct 2022, Accepted 17 Jan 2023, Published online: 07 Mar 2023

Abstract

Objectives

Thus far, the diagnosis of insomnia is based on purely clinical criteria. Although a broad range of altered physiological parameters has been identified in insomniacs, the evidence to establish their diagnostic usefulness is very limited. Purpose of this WFSBP Task Force consensus paper is to systematically evaluate a series of biomarkers as potential diagnostic tools for insomnia.

Methods

A newly created grading system was used for assessing the validity of various measurements in establishing the diagnosis of insomnia; these measurements originated from relevant studies selected and reviewed by experts.

Results

The measurements with the highest diagnostic performance were those derived from psychometric instruments. Biological measurements which emerged as potentially useful diagnostic instruments were polysomnography-derived cyclic alternating pattern, actigraphy, and BDNF levels, followed by heart rate around sleep onset, deficient melatonin rhythm, and certain neuroimaging patterns (mainly for the activity of frontal and pre-frontal cortex, hippocampus and basal ganglia); yet, these findings need replication, as well as establishment of commonly accepted methodology and diagnostic cut-off points. Routine polysomnography, EEG spectral analysis, heart rate variability, skin conductance, thermoregulation, oxygen consumption, HPA axis, and inflammation indices were not shown to be of satisfactory diagnostic value.

Conclusions

Apart from psychometric instruments which are confirmed to be the gold standard in diagnosing insomnia, six biomarkers emerge as being potentially useful for this purpose.

Introduction

Insomnia is regarded as one of the major health challenges, deserving accuracy in its diagnosis considering its global burden related to mental and physical health (Collins et al. Citation2011). The diagnosis of insomnia, as defined in the major diagnostic classification systems published by the World Health Organisation (ICD-11; WHO 2019), the American Psychiatric Association (DSM-5; APA Citation2013), and the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (ICSD-3; AASM Citation2014), is based on a number of clinical criteria referring to subjective complaints, i.e. difficulty in initiating or maintaining sleep, dissatisfaction with quantity and/or quality of sleep, as well as psychological and other negative consequences of sleep disturbance. In both the ‘Practice Parameters for the Evaluation of Chronic Insomnia’ developed by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine (Chesson et al. Citation2000) and the most recently published European Guideline for the Diagnosis and Treatment of Insomnia developed by the European Sleep Research Society—ESRS (Riemann et al. Citation2017) the diagnosis of insomnia is established through the use of the above mentioned criteria. In spite of the fact that these universally accepted criteria are purely clinical, certain biological measurements are being considered for the diagnosis of insomnia in both the above guideline papers, as well as in an AASM companion paper for the evaluation of insomnia (Sateia et al. Citation2000).

Since the diagnosis of insomnia is based on subjective complaints (ICD-11, DSM5, ICSD-3), rating scales and other means of psychometric assessment are expected to show considerable diagnostic validity. On the other hand, biological research has identified a broad range of altered physiological parameters among insomnia patients, which could be considered as appropriate biomarkers (Califf Citation2018). However, as yet, there has been very limited evidence on sensitivity, specificity, and/or other appropriate measures of diagnostic accuracy of these parameters to establish their usefulness as diagnostically valid biomarkers for insomnia. Both the American (Chesson et al. Citation2000) and the European (Riemann et al. Citation2017) guidelines mainly rely on expert evaluation of the existing literature to generate the respective recommendations for the diagnosis of insomnia. In the extensive review of studies for the diagnosis of insomnia by Sateia et al. (Citation2000), on which the American guidelines are based, the approach to detect the diagnostic potential of the methods examined relies primarily on the value of alpha and beta errors, indices which denote statistical significance but do not necessarily correspond to specific measures reflecting diagnostic accuracy per se.

The present consensus paper consists of a systematic evaluation of biomarkers as potential diagnostic tools for insomnia based on measures of diagnostic accuracy, following an identical assessment of the diagnostic accuracy of psychometric instruments in diagnosing insomnia. For this purpose, we created a novel grading system for assessing the studies which are reviewed herein regarding the degree of pertinence of their reported data and the level of accuracy of their method approach in establishing the diagnosis of insomnia. It is noteworthy that the diagnostic usefulness of the biological parameters for insomnia has not been previously compared among themselves, neither vs. psychometric measurements.

Methods

In a business meeting of the WFSBP Task Force on Sleep Disorders, it was decided to create a consensus statement on the value of biomarkers and other means of assessment for the diagnosis of insomnia. To this effect, a steering group consisting of Adam Wichniak, Dimitris Dikeos, and Constantin Soldatos took the initiative to invite several colleagues, each having expertise in a specific research/clinical domain for the study of insomnia. Those who contributed to the consensus statement and their respective specific fields of expertise are as follows: Dieter Riemann (DR) and Periklis Ktonas (PK) on polysomnography and sleep EEG spectral analysis; Adam Wichniak (AW) on actigraphy; Kai Spiegelhalder (KS) on neuroimaging; Thorsten Mikoteit (TM) and Martin Hatzinger (MH) on psychophysiology (heart rate, skin conductance, thermoregulation, oxygen consumption rate); TM, MH, and Anne Eckert (AE) on neuroendocrinology (HPA axis and melatonin), inflammation and neuroplasticity; Dimitris Dikeos (DD) and Constantin Soldatos (CS) on psychometrics through scales and questionnaires for assessing insomnia; Jana Kopřivová (JK) on psychometrics through scales and questionnaires for assessing dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep; and Tatiana Crönlein (TC) on inventories for assessing personality patterns. Maria Ntafouli (MN) contributed to data handling, preparation of tables, and the procedure of composing the paper.

The contributors provided their respective parts of the manuscript, each accompanied by a table presenting the studies on which their specific reports were based. The text provided by each contributor appears in part as an appropriate section of the review of studies and in part in the discussion; the unified text of the entire paper was based on the integration of the parts provided by the contributors implemented by DD and CS.

In their tables, the contributors included summarised information for each study as follows: Citation, Type of Study, Type and Number of Subjects, Instruments and Methods, Results, Main Conclusions, and Comments. The tables provided by the contributors were then homogenised by DD and CS through (a) presenting all data included in them in a similar way, (b) adding appropriate data to the individual entries (whenever those were not included in the tables originally provided by the contributors) after scrutinising the papers on which the entries were based, (c) calculating appropriate values of Cohen’s d or Hedge’s g based on the data available in each study (see Supplementary Table S1), and (d) adding/restructuring conclusions and comments by using information originating from the texts provided by the contributors and/or from the original papers cited by them. Moreover, the selection of entries which were to appear in the main tables of the paper was based on the following criteria: (a) study samples included well defined groups of insomniacs and control subjects, (b) study samples were not based only on special populations (e.g. elderly, children, patients with somatic disorders, etc.), and (c) respective papers were either reviews/meta-analyses or provided precise numerical values for diagnostic accuracy, effect size or other values reflecting the strength of differences between compared groups. Data of other studies which were referred to by the contributors but did not meet any of the above mentioned requirements were placed in the online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3. It should be noted that, based on each original study’s description, it was not always possible to specify whether patients included were diagnosed with any type of insomnia (INS) or specifically with primary insomnia (PI). Thus, we resorted to include in the tables the rubric used by the respective authors (e.g. INS, PI, etc.) and to base our analysis on patients with PI only whenever this was specifically mentioned or, otherwise, on unspecified insomnia patients.

The review section contains two main parts: one on Psychometric Measurements and another on Biological Measurements. Each of these parts is accompanied by its respective main table and the corresponding Supplementary Table in the online material. The two main tables are subdivided into several sections, each corresponding to a specific scientific area (psychometric or biological approach for the diagnosis of insomnia) and are organised in a chronological order based on the year of publication, whereas the entries in the Supplementary Tables are listed in alphabetical order based on first author’s surname.

A grading system for establishing the quality of diagnosis was created as follows: data provided in the individual studies which are included in the main tables were categorised in four levels (A to D) based on the degree of pertinence of their reported indices to diagnose insomnia (Grade of Diagnostic Pertinence, see ). Thus, ‘A’ is for studies directly assessing the data in a detection framework (e.g. ROC) while ‘B’ is for studies using only general distribution parameters like effect sizes or means and variances. The Diagnostic Accuracy potential of the psychometric or biological approach examined in each study was graded in four levels (1–4), as also shown in . Thus, a diagnostic grading index could be attributed to each study based on the combination of the grades of diagnostic pertinence and diagnostic accuracy. For example, a diagnostic grading index of A1 corresponds to a study whose results show high diagnostic accuracy which is based on measures of high pertinence (i.e. Sensitivity, Specificity, or ROC-AUC higher than 0.80), A3 corresponds to high quality diagnostic measures showing low diagnostic accuracy, while C1 corresponds to a study showing high diagnostic accuracy which is, however, based on measures of low pertinence. The cut-off scores used for establishing the level of diagnostic accuracy were based on relevant literature (Hosmer and Lemeshow Citation2000; Ferguson Citation2009; Sawilowsky Citation2009). In case a study reported several measures, the diagnostically pertinent index which was chosen was the most accurate one among these measures. Grade D of diagnostic pertinence corresponds to a study reporting possibly useful information on differences between insomniacs and controls, but the data provided are not adequate to assess the diagnostic validity of the existing differences; therefore, no level of diagnostic accuracy can be established.

Table 1. Grading system for establishing diagnosis.

Review of studies on psychometric measurements

Rating scales and questionnaires for assessing insomnia

There is a very large number of scales and questionnaires for diagnosing insomnia; therefore, a search based on Pubmed and secondary citations was performed for original papers, each having being cited at least ten times in ‘Web of Science’ or Google Scholar. The papers produced through this search had to report on suitable measures of diagnostic validity for insomnia, based on a well-defined population of insomniacs vs. a sample of non-insomniac controls. Consequently, five studies reporting on an equal number of psychometric instruments (Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, Buysse et al. Citation1989; Athens Insomnia Scale, Soldatos et al. Citation2000; Global Sleep Assessment Questionnaire, Roth et al. Citation2002; Sleep-50 Questionnaire, Spoormaker et al. Citation2005; Brief Insomnia Questionnaire, Kessler et al. Citation2010) were finally selected for presentation herein.

As presented in , the diagnostic validity of all five psychometric instruments was quite satisfactory, based on measures of sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value (PPV), negative predictive value (NPV), and area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (ROC-AUC). The values corresponding to these measures indicate that scales and questionnaires for assessing insomnia are indeed useful psychometric tools (diagnostic grading A1 for three of the above scales and A2 for two of them).

Table 2. Studies of psychometric measurements in diagnosing insomnia.

