7,622
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

Identifying viable theoretical frameworks with essential parameters for real-time and real world alcohol craving research: a systematic review of craving models

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon show all
Pages 35-51 | Received 15 Nov 2016, Accepted 18 Mar 2017, Published online: 13 Apr 2017

Abstract

Background: Substance use is known to be episodic, dynamic, complex, and highly influenced by the environment, therefore a situational and momentary focus to alcohol craving research is appropriate. Current advances in mobile and wearable technology provide novel opportunities for craving research. However, the lack of consensus within craving theory impedes the identification and prioritization of parameters to be monitored. The aim of this study is to critically review current craving models in order to determine viable theoretical frameworks of alcohol craving and its essential parameters.

Methods: Eighteen models of craving were reviewed by applying a literature search with a five-step strategy that accounted for the momentary nature of craving and included a snowballing search and a key term extraction algorithm. Based on this review, multiple decision criteria were defined upon which to evaluate the models.

Results: Six models for alcohol craving were supported by sufficient empirical research to be eligible. The inferences drawn on these six models resulted in three decision criteria: the model should (1) incorporate negative affect as a predictor of relapse; (2) explain that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias towards alcohol cues than nondependent drinkers; (3) incorporate increased risk of relapse with heightened stress levels.

Conclusions: The affective processing model of negative reinforcement, the cognitive processing model, the incentive sensitization theory of addiction and the theory of neural opponent motivation are classified as viable theoretical frameworks, resulting in negative affect and stress as relevant parameters to include in real-time craving monitoring research.

Introduction

Craving, defined as the subjective, unwanted desire or urge to use a substance while attempting to abstain (Serre et al. Citation2015), is generally considered a fundamental precursor to relapse in alcohol addiction (Rohsenow & Monti Citation1999). Many patients and clinicians in addiction care regard craving as a highly challenging obstacle for achieving recovery (Lowman et al. Citation2000). Although cognitive-behavioral treatment offers patients useful relapse prevention techniques, patients frequently fail to apply these consistently or timely since they often simply do not recognize their high-risk (re)lapse situation (Larimer et al. Citation1999). Rohsenow and Monti (Citation1999) state that making patients aware of their craving in high-risk situations may protect them from relapse by prompting them to mobilize their coping resources. Current advances in mobile (Serre et al. Citation2015) and wearable technology (Shiffman Citation2009) provide novel opportunities for craving research to detect such high-risk situations, by enabling continues real-time monitoring in natural environments (Intille Citation2012).

Shiffman (Citation2009) argues that since substance use is known to be episodic, dynamic, complex, and highly influenced by the environment, a situational and momentary focus to evaluate theory would be natural. However, this situational (or ecological) and momentary approach in research is novel and the number of studies in craving research is still limited (Serre et al. Citation2015). As a result, not much is known yet about how craving relates to substance use in real-time in the real world (Wray et al. Citation2014). In fact, most craving research so far has been carried out in a laboratory setting (Drummond Citation2001). Drummond (Citation2001) argues that short-term, cue-elicited responses may be more relevant to predict relapse than withdrawal or background variables related to craving. He states that little attention has been paid to this dynamic character in existing craving measures, with only a few examples of studies using ecological momentary assessment. Concluding, real-time monitoring in a person’s natural environment has great potential for both craving research and addiction treatment.

Before being able to apply real-time ecological monitoring in craving research, the essential parameters in this real-time process of craving need to be identified. However, a lack of consensus on a definition for the concept of craving, and the multitude of available theoretical models with numerous variables (Skinner & Aubin Citation2010) impedes the identification and prioritization of relevant parameters. Therefore, the present study aims to critically review the current alcohol craving literature in order to select the essential parameters to include in real-time ecological alcohol craving research. With a focus on the process of craving and relapse in persons with alcohol addiction, in contrast to the process of developing an addiction. To achieve this objective, a theory-centered approach was adopted in which craving models are reviewed.