Further to their diagnostic validity, Cronbach’s alphas were invariably high (0.77–0.90), indicating the strong internal consistency of the five instruments presented above. In studies of other well-known scales and questionnaires for assessing insomnia, which are not presented here as they do not meet the inclusion criteria mentioned above, similarly high values of Cronbach coefficients were also observed (Parrott and Hindmarch Citation1980; Ellis et al. Citation1981; Jenkins et al. Citation1988; Bastien et al. Citation2001; Pallesen et al. Citation2008; Okun et al. Citation2009; Drake et al. Citation2014; Espie et al. Citation2014). Test-retest reliability, in the four studies in which it was assessed (), was found to range from 0.78 to 0.90 (for periods of 1–3 weeks). The high values of both Cronbach’s alpha and test-retest reliability show that the psychometric instruments (scales and questionnaires) focussing on the assessment of the subjective complaints of insomnia are indeed robust and retain their ability to record sleep disturbance consistently over repeated administration.

Scales and questionnaires for assessing dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep

Cognitive processes play an important role in insomnia (Harvey Citation2002). To quantify sleep-related cognitions, the Dysfunctional Beliefs and Attitudes about Sleep scale (DBAS) has been developed by Morin et al. (Citation1993, see Supplementary Table S2), containing 30-items. More recently, however, shorter versions have been published (DBAS-16 by Morin et al. Citation2007, see Supplementary Table S2; DBAS-10 by Espie et al. Citation2000 and by Edinger and Wohlgemuth Citation2001, see Supplementary Table S2). Psychometric and clinical studies reported satisfactory internal consistency of all DBAS versions (e.g. Smith and Trinder Citation2001, see ; Carney and Edinger Citation2006; Morin et al. Citation2007, see Supplementary Table S2; Carney et al. Citation2010, see ; Fortier-Brochu and Morin Citation2014; Hertenstein et al. Citation2015, see ; Palagini et al. Citation2016).

Four studies demonstrated that DBAS adequately differentiates between insomnia patients and good sleepers (Smith and Trinder Citation2001, diagnostic grading A1; Carney et al. Citation2010, diagnostic grading A2 for the primary insomnia group; Hertenstein et al. Citation2015, diagnostic grading B1; Crönlein et al. Citation2014, diagnostic grading B2; see ). DBAS score is also positively correlated with insomnia severity (Bluestein et al. Citation2010; Crönlein et al. Citation2014, see ; Seow et al. Citation2018), although increased DBAS scores have been also reported in other sleep disorders, such as restless leg syndrome or restless leg syndrome with sleep apnoea (Crönlein et al. Citation2014).

Besides DBAS, other scales focussing on cognitive processes have also been found to be useful in diagnosing insomnia: the Glasgow Content of Thoughts Inventory (Harvey and Espie Citation2004, diagnostic grading A1, see ) and the Glasgow Sleep Effort Scale (Broomfield and Espie Citation2005, diagnostic grading A1; Hertenstein et al. Citation2015, diagnostic grading B1; see ).

Inventories of assessing personality patterns

In the early eighties, a study of insomniacs vs. good sleepers based on the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) found more frequently at least one pathologically elevated scale (79 vs. 29%) and higher scores of clinical scales D (depression), Pt (psychasthenia), Hy (hysteria), Hs (hypochondriasis) in insomniacs, denoting a personality profile characterised by depression, rumination, anxiety and inhibition of emotions in insomnia (Kales et al. Citation1983, diagnostic grading A2, see ). Subsequent studies have produced similar findings, as reviewed by Van de Laar et al. (Citation2010, diagnostic grading C2, see ) showing that, in the majority of studies, the scales which were pathologically elevated were indeed D, Hy, Hs, and Pt.

In studies examining personality traits (such as the Big Five, Costa and McCrae Citation1992) in insomniacs, a high score on neuroticism was found to be the strongest predictor of insomnia, when poor sleep was assessed either subjectively or objectively (Williams and Moroz Citation2009; Duggan et al. Citation2014; Hintsanen et al. Citation2014; Stephan et al. Citation2018; Križan and Hisler Citation2019); however, the association of neuroticism with insomnia was not found to be diagnostically useful (Stephan et al. Citation2018, diagnostic grading C4, see ). On the other hand, extraversion and positive affectivity were associated with subjectively reported good sleep quality in studies that did not meet the criteria to be included herein (Gray and Watson Citation2002; Williams and Moroz Citation2009; Duggan et al. Citation2014).

Studies evaluating other personality characteristics beyond those assessed through the MMPI and the Big Five have shown that patients with chronic insomnia were more perfectionistic than healthy controls (Lundh et al. Citation1994; Vincent and Walker Citation2000). Cross-sectional studies, as well as longitudinal studies, confirmed significant associations between perfectionism and poor sleep even when measured with polysomnography (Vincent and Walker Citation2000; Azevedo et al. Citation2010; Jansson-Fröjmark et al. Citation2011; Johann et al. Citation2017, see Supplementary Table S2), although the available data did not allow to establish the diagnostic usefulness of measures of perfectionism in insomnia.

Review of studies on biological measurements

Polysomnography and sleep EEG analysis

To clarify the clinical value of polysomnography for diagnosing insomnia Baglioni, Regen, et al. (Citation2014, see ) conducted a meta-analysis including all polysomnographic studies published up to that time in 582 patients with chronic primary insomnia and 485 good sleeper controls. Many significant differences were found between insomniacs and good sleepers as reflected in reduced sleep efficiency, increased sleep onset latency, reduced total sleep time, increased number of awakenings, reduced slow wave sleep and REM sleep, and increased wake time. However, the between groups difference for total sleep time was only 25 min (Cohen’s d = −0.6); also the d-values for all PSG measures indicating reduced sleep quantity were generally low, the highest being for Sleep Efficiency and Slow Wave Sleep duration (−0.7 and −0.8, respectively), which correspond to a weak level of evidence for diagnostic accuracy (diagnostic grading B3). Interestingly, based on sleep diary data, the effect between groups was much stronger, with an average difference of ∼2 h for subjectively estimated total sleep time.

Table 3. Studies of biological measurements in diagnosing insomnia.

Through spectral analysis of sleep EEG, Spiegelhalder et al. (Citation2012, see ) were able to demonstrate increased amounts of fast frequencies (in the sigma and beta bands), although the level of diagnostic accuracy at this finding was weak (diagnostic grading B3). It should also be noted that Colombo et al. (Citation2016), based on high-density EEG, demonstrated that increased amounts of beta waves can also be detected in the resting-state EEG of patients with insomnia during wakefulness.

Riedner et al. (Citation2016, see ) analysing all-night NREM sleep EEG found more power in high frequency EEG activity (>16 Hz) in chronic insomnia subjects vs. controls widespread across the scalp, indicating a possible hyperarousal state in chronic insomnia, even during the deepest stage of sleep. Buysse et al. (Citation2008, see ) found evidence of hyperarousal (elevated EEG power in 18–40 Hz) only in the first NREM sleep period of insomniacs vs. controls. However, the results of this study as well as those of the study by Svetnik et al. (Citation2017, see ) indicate that hyperarousal during sleep in insomnia may relate to female subjects only. Nonetheless, data provided in the above three studies were insufficient for establishing diagnostic validity (grade D for all of them). Moreover, Schwabedal et al. (Citation2016, see Supplementary Table S3) found that the alpha wave frequency during sleep arousals in male insomniacs is similar to that during wake, while it is less than wake levels in controls, indicating a possible presence of hyperarousal during sleep also in male insomnia subjects.

Another possibility to analyse polysomnography is the so-called cyclic alternating pattern (Terzano et al. Citation2001). This is a phenomenon of NREM sleep and shows some overlap with certain Rechtschaffen and Kales events (mostly arousals). The cyclic alternating pattern (CAP) is based on the observation that certain EEG elements, distinct from background EEG, recur with a periodicity of 20–40 s. It marks an arousal fluctuation, containing unstable or aroused parts of sleep alternating with consolidated sleep. A large CAP rate marks disturbed sleep with poor sleep quality. Chouvarda et al. (Citation2012, see ) showed that altered CAP patterns differentiate insomniacs from good sleepers, with satisfactory diagnostic accuracy (diagnostic grading A1) arguing for a destabilisation of sleep in insomnia.

When considering the diagnostic validity of sleep EEG-derived measures based on the grading system for establishing diagnosis, as applied to the studies of , CAP rate emerges as the most reliable biomarker for diagnosing insomnia (diagnostic grading A1), while there is rather weak evidence that reduced sleep efficiency and SWS duration (as measures of sleep macro-architecture) as well as the increased power of fast frequency EEG bands sigma and beta (as measures of sleep micro-architecture) may also be considered as potential biomarkers (diagnostic grading B3 for measures of both macro- and micro-architecture of sleep). It should be noted, however, that the high diagnostic grading found for CAP is based on just one study as yet.

Actigraphy

Actigraphy is a non-invasive method for monitoring physical activity and the rest-activity cycle. In sleep medicine it is used to assess circadian sleep-wake rhythm and the duration of rest periods (Wichniak et al. Citation2017; Smith et al. Citation2018); yet, the usefulness of this method in the assessment of insomnia is still being discussed. Actually, many studies on actigraphy in insomnia report only group differences and their level of significance, without providing diagnostic cut-off scores of derived sleep parameters and their sensitivity, specificity, and predictive values.

Actigraphy as a diagnostic method suffers also from an absence of common standards of technical solutions and software algorithms for detecting sleep; thus, published results of studies using a certain device cannot be generalised to apply to devices of other manufacturers. Another obstacle for the use of actigraphy as a diagnostic or research biomarker for insomnia is related to its lower accuracy to detect wakefulness in case of low sleep efficiency, since insomniacs may frequently lie immobile in bed while not asleep. Nonetheless, two large studies that reported on the diagnostic accuracy of actigraphy for insomnia () show that actigraphy has very satisfactory diagnostic validity (Natale et al. Citation2009, Citation2014), diagnostic grading A1 and A2, respectively. In addition, it seems that actigraphy can also be useful for the prediction of treatment response to Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for insomnia (Bathgate et al. Citation2017, see Supplementary Table S3).

Heart rate and heart rate variability

Psychophysiological stress and the consequent arousal lead to an increase of heart rate (HR) and to a decrease of heart rate variability (HRV), i.e. of the variability of time between consecutive heart beats, commonly referred to as the RR (R-wave to R-wave) intervals. Electrocardiographic (ECG) recordings during wake time or sleep allow the assessment of RR-intervals, which vary as a function of autonomic nervous system (for more details see Malik et al. Citation1996; Stein and Pu Citation2012). Spectral analyses describe the oscillations of RR-intervals in three frequency bands: high frequency power (HF), reflecting parasympathetic input; low frequency power (LF), which depends on both sympathetic and parasympathetic input; very low frequency power (VLF), which is affected by parasympathetic influences. An indicator of the sympathovagal balance appears to be the LF/HF-ratio (but, see for opposite positions Billman Citation2013).

In insomnia, HR was shown to be increased (or heart period shortened) especially in the wake period before sleep and in early stage 2 sleep, although the level of diagnostic accuracy of this marker was found to be variable across studies (Bonnet and Arand Citation1998; de Zambotti et al. Citation2013; Farina et al. Citation2014, diagnostic grading ranging from B1 to B3, see ).

Specific alterations of HRV patterns have been reported in insomniacs (Tobaldini et al. Citation2013). Bonnet and Arand (Citation1998, see ) in a small sample of individuals with objectively defined insomnia found increased LF and decreased HF during all sleep stages; this pattern of results mirrored the expected alteration in sympathovagal balance. Others examined HRV of insomniacs vs. healthy controls in resting state, before and after sleep onset, or during specific sleep stages (Fang et al. Citation2008; de Zambotti et al. Citation2013; Farina et al. Citation2014, see ); two patterns emerged for insomnia: either, there was no difference between LF or HF, or there was a tendency for decreased HF as indicator for decreased parasympathetic tone (Fang et al. Citation2008) or an increase in LF as indicator for increased sympathetic activity (de Zambotti et al. Citation2013; Farina et al. Citation2014). Findings were most prominent in wake before sleep onset and Stage 2 sleep (Farina et al. Citation2014). To explain these inconsistent results, sleep duration should be considered as a possible confounder. Findings on HF seem to refer prevalently to insomnia with short sleep duration, while HF and LF was normal in insomnia with normal sleep duration (Spiegelhalder et al. Citation2011, see ; Miller et al. Citation2016, see Supplementary Table S3). Nevertheless, the level of diagnostic accuracy of HRV, wherever it was possible to be assessed, was virtually absent (diagnostic grading B4). On the same line, Dodds et al. (Citation2017) having thoroughly reviewed 22 studies on HRV and insomnia concluded that the heterogeneity in regards to sample selection, methods of HRV assessment, and reporting bias precluded empirical evidence to support the hypothesis of HRV impairment in insomnia.

To summarise, an increased HR before and after sleep onset can be considered as a promising psychophysiological biomarker for diagnosing insomnia (diagnostic grading ranging from B1 to B3). Based on the reviewed studies, however, HRV does not seem to be useful as a diagnostic biomarker for insomnia (diagnostic grading B4).

Skin conductance

Skin conductance level (SCL) is another psychophysiological index of ANS, representing the sympathetic activity in reaction to emotional stimuli. In an experimental habituation study in wake time, Broman and Hetta (Citation1994, see ) found in individuals with insomnia compared to healthy controls increased SCL (without, however, providing indices useful for estimation of diagnostic accuracy) and poor habituation to repeated stimuli, although the diagnostic sensitivity of the latter for insomnia was very low (40%), indicating that the habituation paradigm cannot be used as a diagnostic marker (grading A4). In contrast to the finding of an increased SCL in insomnia during daytime, which is compatible with the hyperarousal model, other findings of SCL during sleep were shown to be inconsistent in a review paper by Riemann et al. (Citation2010).

Thermoregulation

Thermoregulation refers to the circadian rhythm of core body temperature and skin temperature (Lack et al. Citation2008; Te Lindert and Van Someren Citation2018). Healthy sleep goes along with a decline in core body temperature at night. In contrast, the desynchronisation between wake-sleep and temperature rhythms may underlie certain specific types of insomnia (Morris et al. Citation1990, see Table S3; Lack et al. Citation2008). Accordingly, objective sleep maintenance insomnia in the elderly was related to elevated nocturnal core body temperature (Lushington et al. Citation2000, see Supplementary Table S3), which might be associated with hyperarousal and hypermetabolism. Increased core body temperature in insomniacs vs. normal sleepers, as shown by Gradisar et al. (Citation2006, see ), could be considered as a potential biomarker of insomnia based on a finding of low diagnostic accuracy (diagnostic grading B3). Moreover, while warming of the body is associated with increased sleep propensity in healthy sleepers, this does not seem to be the case for individuals with insomnia (Raymann et al. Citation2007, see Supplementary Table S3).

Oxygen consumption rate

Overall oxygen use (VO2) as a proxy of whole body metabolic rate enables to assess the physiological activity. Chapman et al. (Citation2018) concluded in a systematic review that metabolic rate in insomnia is modestly elevated during the day and throughout the night. This finding is in line with the hyperarousal model, which refers both to wake and to sleep time. Remarkably, the metabolic rate was not only found to be increased in objectively verified insomnia (Bonnet and Arand Citation1996), but was also shown to be increased in ‘sleep state misperception’ insomnia, albeit to a lesser degree (Bonnet and Arand Citation1998), as shown in , with diagnostic grading being B3 and B4, respectively. Consequently, there is some evidence that VO2 consumption rate may be considered as a potential biomarker for objectively verified insomnia.

Neuroimaging

In the last two decades, neuroradiological and nuclear medicine techniques have been used to investigate biological markers of insomnia. The first study in this field acquired single photon emission computerised tomography (SPECT) data from five patients with insomnia and four healthy controls (Smith et al. Citation2002, see ), and surprisingly, reported a reduced cerebral blood flow in medial frontal, parietal and occipital brain areas as well as in the basal ganglia of insomnia patients during NREM sleep. This finding, which shows high diagnostic validity (grading B1), is in disagreement with the hyperarousal model of insomnia (Riemann et al. Citation2010), and has not been replicated.

Some further studies used 18F-FDG-PET ([18F]fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography) to quantify brain metabolic patterns. Nofzinger et al. (Citation2004) investigated seven patients with insomnia in comparison to 20 healthy controls during NREM sleep and wakefulness. One of the key findings was that patients with insomnia had a smaller decrease in brain metabolism from wakefulness to NREM sleep than healthy controls, in particular in the ascending reticular activating system (ARAS) as well as in areas related to emotion regulation (amygdala, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex) and cognition (prefrontal cortex). This finding has been partially replicated in a later study from the same group of 44 patients with insomnia and 40 healthy controls (Kay et al. Citation2016, diagnostic grading D, see ). A further study of this group suggests that brain areas involved in conscious awareness might also be involved in the perception of wake-sleep transitions (Kay et al. Citation2017), which is characteristically altered in insomnia (Harvey and Tang Citation2012).

Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in insomnia can be divided into those that used specific tasks in the scanner and those that investigated functional connectivity during resting state conditions. One task-related study investigated 22 patients with insomnia and 38 healthy controls and suggested an increased reactivity of the amygdala in patients with insomnia in response to the presentation of sleep-related stimuli (Baglioni, Spiegelhalder, et al. Citation2014). Furthermore, during the performance of cognitive tasks, a reduced recruitment of frontal brain areas has been reported in several studies using Tower of London, working memory, and category and letter fluency tasks (Altena et al. Citation2008 with diagnostic grading B1, see ; Drummond et al. Citation2013; Stoffers et al. Citation2014). More recently, Seo et al. (Citation2018) reported a delayed deactivation of the fear extinction network in 23 patients with insomnia compared to an equal number of healthy controls.

Resting-state functional connectivity (RSFC) in insomnia was investigated in many recent studies; however, the results are clearly very inconsistent, partly due to a liberal use of statistical significance thresholds. Most notably, Leerssen et al. (Citation2019, see ) reported an increased connectivity between the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex in 65 patients with insomnia compared to 65 healthy controls (diagnostic grading B1) while Regen et al. (Citation2016, see ) did not find significant group differences in 20 patients with insomnia and 20 healthy controls; however, RSFC components were found to be weakly correlated to sleep efficiency and sleep latency (diagnostic grading C3).

Reviewing the literature summarised above, previous narrative reviews came to the conclusion that corticolimbic overactivity is a key feature of patients with insomnia (e.g. Riemann et al. Citation2015). However, an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis reported a lack of consistent brain alterations in insomnia across studies (Tahmasian et al. Citation2018). While this kind of meta-analysis has specific strengths and limitations, it still appears to be too early to draw definitive conclusions based on reported data from neuroradiological and nuclear medicine studies on insomnia. In particular, small sample sizes in combination with purely exploratory data analysis strategies limit the validity of this literature and confer a high risk for false negative and false positive findings (Button et al. Citation2013). Moreover, based on our data it seems that some studies provide evidence that various indices could have diagnostic validity for insomnia (see where diagnostic grading values are B1 for three studies and C3 for another one); yet, the inconsistencies and large diversity of these indices make it difficult to establish common standards across neuroimaging approaches for diagnostic purposes.

HPA axis

Findings of alterations in cortisol levels in individuals with insomnia have been inconsistent. Some studies described statistically significantly elevated ACTH and cortisol levels in the evening and in the first half of the night in insomniacs compared to healthy controls (Vgontzas et al. Citation2001 and Seelig et al. Citation2013, see ; Steiger Citation2007; Zhang et al. Citation2014, see Supplementary Table S3), and other studies did not (Riemann et al. Citation2002; van Neijenhof et al. Citation2018). Different degrees of insomnia severity across studies may explain these discrepant findings: individuals with insomnia and a shortened total sleep time (TST) demonstrate elevated ACTH and cortisol concentrations in the first half of the night and also in the morning; in contrast, individuals with insomnia and a normal or only slightly decreased TST show unaltered cortisol levels (Vgontzas et al. Citation2001 see ; Fernandez-Mendoza et al. Citation2014; D'Aurea et al. Citation2015, see Supplementary Table S3).

A similarly inconsistent pattern of results is observed for the morning HPA axis activity: salivary morning cortisol after awakening was either decreased (Backhaus et al. Citation2004, see Supplementary Table S3; Mikoteit et al. Citation2019, see ) or comparable to cortisol levels of normal sleepers (Seelig et al. Citation2013; Zhang et al. Citation2014; van Neijenhof et al. Citation2018). Lower morning cortisol concentrations correlated with lower sleep quality and with non-restorative sleep (Backhaus et al. Citation2004; Mikoteit et al. Citation2019). On the other hand short objective TST was related to increased morning cortisol levels (D'Aurea et al. Citation2015; Fernandez-Mendoza et al. Citation2017). Finally, the dexamethasone/corticotropin releasing hormone (Dex/CRH) test, the gold standard of a pharmacological HPA axis challenge, does not show a hyperactivity of HPA axis in insomnia (Lattova et al. Citation2011, see Supplementary Table S3). One explanation for this finding may be that chronic insomnia is often associated only with a mild to moderate shortened sleep time, which appears to allow for adaptation of the HPA axis activity.

Regarding the diagnostic usefulness of HPA axis measures as a biomarker for insomnia, one study showed that higher 24-h serum ACTH and cortisol levels can distinguish insomniacs from healthy controls (Vgontzas et al. Citation2001, see ; diagnostic grading B1), while for three other studies using either evening and nocturnal serum cortisol levels (Riemann et al. Citation2002, see ) or midnight and morning salivary cortisol levels (Seelig et al. Citation2013 and Mikoteit et al. Citation2019, see ) diagnostic grading was B4.