For this concise, yet comprehensive review, we employed a heuristic review methodology to disclose the most relevant evidence efficiently. Applying a more common, exhaustive review methodology (e.g. Higgins and Green (Citation2011)) would be practically infeasible for the purpose of our review, due to both the sheer quantity and the heterogeneity of evidence to be considered. As a first step, we reduced the number of models to be included by starting with Skinner and Aubin’s review (Skinner and Aubin Citation2010), which was built on earlier reviews by Tiffany (Citation1999) and Drummond (Citation2001).

Models of alcohol craving

Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) classified 18 craving models into four categories: conditioning, cognitive, psychobiological and motivational. These models reflect that craving is: (1) a multidimensional construct, (2) examined by a variety of scientific disciplines, yet (3) no model appears to be clearly better than all the others (Drummond Citation2001). These 18 models are listed in .

Table 1. Descriptions of craving models, as reviewed by Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010). The left column presents the model name and its founding publication as identified by Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010). The central column briefly describes the model, and the right column shows Skinner and Aubin’s (Citation2010) critical appraisal of the various models.

The review of Skinner and Aubin is primarily a descriptive taxonomy of craving models without the aim of providing a ranking among them, leading to difficulty in choosing a viable theoretical framework. As can be seen in , for several models the authors provided no criticisms. Either this implies a lack of available evidence for a critical appraisal of these models, or Skinner and Aubin’s (Citation2010) review may have lacked sufficient rigor in retrieving relevant evidence in support of these models. Here, we build on their work by systematically disclosing the empirical evidence for all these models in an attempt to identify viable theoretical frameworks. Of the 18 models, the four conditional models were excluded a priori since Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) convincingly argued that the assumption that craving is merely a conditioning concept has been disproven.

Material and methods

To test the validity of the fourteen remaining models, decision criteria were determined. These decision criteria served as assessment rules to critically evaluate model assumptions against the empirical evidence. These decision criteria were derived from two sources.

First, the existing criticisms by Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) presented in were reformulated into decision criteria. Second, decision criteria were derived from empirical evidence, as outlined in the Literature Search Strategy section. After defining these decision criteria, an empirically based evaluation of the models can be made and viable theoretical frameworks of craving are determined. The procedure to arrive upon this framework based on the decision criteria is described in the ‘Determining Viable Theoretical frameworks’ section, and graphically depicted in . After determining viable theoretical frameworks of craving, parameters are retrieved from the models. These parameters are assessed on relevance for the dynamic and fluctuating character of craving, which is further explained in the ‘Selecting Parameters for Real-time Ecological Craving Monitoring’ section.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the extraction of decision criteria. M represents the number of models included or excluded at each step of the procedure.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of the extraction of decision criteria. M represents the number of models included or excluded at each step of the procedure.

Literature search strategy

Due to the wide scope of this review, a more selective rather than exhaustive search strategy was employed. The aim of this strategy was to find relevant empirical studies for each model, which assessed the validity of the corresponding model assumptions. We assumed that researchers who are investigating model assumptions referenced its original founding publication. The article search could consequently be restricted to snowballing through the citations of these founding publications, instead of having to search through all articles available. However, due to the high number and heterogeneity of articles retrieved, simply including all articles found trough snowballing was still infeasible. To further restrict the number of relevant articles, the articles were classified into homogeneous groups of content through key term classification (Siddiqi & Sharan Citation2015). A key term classification algorithm generated a frequency table of key terms per model found in the retrieved articles. Articles containing a high frequency key term (found in more than four articles) were then included for reviewing. This threshold increased the likelihood of making relevant inferences based on multiple empirical studies instead of a single one. This threshold of four articles seemed to be a relevant cutoff, considering that nine was the next number of articles found for a keyterm. We used the collective results of the empirical studies within a homogeneous group of content, to draw inferences on the validity of the model assumption related to that content.

Summarizing, a 5-step search strategy was used, consisting of a combination of snowballing and a subsequent key term extraction algorithm, as follows:

  1. Searching articles citing the founding publication. According to Greenhalgh and Peacock (Citation2005), a snowballing approach is the most efficient way of collecting articles in a literature review. The snowballing method was applied by searching Scopus for all articles citing the founding publication of each model up to 26 April 2016.