Melatonin

The hormone melatonin is an important moderator of the circadian sleep-wake rhythm. The nocturnal darkness-dependent rise of melatonin levels induces proneness to sleep and seems to be also involved in the physiological sleep structure characterised by the preponderance of SWS in the first half of the night (Zhang et al. Citation2014; Xie et al. Citation2017). Moreover, melatonin is considered to regulate the circadian rhythm of the HPA axis activity (Lattova et al. Citation2011). Melatonin has been found to start its rise earlier in the evening and to have a lower peak at night in insomniacs compared to healthy controls; this finding has a satisfactory diagnostic validity for insomnia (Hajak et al. Citation1995 and Riemann et al. Citation2002; for both B2 diagnostic grading, see ).

Markers of inflammation

Insomnia was related to moderately increased CRP levels and unrelated to IL-6 and TNF-α levels (Okun et al. Citation2009; Laugsand et al. Citation2012, see Supplementary Table S3; Slavish et al. Citation2018). Higher CRP levels were also observed in patients with insomnia persisting over a six-year period compared to non-insomniacs as well as to patients with non-persistent insomnia, although the data in that study (being expressed as geometric means only) were not pertinent for the assessment of the usefulness of CRP level as a biomarker of insomnia (Parthasarathy et al. Citation2015, see , diagnostic grading D).

Markers of neuroplasticity

After controlling for comorbid depression, individuals with insomnia had lower serum BDNF levels than healthy controls (Giese et al. Citation2014, see Supplementary Table S3). A subsequent study provided support for this finding, also showing that serum BDNF seems to be a valuable biomarker for insomnia irrespectively of objectively assessed sleep continuity (Mikoteit et al. Citation2019, diagnostic grading A1, see ). In another study, as well, low BDNF levels were found to be a biomarker for the diagnosis of insomnia with short sleep duration in PSG, as well as of insomnia with normal sleep duration (Fan et al. Citation2019, diagnostic grading B1, see ).

Among individuals with insomnia, lower BDNF levels were associated with a lower percentage of REM sleep, although this was not the case for objective sleep continuity parameters assessed through polysomnography (Deuschle et al. Citation2018, see Supplementary Table S3; Mikoteit et al. Citation2019). Finally, an association between low BDNF levels and poor cognitive performance has been found in individuals with insomnia and shortened sleep duration (Fan et al. Citation2019, see ).

Discussion

This is the first study to establish the usefulness of various methods for the diagnosis of insomnia by comparing diagnostic validity indices of psychometric and biological measurements. To achieve this goal, a diagnostic grading system was developed consisting of the estimation of both diagnostic pertinence and diagnostic accuracy for the evaluation of the measurements used in each study. In most studies reviewed, there was adequate data to extract an index of diagnostic discrimination, provided that the actual diagnosis of each study’s sample had been performed by accepted means (i.e. following recommendation of diagnostic manuals); it must be noted, however, that in a few studies, it was stated that the patient group had the diagnosis of insomnia, without specifying the exact diagnostic procedure. Some of the studies examined did not provide data adequate enough for the evaluation of diagnostic accuracy (grade D of diagnostic pertinence); nonetheless, their data on the existing differences between insomniacs and control subjects are still scientifically valuable for understanding the nature and mechanisms of insomnia as well as to potentially guide treatment practices (Dikeos and Soldatos Citation2005; Pevernagie Citation2021).

As expected, scales and questionnaires specifically developed for diagnosing insomnia were found to have the highest diagnostic gradings; this is understandable taking into account that the diagnostic criteria for insomnia are based solely on subjective complaints of poor sleep quantity and/or quality, without including any biological or other objectively measurable parameters (APA 2013; AASM 2014; WHO 2019). This finding shows that the assessment of subjective sleep complaints, which actually constitute the condition of insomnia, reflects the impact of insomnia on the suffering of the individual and the accompanying help-seeking behaviour. Moreover, the existence of cut-off scores of insomnia scales allows them to be useful tools in clinical and research practice for reliably establishing the diagnosis of insomnia (Soldatos et al. Citation2003).

The fact that high diagnostic gradings were also found for scales and questionnaires assessing beliefs and attitudes about sleep is not surprising since the dysfunctional beliefs about sleep are intrinsically related to the development of insomnia (Doos Ali Vand et al. Citation2014; Yang et al. Citation2014), as well as to its persistence (Norell-Clarke et al. Citation2014). In addition, these dysfunctional beliefs have been associated with treatment adherence (Cvengros et al. Citation2015) and treatment response (Tremblay et al. Citation2009); thus, their knowledge by clinicians may guide optimal treatment choice (Montserrat Sánchez-Ortuño and Edinger Citation2010). Consequently, it seems that scales on dysfunctional beliefs about sleep are useful tools for the prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of insomnia.

The potential of MMPI as a tool for diagnosing insomnia was found to be quite satisfactory, although lower than that of the scales and questionnaires mentioned above. The personality profile of insomniacs according to MMPI is characterised by a pattern of elevation of certain scales denoting the presence of neurotic depression, rumination, anxiety, and internalisation of negative emotions leading to psychophysiological activation and ultimately to the vicious cycle of generation and perpetuation of insomnia (Kales et al. Citation1983; Dikeos and Soldatos Citation2005; Van de Laar et al. Citation2010). It is noteworthy that in a study by Vgontzas et al. (Citation1994) comparing the validity and clinical utility of sleep laboratory measurements for diagnosing insomnia to those of MMPI patterns, the latter were found to have a better positive predictive value than the former (73 vs. 48%, p < 0.01). Neuroticism and perfectionism were also found to be predictors of insomnia through the Big Five inventory and other personality assessment tools; however, contrary to the MMPI, these tools do not seem to be useful for diagnosing insomnia. Neurotic persons show negative mood, anxiety, dysfunctional thoughts, as well as behaviours and coping strategies leading to emotional distress (Costa and McCrae Citation1992). This may add to vulnerability for stress-related sleep disturbance and the development of insomnia (Williams and Moroz Citation2009; Van de Laar et al. Citation2010; Harvey et al. Citation2014).

It has been also reported that personality characteristics, degree of psychopathology, comorbid psychiatric disorders, and levels of sleep disturbance can be used to subclassify insomnia into various phenotypes; early onset insomnia was the most severe of these phenotypes (Van de Laar et al. Citation2017, see Supplementary Table S2). In another study, five robust insomnia disorder subtypes have been identified based on life history, personality traits, and affect (Blanken et al. Citation2019, see Supplementary Table S2), opening the door widely for further research.

Etiologic models of insomnia emphasise the bidirectional interaction between psychological symptoms, such as extensive worrying about sleep (Harvey Citation2002), and physiological conditions, such as hyperarousal (Riemann et al. Citation2010). Insomnia patients trapped in this vicious cycle show more or less homogenous patterns of personality and psychopathology, although whether the persistence of insomnia leads to further changes in personality or vice versa has not been clearly understood (Van de Laar et al. Citation2010).

The review on the usefulness of biological measures for the diagnosis of insomnia showed that these measures have a lower diagnostic potential than that of scales and questionnaires. It also showed that there is a marked variability of the level of evidence across studies both for the diagnostic pertinence of measures provided as well as for the diagnostic accuracy of the methods examined.

The evidence for diagnostic accuracy of PSG-based macroarchitecture of sleep as diagnostic marker for insomnia is shown to be rather weak. For many years it has been reasonably assumed that PSG would be a very helpful tool for the diagnosis of insomnia. Yet, this has not been proven to be the case and, consequently, US guidelines (Chesson et al. Citation2000) do not routinely recommend polysomnography (PSG) as a diagnostic tool for insomnia in clinical practice, while the European guidelines (Riemann et al. Citation2017) suggest a broader indication to perform polysomnography only in certain cases of insomnia. One of the main reasons for the hesitant approach to include PSG in the diagnostic process for insomnia resulted from the fact that polysomnographic data do not strongly correlate with the subjective assessment of sleep by insomniac patients themselves. An early study to document this (Carskadon et al. Citation1976), clearly showed that patients with insomnia consistently underestimate the time spent asleep and overestimate the time it takes them to fall asleep, denoting that there is a significant discrepancy between objective and subjective findings (for overview see Harvey and Tang Citation2012). However, in spite of the overall weak diagnostic potential of PSG, objectively assessed sleep quantity parameters may distinguish insomniacs from those with short or with normal sleep duration, with the former constituting the most biologically severe phenotype, with significant therapeutic and prognostic implications (Vgontzas et al. Citation2013; Bertisch et al. Citation2018).

Since the publication of the neurocognitive model of insomnia (Perlis et al. Citation1997), spectral analysis of sleep EEG is assumed to provide a more fine grained approach to evaluate changes in sleep of patients with insomnia; fast frequencies (in the sigma/beta range), which seem to be overexpressed in the sleep of insomniac patients, may reflect increased levels of consciousness during sleep, thus corresponding or even reflecting the increased ruminations patients with insomnia typically suffer from. As shown in , all four studies reviewed indicate the presence of sleep EEG spectral analysis findings denoting a considerable degree of hyperarousal during sleep; however, in three of them, the data reported were inadequate to establish diagnostic validity (Buysse et al. Citation2008; Riedner et al. Citation2016; Svetnik et al. Citation2017), while the diagnostic grading of the remaining one (Spiegelhalder et al. Citation2012) corresponds to low diagnostic accuracy. To the contrary, a study on CAP rate analysis showed high diagnostic accuracy (Chouvarda et al. Citation2012). In general, the evidence from studies applying more fine-grained neurophysiological methods, such as sleep EEG spectral analysis or CAP rate analysis, favours the hypothesis of an increased CNS hyperarousal in patients with insomnia in relation to good sleepers (Riemann et al. Citation2010, Citation2015; Morin et al. Citation2015; Zhao et al. Citation2021), which may be reduced after cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia (Cervena et al. Citation2004; Krystal and Edinger Citation2010, see Supplementary Table S2 for both).

Actigraphy seems to be a valuable method for the diagnosis of insomnia, with the studies reviewed herein showing high diagnostic accuracy (Natale et al. Citation2009, Citation2014), although the quantification of the resulting recordings and the establishment of relevant diagnostic cut-offs have not been achieved as yet. Actually, recent guidelines for the treatment of insomnia (Riemann et al. Citation2017), recommend actigraphy only in the case of clinical suspicion of irregular sleep-wake schedule or circadian rhythm disorders (strong recommendation), but to a much smaller extent for the quantitative assessment of sleep parameters (weak recommendation). On the other hand, the value of actigraphy is also supported by the fact that actigraphs are easy to use across many nights with almost no impact on patient’s activities and habits. Finally, with the existing pace of progress regarding personal electronic devices and wearables, we can look forward to new methods of collection and analysis of actigraphic data.

The studies presented in show that an increase of heart rate before and after sleep onset can be considered as a psychophysiological biomarker for the diagnosis of insomnia. To the contrary, no diagnostic usefulness has been shown for heart rate variability (HRV), although alterations of HRV patterns have been reported, as an indication of autonomic hyperarousal and a sympathovagal disbalance in insomnia. Consequently, it has been suggested that more research needs to be conducted including further HRV measures, such as VLF, non-linear complexity indices, or qEEG-ECG coupling analyses (Jurysta et al. Citation2009; Yang et al. Citation2011; Mikoteit et al. Citation2017, Citation2019).

Regarding other psychophysiological measures, which have been studied in insomnia and are reviewed in this paper (), an increased core body temperature in insomniancs shows some promise for diagnostic potential, although the procedure of measuring core body temperature is obviously impractical for clinical purposes. Oxygen consumption rate has also shown to have some diagnostic ability, but not for insomniacs with only subjective complaint; the clinical application of this method, however, is also impractical. As far as skin conductance is concerned, it has not shown diagnostic accuracy. In spite of the lack of usefulness of the above methods for diagnostic purposes, converging evidence from many studies on different psychophysiological markers of the autonomic nervous system (ANS), support the hyperarousal model for insomnia (Riemann et al. Citation2010). However, there is a need for further research to clarify if indicators of hyperarousal are secondary to sleep disturbance and fragmentation caused by insomnia, or if hyperarousal is a primary background of insomnia itself.

Neuroimaging studies, especially those based on PET scan and fMRI, generally indicate that in insomnia there is a failure of arousal mechanisms to decline in activity from wake to sleep; besides, in certain studies, a lower recruitment of frontal areas during cognitive tasks has been observed. While these findings are in keeping with the understanding that insomnia is characterised by the persistence of hyperarousal, it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions due to small number of participants and the variability of methods used across various studies. Also, even if the diagnostic validity is satisfactory, as shown in certain studies, the use of the neuroimaging methods for diagnostic purposes in insomnia is practically prohibited by their complexity and high cost. The recently established sleep working group within the ENIGMA neuroimaging genetic consortium constitutes a promising international effort, which is expected to provide further insight regarding neuroimaging of sleep disorders (Tahmasian et al. Citation2018).

The development and maintenance of insomnia is most often associated with chronic stress (Basta et al. Citation2007; Lo Martire et al. Citation2020). It appears plausible that insomnia is associated with alterations of the neuroendocrine stress response system, i.e. with the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, and with increased cortisol concentrations. Actually, certain studies on the HPA activity indices show that cortisol levels and ACTH are elevated in insomnia either throughout the 24-h period or at certain time-points before, during, or after sleep. The findings of some other studies, however, did not show differences between insomniacs and controls for cortisol or ACTH levels, neither for the Dexamethasone Suppression Test. Differences in hormonal sampling methods (serum vs. saliva and/or collection time-points) across studies may partly explain such discrepancies; also, participant characteristics may play a role, since it has been shown that HPA axis irregularities are more pronounced in insomniacs with objectively verified short sleeping time (Vgontzas et al. Citation2001; Fernandez-Mendoza et al. Citation2017). Regarding the value of HPA axis activity indices for diagnosing insomnia, in only one of the two studies on 24-h cortisol and ACTH serum levels reviewed herein, the level of diagnostic accuracy was high; moreover, in both studies on salivary cortisol the differences found between insomniacs and controls were significant, but not strong enough to reach even a low level of diagnostic accuracy.

Plasma melatonin levels are lower among insomniacs than among good sleepers, regarding the entire night and especially the first half and the middle of the night. Melatonin levels, also, start rising earlier in the evening, thus showing a more flattened circadian rhythm in insomnia. This finding seems to be independent of objectively assessed sleep parameters through PSG. The association of melatonin levels with insomnia is further strengthened by the fact that melatonin administration has been found to improve sleep onset insomnia (Brzezinski et al. Citation2005) both in children (Van Der Heijden et al. Citation2005, Citation2007; Van Maanen et al. Citation2017) and in adults (Lemoine et al. Citation2007; Luthringer et al. Citation2009). Melatonin level differences between insomniacs and controls in the two studies reviewed herein (Hajak et al. Citation1995; Riemann et al. Citation2002) show a satisfactory level of diagnostic accuracy, although the fact that this finding regards night-time blood sampling poses limitations for its clinical application.

The levels of C-reactive protein (CRP), as a marker of inflammation, were found to be moderately elevated in insomnia (particularly when its course is chronic) in comparison to normal levels, but no diagnostic use of this marker could be established based on the study reviewed herein. Since insomnia, especially in its chronic form, is characterised by stress and anxiety, it is expected to be accompanied by high levels of markers of inflammation. CRP and proinflammatory cytokines, such as Interleukin-6 (IL-6) and tumour necrosis factor (TNF-α), have been reported to be elevated in individuals with chronically disturbed sleep and to mediate the association between chronic sleep deprivation and somatic diseases, e.g. inflammatory diseases or cancer (Irwin et al. Citation2016). However, of the above markers, insomnia was only found to be associated with elevated CRP, while, in addition, chronically elevated CRP levels were not found to mediate the association between insomnia and mortality (Parthasarathy et al. Citation2015), thus obscuring the exact relationship of inflammation to either insomnia or chronic sleep deprivation.

Serum levels of BDNF, a marker of neuroplasticity, have been found to be significantly reduced in insomniacs compared with normal controls; the diagnostic accuracy of low BDNF for insomnia was high in both of the two studies reviewed herein. It is known that serum levels of BDNF correlate with central BDNF concentrations (Duman and Monteggia Citation2006) and that synaptic homeostasis and memory consolidation occur during sleep (Tononi and Cirelli Citation2014), which suggests that poor sleep may be associated with impaired neuroplasticity. Also, an acute stress challenge, such as acute sleep deprivation, increases BDNF levels; in contrast, chronically poor sleep is associated with decreased BDNF (Schmitt et al. Citation2016). Finally, a causality relationship between abnormal BDNF values and insomnia is supported by the finding that a genetic association has been observed between insomnia and the BDNF gene (Zaki et al. Citation2019, see Supplementary Table S3).

Conclusion

In conclusion, as shown in the overview presented in , besides the psychometric instruments (scales and questionnaires for insomnia or beliefs about sleep) which are the gold standard for the diagnosis of insomnia based on established cut-off scores, the biological measurements which emerge with high diagnostic performance for insomnia and can be considered as its diagnostic biomarkers are the polysomnography-derived cyclic alternating pattern (CAP), actigraphy, and low serum BDNF levels; they are followed by increased heart rate (immediately before or right after sleep onset), neuroimaging findings (showing high activation of certain brain circuits during sleep and/or wakefulness) and a deficient circadian melatonin rhythm. It must be noted, however, that for some of the above potential biomarkers for diagnosing insomnia (e.g. for MRI or for measurements requiring serial blood sampling) their applicability as widespread methods for diagnosing insomnia is curtailed, both in terms of their cost-effectiveness as well as in terms of the complexity of the procedures involved. Another issue that needs to be taken into account for the majority of emerging biomarkers is that, after having evaluated their discrimination power, they should be further examined regarding the domain they assess (e.g. HPA axis) as well as regarding possible diagnostic entities that these markers may add information to. Thus, more work is needed in order: (a) to replicate findings that are based on one or a very small number of studies, in view of the need of having a sufficient number of studies to assess diagnostic discrimination, (b) to create commonly accepted methodologies and algorithms for their outcome measures across studies, and (c) to establish appropriate diagnostic cut-off points for insomnia and other diagnostic entities. In any event, for future research, it is most important to report diagnostic accuracy measures for all identified group differences. Further to their use as diagnostic indices, biomarkers are relevant in understanding the nature and mechanisms of insomnia, in distinguishing among subtypes of this disorder, and in potentially guiding targeted treatment. For example, macro-architecture of sleep, as assessed through polysomnography and measurements of HPA axis activity, although they are not proven as yet to be diagnostically useful, may have the potential to distinguish subtypes of insomnia with different clinical characteristics, prognosis, and treatment needs. Another major observation regarding the value of most biomarkers (diagnostically useful or not) in understanding the nature of insomnia is the proof they provide in favour of the hyperarousal concept of insomnia both during sleep and wakefulness.

Table 4. Overview of the diagnostic potential for insomnia of psychometric tools and biological measurements.

WFSBP Task Force on Sleep Disorders

Constantin R. Soldatos (Chair), Dimitris Dikeos (Co-Chair), Adam Wichniak (Secretary), Francesco Benedetti, Tatiana Crönlein, Anne Eckert, Martin Hatzinger, Jana Kopřivová, Periklis Ktonas, Birendra N. Malik, Thorsten Mikoteit, Maria Ntafouli, Tatsuro Ohta, Fuat Őzgen, Dieter Riemann, Kai Spiegelhalder, Giedrius Varoneckas.

Supplemental material

Supplementary Table S1

Download MS Word (270 KB)

Supplementary Table S3

Download MS Word (60.5 KB)

Supplementary Table S2

Download MS Word (43 KB)

Acknowledgements

The authors DD and CS appreciate the most valuable contribution to the establishment of the Diagnostic Grading System utilized in this study of the following members of the Department of Hygiene, Epidemiology, and Medical Statistics, Medical School of National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece: Professor Giota Touloumi, Assistant Professor Nikos Pantazis, and Valia Baralou, MSc.

Statement of interest

None to declare.

References

  • Altena E, Van Der Werf YD, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Voorn TA, Rombouts SARB, Kuijer JPA, Van Someren EJW. 2008. Prefrontal hypoactivation and recovery in insomnia. Sleep. 31(9):1271–1276.
  • [AASM] American Academy of Sleep Medicine. 2014. International classification of sleep disorders. 3rd ed. Darien (IL): American Academy of Sleep Medicine.
  • [APA] American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association.
  • Azevedo MH, Bos SC, Soares MJ, Marques M, Pereira AT, Maia B, Gomes AA, Macedo A. 2010. Longitudinal study on perfectionism and sleep disturbance. World J Biol Psychiatry. 11(2 Pt 2):476–485.
  • Backhaus J, Junghanns K, Hohagen F. 2004. Sleep disturbances are correlated with decreased morning awakening salivary cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 29(9):1184–1191.
  • Baglioni C, Regen W, Teghen A, Spiegelhalder K, Feige B, Nissen C, Riemann D. 2014. Sleep changes in the disorder of insomnia: a meta-analysis of polysomnographic studies. Sleep Med Rev. 18(3):195–213.
  • Baglioni C, Spiegelhalder K, Regen W, Feige B, Nissen C, Lombardo C, Violani C, Hennig J, Riemann D. 2014. Insomnia disorder is associated with increased amygdala reactivity to insomnia-related stimuli. Sleep. 37(12):1907–1917.
  • Basta M, Chrousos GP, Vela-Bueno A, Vgontzas AN. 2007. Chronic insomnia and the stress system. Sleep Med Clin. 2(2):279–291.
  • Bastien CH, Vallières A, Morin CM. 2001. Validation of the insomnia severity index as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2(4):297–307.
  • Bathgate CJ, Edinger JD, Krystal AD. 2017. Insomnia patients with objective short sleep duration have a blunted response to cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Sleep. 40:Zsw012.
  • Bertisch SM, Pollock BD, Mittleman MA, Buysse DJ, Bazzano LA, Gottlieb DJ, Redline S. 2018. Insomnia with objective short sleep duration and risk of incident cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality: sleep heart health study. Sleep. 41:Zsy047.
  • Billman GE. 2013. The LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front Physiol. 4:26.
  • Blanken TF, Benjamins JS, Borsboom D, Vermunt JK, Paquola C, Ramautar J, Dekker K, Stoffers D, Wassing R, Wei Y, et al. 2019. Insomnia disorder subtypes derived from life history and traits of affect and personality. Lancet Psychiatry. 6(2):151–163.
  • Bluestein D, Rutledge CM, Healey AC. 2010. Psychosocial correlates of insomnia severity in primary care. J Am Board Fam Med. 23(2):204–211.
  • Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 1996. The consequences of a week of insomnia. Sleep. 19(6):453–461.
  • Bonnet MH, Arand DL. 1998. Heart rate variability in insomniacs and matched normal sleepers. Psychosom Med. 60(5):610–615.
  • Broman JE, Hetta J. 1994. Electrodermal activity in patients with persistent insomnia. J Sleep Res. 3(3):165–170.
  • Broomfield NM, Espie CA. 2005. Towards a valid, reliable measure of sleep effort. J Sleep Res. 14(4):401–407.
  • Brzezinski A, Vangel MG, Wurtman RJ, Norrie G, Zhdanova I, Ben-Shushan A, Ford I. 2005. Effects of exogenous melatonin on sleep: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 9(1):41–50.
  • Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ, Munafò MR. 2013. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 14(5):365–376.
  • Buysse DJ, Germain A, Hall ML, Moul DE, Nofzinger EA, Begley A, Ehlers CL, Thompson W, Kupfer DJ. 2008. EEG spectral analysis in primary insomnia: NREM period effects and sex differences. Sleep. 31(12):1673–1682.
  • Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. 1989. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28(2):193–213.
  • Califf RM. 2018. Biomarker definitions and their applications. Exp Biol Med. 243(3):213–221.
  • Carney CE, Edinger JD. 2006. Identifying critical beliefs about sleep in primary insomnia. Sleep. 29(4):444–453.
  • Carney CE, Edinger JD, Morin CM, Manber R, Rybarczyk B, Stepanski EJ, Wright H, Lack L. 2010. Examining maladaptive beliefs about sleep across insomnia patient groups. J Psychosom Res. 68(1):57–65.
  • Carskadon MA, Dement WC, Mitler MM, Guilleminault C, Zarcone VP, Spiegel R. 1976. Self reports versus sleep laboratory findings in 122 drug free subjects with complaints of chronic insomnia. Am J Psychiatry. 133(12):1382–1388.
  • Cervena K, Dauvilliers Y, Espa F, Touchon J, Matousek M, Billiard M, Besset A. 2004. Effect of cognitive behavioural therapy for insomnia on sleep architecture and sleep EEG power spectra in psychophysiological insomnia. J Sleep Res. 13(4):385–393.
  • Chapman JL, Comas M, Hoyos CM, Bartlett DJ, Grunstein RR, Gordon CJ. 2018. Is metabolic rate increased in insomnia disorder? A systematic review. Front Endocrinol. 9:374.
  • Chesson A, Hartse K, Anderson WM, Davila D, Johnson S, Littner M, Wise M, Rafecas J. 2000. Practice parameters for the evaluation of chronic insomnia. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine report. Standards of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Sleep. 23(2):237–241.
  • Chouvarda I, Mendez MO, Rosso V, Bianchi AM, Parrino L, Grassi A, Terzano MG, Cerutti S, Maglaveras N. 2012. Cyclic alternating patterns in normal sleep and insomnia: structure and content differences. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng. 20(5):642–652.
  • Collins PY, Patel V, Joestl SS, March D, Insel TR, Daar AS, Anderson W, Dhansay MA, Phillips A, Shurin S, et al. 2011. Grand challenges in global mental health. Nature. 475(7354):27–30.
  • Colombo MA, Ramautar JR, Wei Y, Gomez-Herrero G, Stoffers D, Wassing R, Benjamins JS, Tagliazucchi E, van der Werf YD, Cajochen C, et al. 2016. Wake high-density electroencephalographic spatiospectral signatures of insomnia. Sleep. 39(5):1015–1027.
  • Costa PT, McCrae R. 1992. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa (FL): Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Crönlein T, Wagner S, Langguth B, Geisler P, Eichhammer P, Wetter TC. 2014. Are dysfunctional attitudes and beliefs about sleep unique to primary insomnia? Sleep Med. 15(12):1463–1467.
  • Cvengros JA, Crawford MR, Manber R, Ong JC. 2015. The relationship between beliefs about sleep and adherence to behavioral treatment combined with meditation for insomnia. Behav Sleep Med. 13(1):52–63.
  • D'Aurea C, Poyares D, Piovezan RD, Passos G, Tufik S, Mello MT. 2015. Objective short sleep duration is associated with the activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in insomnia. Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 73(6):516–519.
  • Deuschle M, Schredl M, Wisch C, Schilling C, Gilles M, Geisel O, Hellweg R. 2018. Serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in sleep-disordered patients: relation to sleep stage N3 and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep across diagnostic entities. J Sleep Res. 27(1):73–77.
  • De Zambotti M, Covassin N, Sarlo M, De Min Tona G, Trinder J, Stegagno L. 2013. Nighttime cardiac sympathetic hyper-activation in young primary insomniacs. Clin Auton Res. 23(1):49–56.
  • Dikeos DG, Soldatos CR. 2005. The condition of insomnia: etiopathogenetic considerations and their impact on treatment practices. Int Rev Psychiatry. 17(4):255–262.
  • Dodds KL, Miller CB, Kyle SD, Marshall NS, Gordon CJ. 2017. Heart rate variability in insomnia patients: a critical review of the literature. Sleep Med Rev. 33:88–100.
  • Doos Ali Vand H, Gharraee B, Farid A-AA, Bandi MG. 2014. Prediction of insomnia severity based on cognitive, metacognitive, and emotional variables in college students. Explore. 10(4):233–240.
  • Drake CL, Hays RD, Morlock R, Wang F, Shikiar R, Frank L, Downey R, Roth T. 2014. Development and evaluation of a measure to assess restorative sleep. J Clin Sleep Med. 10(7):733–741.
  • Drummond SPA, Walker M, Almklov E, Campos M, Anderson DE, Straus LD. 2013. Neural correlates of working memory performance in primary insomnia. Sleep. 36(9):1307–1316.
  • Duggan KA, Friedman HS, McDevitt EA, Mednick SC. 2014. Personality and healthy sleep: the importance of conscientiousness and neuroticism. PLOS One. 9:e90628.
  • Duman RS, Monteggia LM. 2006. A Neurotrophic model for Stress-Related mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 59(12):1116–1127.
  • Edinger JD, Wohlgemuth WK. 2001. Psychometric comparisons of the standard and abbreviated DBAS-10 versions of the dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep questionnaire. Sleep Med. 2(6):493–500.
  • Ellis BW, Johns MW, Lancaster R, Raptopoulos P, Angelopoulos N, Priest RG. 1981. The St. Mary’s Hospital Sleep Questionnaire: a study of reliability. Sleep. 4(1):93–97.
  • Espie CA, Inglis SJ, Harvey L, Tessier S. 2000. Insomniacs’ attributions. Psychometric properties of the dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep scale and the sleep disturbance questionnaire. J Psychosom Res. 48(2):141–148.
  • Espie CA, Kyle SD, Hames P, Gardani M, Fleming L, Cape J. 2014. The Sleep condition indicator: a clinical screening tool to evaluate insomnia disorder. BMJ Open. 4:0041843.
  • Fan TT, Chen WH, Shi L, Lin X, Tabarak S, Chen SJ. 2019. Objective sleep duration is associated with cognitive deficits in primary insomnia: BDNF may play a role. Sleep. 42:zsy192.
  • Fang SC, Huang CJ, Yang TT, Tsai PS. 2008. Heart rate variability and daytime functioning in insomniacs and normal sleepers: preliminary results. J Psychosom Res. 65(1):23–30.
  • Farina B, Dittoni S, Colicchio S, Testani E, Losurdo A, Gnoni V, Di Blasi C, Brunetti R, Contardi A, Mazza S, et al. 2014. Heart rate and heart rate variability modification in chronic insomnia patients. Behav Sleep Med. 12(4):290–306.
  • Ferguson CJ. 2009. An efffect size primer: a guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof Psychol Res Pract. 40:532–538.
  • Fernandez-Mendoza J, Baker JH, Vgontzas AN, Gaines J, Liao D, Bixler EO. 2017. Insomnia symptoms with objective short sleep duration are associated with systemic inflammation in adolescents. Brain Behav Immun. 61:110–116.
  • Fernandez-Mendoza J, Vgontzas AN, Calhoun SL, Vgontzas A, Tsaoussoglou M, Gaines J, Liao D, Chrousos GP, Bixler EO. 2014. Insomnia symptoms, objective sleep duration and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity in children. Eur J Clin Invest. 44(5):493–500.
  • Fortier-Brochu E, Morin CM. 2014. Cognitive impairment in individuals with insomnia: clinical significance and correlates. Sleep. 37(11):1787–1798.
  • Giese M, Unternährer E, Hüttig H, Beck J, Brand S, Calabrese P, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Eckert A. 2014. BDNF: an indicator of insomnia. Mol Psychiatry. 19(2):151–152.
  • Gradisar M, Lack L, Wright H, Harris J, Brooks A. 2006. Do chronic primary insomniacs have impaired heat loss when attempting sleep? Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. 290:R1115–1121.
  • Gray EK, Watson D. 2002. General and specific traits of personality and their relation to sleep and academic performance. J Pers. 70(2):177–206.
  • Hajak G, Bianchi M, Boichut D, Crowe C, Hajak G, Hetta J, SINE Study Group. Study of Insomnia in Europe. 2001. Epidemiology of severe insomnia and its consequences in Germany. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 251(2):49–56.
  • Hajak G, Rodenbeck A, Staedt J, Bandelow B, Huether G, Rüther E. 1995. Nocturnal plasma melatonin levels in patients suffering from chronic primary insomnia. J Pineal Res. 19(3):116–122.
  • Harvey AG. 2002. A cognitive model of insomnia. Behav Res Ther. 40(8):869–893.
  • Harvey AG, Tang NKY. 2012. (Mis)perception of sleep in insomnia: a puzzle and a resolution. Psychol Bull. 138(1):77–101.
  • Harvey CJ, Gehrman P, Espie CA. 2014. Who is predisposed to insomnia: a review of familial aggregation, stress-reactivity, personality and coping style. Sleep Med Rev. 18(3):237–247.
  • Harvey KJ, Espie CA. 2004. Development and preliminary validation of the Glasgow Content of Thoughts Inventory (GCTI): a new measure for the assessment of pre-sleep cognitive activity. Br J Clin Psychol. 43(Pt 4):409–420.
  • Hertenstein E, Nissen C, Riemann D, Feige B, Baglioni C, Spiegelhalder K. 2015. The exploratory power of sleep effort, dysfunctional beliefs and arousal for insomnia severity and polysomnography-determined sleep. J Sleep Res. 24(4):399–406.
  • Hintsanen M, Puttonen S, Smith K, Törnroos M, Jokela M, Pulkki-Råback L, Hintsa T, Merjonen P, Dwyer T, Raitakari OT, et al. 2014. Five-factor personality traits and sleep: evidence from two population-based cohort studies. Health Psychol. 33(10):1214–1223.
  • Hosmer DW, Lemeshow S. 2000. Applied logistic regression. 2nd ed. Chapter 5. New York (NY): John Wiley and Sons; p. 160–164.
  • Irwin MR, Olmstead R, Carroll JE. 2016. Sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and inflammation: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies and experimental sleep deprivation. Biol Psychiatry. 80(1):40–52.
  • Jansson-Fröjmark M, Harvey AG, Lundh LG, Norell-Clarke A, Linton SJ. 2011. Psychometric properties of an insomnia-specific measure of worry: the anxiety and preoccupation about sleep questionnaire. Cogn Behav Ther. 40(1):65–76.
  • Jenkins CD, Stanton BA, Niemcryk SJ, Rose RM. 1988. A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. J Clin Epidemiol. 41(4):313–321.
  • Johann AF, Hertenstein E, Kyle SD, Baglioni C, Feige B, Nissen C, Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K. 2017. Perfectionism and polysomnography-determined markers of poor sleep. J Clin Sleep Med. 13(11):1319–1326.
  • Jurysta F, Lanquart J-P, Sputaels V, Dumont M, Migeotte P-F, Leistedt S, Linkowski P, van de Borne P. 2009. The impact of chronic primary insomnia on the heart rate – EEG variability link. Clin Neurophysiol. 120(6):1054–1060.
  • Kales A, Caldwell AB, Soldatos CR, Bixler EO, Kales JD. 1983. Biopsychobehavioral correlates of insomnia. II. Pattern specificity and consistency with the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory. Psychosom Med. 45(4):341–356.
  • Kay DB, Karim HT, Soehner AM, Hasler BP, Wilckens KA, James JA, Aizenstein HJ, Price JC, Rosario BL, Kupfer DJ, et al. 2016. Sleep-Wake differences in relative regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose among patients with insomnia compared with good sleepers. Sleep. 39(10):1779–1794.
  • Kay DB, Karim HT, Soehner AM, Hasler BP, James JA, Germain A, Hall MH, Franzen PL, Price JC, Nofzinger EA, et al. 2017. Subjective-objective sleep discrepancy is associated with alterations in regional glucose metabolism in patients with insomnia and good sleeper controls. Sleep. 40:zsx155.
  • Kessler RC, Coulouvrat C, Hajak G, Lakoma MD, Roth T, Sampson N, Shahly V, Shillington A, Stephenson JJ, Walsh JK, et al. 2010. Reliability and validity of the brief insomnia questionnaire in the America insomnia survey. Sleep. 33(11):1539–1549.
  • Križan Z, Hisler G. 2019. Personality and sleep: neuroticism and conscientiousness predict behaviourally recorded sleep years later. Eur J Pers. 33:133–153.
  • Krystal AD, Edinger JD. 2010. Sleep EEG predictors and correlates of the response to cognitive behavioral therapy for insomnia. Sleep. 33(5):669–677.
  • Lack LC, Gradisar M, Van Someren EJW, Wright HR, Lushington K. 2008. The relationship between insomnia and body temperatures. Sleep Med Rev. 12(4):307–317.
  • Lattova Z, Keckeis M, Maurovich-Horvat E, Wetter TC, Wilde-Frenz J, Schuld A, Pollmächer T. 2011. The stress hormone system in various sleep disorders. J Psychiatr Res. 45(9):1223–1228.
  • Laugsand LE, Vatten LJ, Bjørngaard JH, Hveem K, Janszky I. 2012. Insomnia and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein: the HUNT study, Norway. Psychosom Med. 74(5):543–553.
  • Leerssen J, Wassing R, Ramautar JR, Stoffers D, Lakbila-Kamal O, Perrier J, Bruijel J, Foster-Dingley JC, Aghajani M, van Someren EJW, et al. 2019. Increased hippocampal-prefrontal functional connectivity in insomnia. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 160:144–150.
  • Lemoine P, Nir T, Laudon M, Zisapel N. 2007. Prolonged-release melatonin improves sleep quality and morning alertness in insomnia patients aged 55 years and older and has no withdrawal effects. J Sleep Res. 16(4):372–380.
  • Lo Martire V, Caruso D, Palagini L, Zoccoli G, Bastianini S. 2020. Stress & sleep: a relationship lasting a lifetime. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 117:65–77.
  • Lundh LG, Broman JE, Hetta J, Saboonchi F. 1994. Perfectionism and insomnia. Scand J Behav Ther. 23:3–18.
  • Lushington K, Dawson D, Lack L. 2000. Core body temperature is elevated during constant wakefulness in elderly poor sleepers. Sleep. 23(4):504–510.
  • Luthringer R, Muzet M, Zisapel N, Staner L. 2009. The effect of prolonged-release melatonin on sleep measures and psychomotor performance in elderly patients with insomnia. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 24(5):239–249.
  • Malik M, Bigger JT, Camm AJ, Kleiger RE, Malliani A, Moss AJ. 1996. Heart rate variability. Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Eur Heart J. 17:354–381.
  • Mikoteit T, Brand S, Eckert A, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Beck J. 2019. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor is a biomarker for subjective insomnia but not objectively assessable poor sleep continuity. J Psychiatr Res. 110:103–109.
  • Mikoteit T, Pawlowski M, Seiffert N, Seelig E, Bilz S, Hatzinger M, et al. 2017. Heart rate variability during sleep distinguishes between insomnia and normal sleep. Sleep Med. 40:e223–e224.
  • Mikoteit T, De Witte M, Beck J, Hatzinger M, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Pawlowski MA. 2019. Sleep stage related heart rate variability distinguishes between insomnia and normal sleep. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 29:578–579.
  • Miller CB, Bartlett DJ, Mullins AE, Dodds KL, Gordon CJ, Kyle SD, Kim JW, D'Rozario AL, Lee RSC, Comas M, et al. 2016. Clusters of insomnia disorder: an exploratory cluster analysis of objective sleep parameters reveals differences in neurocognitive functioning, quantitative EEG, and heart rate variability. Sleep. 39(11):1993–2004.
  • Montserrat Sánchez-Ortuño M, Edinger JD. 2010. A penny for your thoughts: patterns of sleep-related beliefs, insomnia symptoms and treatment outcome. Behav Res Ther. 48(2):125–133.
  • Morin CM, Drake CL, Harvey AG, Krystal AD, Manber R, Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K. 2015. Insomnia disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 1:15026.
  • Morin CM, Stone J, Trinkle D, Mercer J, Remsberg S. 1993. Dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep among older adults with and without insomnia complaints. Psychol Aging. 8(3):463–467.
  • Morin CM, Vallières A, Ivers H. 2007. Dysfunctional beliefs and attitudes about sleep (DBAS): validation of a brief version (DBAS-16). Sleep. 30(11):1547–1554.
  • Morris M, Lack L, Dawson D. 1990. Sleep-onset insomniacs have delayed temperature rhythms. Sleep. 13(1):1–14.
  • Natale V, Plazzi G, Martoni M. 2009. Actigraphy in the assessment of insomnia: a quantitative approach. Sleep. 32(6):767–771.
  • Natale V, Léger D, Martoni M, Bayon V, Erbacci A. 2014. The role of actigraphy in the assessment of primary insomnia: a retrospective study. Sleep Med. 15(1):111–115.
  • Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Germain A, Price JC, Miewald JM, Kupfer DJ. 2004. Functional neuroimaging evidence for hyperarousal in insomnia. Am J Psychiatry. 161(11):2126–2128.
  • Norell-Clarke A, Jansson-Fröjmark M, Tillfors M, Harvey AG, Linton SJ. 2014. Cognitive processes and their association with persistence and remission of insomnia: findings from a longitudinal study in the general population. Behav Res Ther. 54:38–48.
  • Okun ML, Coussons-Read M, Hall M. 2009. Disturbed sleep is associated with increased C-reactive protein in young women. Brain Behav Immun. 23(3):351–354.
  • O'Neal WT, Chen LY, Nazarian S, Soliman EZ. 2016. Reference ranges for short-term heart rate variability measures in individuals free of cardiovascular disease: The Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). J Electrocardiol. 49(5):686–690.
  • Palagini L, Cellini N, Mauri M, Mazzei I, Simpraga S, dell’Osso L, Linkenkaer-Hansen K, Riemann D. 2016. Multiple phenotypes of resting-state cognition are altered in insomnia disorder. Sleep Health. 2(3):239–245.
  • Pallesen S, Bjorvatn B, Nordhus IH, Sivertsen B, Hjørnevik M, Morin CM. 2008. A new scale for measuring insomnia: the Bergen Insomnia Scale. Percept Mot Skills. 107(3):691–706.
  • Parrott AC, Hindmarch I. 1980. The Leeds sleep evaluation questionnaire in psychopharmacological investigations-a review. Psychopharmacology. 71(2):173–179.
  • Parthasarathy S, Vasquez MM, Halonen M, Bootzin R, Quan SF, Martinez FD, Guerra S. 2015. Persistent insomnia is associated with mortality risk. Am J Med. 128(3):268–275.e2.
  • Perlis ML, Giles DE, Mendelson WB, Bootzin RR, Wyatt JK. 1997. Psychophysiological insomnia: the behavioural model and a neurocognitive perspective. J Sleep Res. 6(3):179–188.
  • Pevernagie D. 2021. Future treatment of sleep disorders: syndromic approach versus management of treatable traits? Sleep Med Clin. 16(3):465–473.
  • Raymann RJEM, Swaab DF, Van Someren EJW. 2007. Skin temperature and sleep-onset latency: changes with age and insomnia. Physiol Behav. 90(2–3):257–266.
  • Regen W, Kyle SD, Nissen C, Feige B, Baglioni C, Hennig J, Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K. 2016. Objective sleep disturbances are associated with greater waking resting-state connectivity between the retrosplenial cortex/hippocampus and various nodes of the default mode network. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 41(5):295–303.
  • Riedner BA, Goldstein MR, Plante DT, Rumble ME, Ferrarelli F, Tononi G, Benca RM. 2016. Regional patterns of elevated alpha and High-Frequency electroencephalographic activity during nonrapid eye movement sleep in chronic insomnia: a pilot study. Sleep. 39(4):801–812.
  • Riemann D, Baglioni C, Bassetti C, Bjorvatn B, Dolenc Groselj L, Ellis JG, Espie CA, Garcia-Borreguero D, Gjerstad M, Gonçalves M, et al. 2017. European guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of insomnia. J Sleep Res. 26(6):675–700.
  • Riemann D, Klein T, Rodenbeck A, Feige B, Horny A, Hummel R, Weske G, Al-Shajlawi A, Voderholzer U. 2002. Nocturnal cortisol and melatonin secretion in primary insomnia. Psychiatry Res. 113(1–2):17–27.
  • Riemann D, Nissen C, Palagini L, Otte A, Perlis ML, Spiegelhalder K. 2015. The neurobiology, investigation, and treatment of chronic insomnia. Lancet Neurol. 14(5):547–558.
  • Riemann D, Spiegelhalder K, Feige B, Voderholzer U, Berger M, Perlis M, Nissen C. 2010. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: a review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med Rev. 14(1):19–31.
  • Roth T, Zammit G, Kushida C, Doghramji K, Mathias SD, Wong JM. 2002. A new questionnaire to detect sleep disorders. Sleep Med. 3:99–108.
  • Sateia MJ, Doghramji K, Hauri PJ, Morin CM. 2000. Evaluation of chronic insomnia. An American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep. 23(2):243–308.
  • Sawilowsky S. 2009. New effect size rules of thumb. J Mod Appl Stat Methods. 8:467–474.
  • Schmitt K, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Eckert A. 2016. BDNF in sleep, insomnia, and sleep deprivation. Ann Med. 48(1–2):42–51.
  • Schwabedal JTC, Riedl M, Penzel T, Wessel N. 2016. Alpha-wave frequency characteristics in health and insomnia during sleep. J Sleep Res. 25(3):278–286.
  • Seelig E, Keller U, Klarhöfer M, Scheffler K, Brand S, Holsboer-Trachsler E, Hatzinger M, Bilz S. 2013. Neuroendocrine regulation and metabolism of glucose and lipids in primary chronic insomnia: a prospective case-control study. PLoS One. 8:e61780.
  • Seo J, Moore KN, Gazecki S, Bottary RM, Milad MR, Song H, Pace-Schott EF. 2018. Delayed fear extinction in individuals with insomnia disorder. Sleep. 41:zsy095.
  • Seow LSE, Abdin E, Chang S, Chong SA, Subramaniam M. 2018. Identifying the best sleep measure to screen clinical insomnia in a psychiatric population. Sleep Med. 41:86–93.
  • Slavish DC, Graham-Engeland JE, Engeland CG, Taylor DJ, Buxton OM. 2018. Insomnia symptoms are associated with elevated C-reactive protein in young adults. Psychol Health. 33(11):1396–1415.
  • Smith MT, McCrae CS, Cheung J, Martin JL, Harrod CG, Heald JL, Carden KA. 2018. Use of actigraphy for the evaluation of sleep disorders and circadian rhythm sleep-wake disorders: an American Academy of Sleep Medicine systematic review, meta-analysis, and GRADE assessment. J Clin Sleep Med. 14(7):1209–1230.
  • Smith MT, Perlis ML, Chengazi VU, Pennington J, Soeffing J, Ryan JM, Giles DE. 2002. Neuroimaging of NREM sleep in primary insomnia: a Tc-99-HMPAO single photon emission computed tomography study. Sleep. 25(3):325–335.
  • Smith S, Trinder J. 2001. Detecting insomnia: comparison of four self-report measures of sleep in a young adult population. J Sleep Res. 10(3):229–235.
  • Soldatos CR, Dikeos DG, Paparrigopoulos TJ. 2000. Athens Insomnia scale: validation of an instrument based on ICD-10 criteria. J Psychosom Res. 48(6):555–560.
  • Soldatos CR, Dikeos DG, Paparrigopoulos TJ. 2003. The diagnostic validity of the Athens Insomnia Scale. J Psychosom Res. 55(3):263–267.
  • Spiegelhalder K, Fuchs L, Ladwig J, Kyle SD, Nissen C, Voderholzer U, Feige B, Riemann D. 2011. Heart rate and heart rate variability in subjectively reported insomnia. J Sleep Res. 20(1 Pt 2):137–145.
  • Spiegelhalder K, Regen W, Feige B, Holz J, Piosczyk H, Baglioni C, Riemann D, Nissen C. 2012. Increased EEG sigma and beta power during NREM sleep in primary insomnia. Biol Psychol. 91(3):329–333.
  • Spoormaker VI, Verbeek I, van den Bout J, Klip EC. 2005. Initial validation of the SLEEP-50 questionnaire. Behav Sleep Med. 3(4):227–246.
  • Steiger A. 2007. Neurochemical regulation of sleep. J Psychiatr Res. 41(7):537–552.
  • Stein PK, Pu Y. 2012. Heart rate variability, sleep and sleep disorders. Sleep Med Rev. 16(1):47–66.
  • Stephan Y, Sutin AR, Bayard S, Križan Z, Terracciano A. 2018. Personality and sleep quality: evidence from four prospective studies. Health Psychol. 37(3):271–281.
  • Stoffers D, Altena E, van der Werf YD, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Voorn TA, Astill RG, Strijers RLM, Waterman D, Van Someren EJW. 2014. The caudate: a key node in the neuronal network imbalance of insomnia? Brain. 137(Pt 2):610–620.
  • Svetnik V, Snyder ES, Ma J, Tao P, Lines C, Herring WJ. 2017. EEG spectral analysis of NREM sleep in a large sample of patients with insomnia and good sleepers: effects of age, sex and part of the night. J Sleep Res. 26(1):92–104.
  • Tahmasian M, Noori K, Samea F, Zarei M, Spiegelhalder K, Eickhoff SB, Van Someren E, Khazaie H, Eickhoff CR. 2018. A lack of consistent brain alterations in insomnia disorder: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 42:111–118.
  • Te Lindert BHW, Van Someren EJW. 2018. Skin temperature, sleep, and vigilance. In: Handbook of clinical neurology. Vol. 156. Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V.; p. 353–365.
  • Terzano MG, Parrino L, Smerieri A. 2001. Neurophysiological basis of insomnia: role of cyclic alternating patterns. Rev Neurol. 157(11 Pt 2):S62–S66.
  • Tobaldini E, Nobili L, Strada S, Casali KR, Braghiroli A, Montano N. 2013. Heart rate variability in normal and pathological sleep. Front Physiol. 4:294–211.
  • Tononi G, Cirelli C. 2014. Sleep and the price of plasticity: from synaptic and cellular homeostasis to memory consolidation and integration. Neuron. 81(1):12–34.
  • Tremblay V, Savard J, Ivers H. 2009. Predictors of the effect of cognitive behavioral therapy for chronic insomnia comorbid with breast cancer. J Consult Clin Psychol. 77(4):742–750.
  • Van de Laar M, Leufkens T, Bakker B, Pevernagie D, Overeem S. 2017. Phenotypes of sleeplessness: stressing the need for psychodiagnostics in the assessment of insomnia. Psychol Health Med. 22(8):902–910.
  • Van de Laar M, Verbeek I, Pevernagie D, Aldenkamp A, Overeem S. 2010. The role of personality traits in insomnia. Sleep Med Rev. 14(1):61–68.
  • Van Der Heijden KB, Smits MG, Van Someren EJW, Boudewijn Gunning W. 2005. Prediction of melatonin efficacy by pretreatment dim light melatonin onset in children with idiopathic chronic sleep onset insomnia. J Sleep Res. 14(2):187–194.
  • Van Der Heijden KB, Smits MG, Van Someren EJW, Ridderinkhof KR, Gunning WB. 2007. Effect of melatonin on sleep, behavior, and cognition in ADHD and chronic sleep-onset insomnia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 46(2):233–241.
  • Van Maanen A, Meijer AM, Smits MG, Van Der Heijden KB, Oort FJ. 2017. Effects of melatonin and bright light treatment in childhood chronic sleep onset insomnia with late melatonin onset: a randomized controlled study. Sleep. 40 (2): zsw038.
  • van Neijenhof RJGP, van Duijn E, Van Den Berg JF, de Waal MWM, van der Mast RC, Comijs HC. 2018. Subjective insomnia symptoms and sleep duration are not related to hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activity in older adults. J Sleep Res. 27(1):40–46.
  • Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Lin HM, Prolo P, Mastorakos G, Vela-Bueno A, Kales A, Chrousos GP. 2001. Chronic insomnia is associated with nyctohemeral activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: clinical implications. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 86(8):3787–3794.
  • Vgontzas AN, Bixler EO, Kales A, Manfredi RL, Tyson K. 1994. Validity and clinical utility of sleep laboratory criteria for insomnia. Int J Neurosci. 77(1–2):11–21.
  • Vgontzas AN, Fernandez-Mendoza J, Liao D, Bixler EO. 2013. Insomnia with objective short sleep duration: the most biologically severe phenotype of the disorder. Sleep Med Rev. 17(4):241–254.
  • Vincent NK, Walker JR. 2000. Perfectionism and chronic insomnia. J Psychosom Res. 49(5):349–354.
  • Wichniak A, Jankowski KS, Skalski M, Skwarło-Sońta K, Zawilska JB, Żarowski M, Poradowska E, Jernajczyk W. 2017. Treatment guidelines for circadian rhythm Sleep-Wake disorders of the polish sleep research society and the section of biological psychiatry of the polish psychiatric association. Psychiatr Pol. 51(5):793–814.
  • Williams PG, Moroz TL. 2009. Personality vulnerability to stress-related sleep disruption: pathways to adverse mental and physical health outcomes. Pers Individ Diff. 46:598–603.
  • [WHO] World Health Organization. 2019. ICD-11: international classification of diseases (11th revision). Available from: https://icd.who.int/
  • Xie Z, Chen F, Li WA, Geng X, Li C, Meng X, Feng Y, Liu W, Yu F. 2017. A review of sleep disorders and melatonin. Neurol Res. 39(6):559–565.
  • Yang AC, Tsai SJ, Yang CH, Kuo CH, Chen TJ, Hong CJ. 2011. Reduced physiologic complexity is associated with poor sleep in patients with major depression and primary insomnia. J Affect Disord. 131(1–3):179–185.
  • Yang CM, Hung CY, Lee HC. 2014. Stress-related sleep vulnerability and maladaptive sleep beliefs predict insomnia at long-term follow-up. J Clin Sleep Med. 10(9):997–1001.
  • Zaki NFW, Saleh E, Elwasify M, Mahmoud E, Zaki J, Spence DW, BaHammam AS, Pandi-Perumal SR. 2019. The association of BDNF gene polymorphism with cognitive impairment in insomnia patients. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 88:253–264.
  • Zhang J, Lam S-P, Li SX, Ma RCW, Kong APS, Chan MHM, Ho C-S, Li AM, Wing Y-K. 2014. A community-based study on the association between insomnia and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: sex and pubertal influences. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 99(6):2277–2287.
  • Zhao W, Van Someren EJW, Li C, Chen X, Gui W, Tian Y, Liu Y, Lei X. 2021. EEG spectral analysis in insomnia disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. 59:101457.