  2. Applying exclusion criteria. Articles found in the preceding step, were excluded if they met one of the following criteria: (1) performed animal or other nonhuman studies; (2) studies did not involve alcohol; and (3) non-English articles.

  3. Key term classification algorithm. The key terms for each model were defined using an automated data-driven approach, with a key term classification algorithm. The key term classification algorithm was written with MATLAB 15 © (The MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, 2000) and is available in the online supplementary materials. The key term classification algorithm extracts key terms from the articles by searching through the titles and keyword fields (keyword fields as defined by articles’ authors), resulting in a frequency table of single and combinations of words (keyterms) found for each model. The frequency tables can be requested from the authors.

  4. Removal of nondiscriminatory key terms. As expected, the key terms with the highest numbers of articles were those that corresponded to the review context, namely: craving, alcohol and/or urge. However, these key terms did not provide a relevant classification since all articles contained them. As all models are craving models, it is expected that all articles citing these models would have craving as keywords or title words. Non-useful key terms were those that pertained to: (1) conjunction/determiners (e.g. ‘and’), (2) the context of the review (e.g., ‘alcohol addiction’), (3) the subject of interest (i.e., ‘craving’), and (4) a descriptors of statistical relations (e.g., ‘correlation’). Several models did not have a key term occurring in at least four articles, indicating an insufficient number of homogenous articles to support these models. Subsequently, these models were not further included in this literature review.

  5. Full articles scanned on relevance. The first author read the articles of the homogeneous groups and manually removed irrelevant articles, often due to an irrelevant target population. As the scope of this review is on the process of craving and relapse in persons with alcohol addiction, populations of for example social drinkers, often student samples, are considered not relevant for our purpose. After inferences are drawn for a specific model, an overall decision criterion based upon that single model inferences was defined in order to assess all models included in the review.

illustrates a flowchart of the literature search strategy.

Figure 2. Flowchart of the literature search strategy. N represents the number of articles included of excluded at each step of the procedure.

Figure 2. Flowchart of the literature search strategy. N represents the number of articles included of excluded at each step of the procedure.

Determining viable theoretical frameworks for alcohol craving research

The models were evaluated on the criteria from Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) and the literature review. The models and criteria obtained in the prior steps are presented in a tabular format in the Results section. Models that in their assumptions violated at least one of the found criteria were rejected. From the remaining models that met the decision criteria, model parameters were selected and evaluated on their relevance for real-time and real world alcohol craving research.

Selecting parameters for real-time and real world craving research

Parameters were assessed as relevant when they fluctuated over short time periods, thus reflecting the episodic, and dynamic nature of substance use. Additionally, since craving is a construct primarily relevant within the context of attempts to sustain abstinence from drinking, the model parameters should be part of the process of craving and relapse rather than the process of substance use initiation or developing addiction. Furthermore, the parameters should allow clear operationalization in a natural environment.

Results

First, the findings from the literature review are presented. Second, an overview of the decision criteria obtained from all the sources is provided. Third, we present our decision for theoretical frameworks based on these criteria. Fourth, the selection of parameters from these theoretical frameworks relevant for real-time and real world craving research is presented.

Literature review

shows the results of the literature search strategy. For the models that have no defined key terms in the last column, no relevant key term was found. These eight models were excluded since these were assumed not to have sufficient evidence for the purpose of this review. Note that for these models, only a small number of articles were found after inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied, confirming the limited empirical evidence available for these models. The remaining six models were further reviewed.

Table 2. Results from snowballing and keyword extraction literature search strategy to identifying craving models. The first column shows the model with its founding publication. The second column shows the number of articles found with snowballing, and the third column shows the number of articles remaining after applying inclusion and exclusion criteria. The overview of key terms, the number of articles found on their basis, and the final number of relevant articles found are presented in the fourth column.

Model 1. Affective processing model of negative reinforcement (Baker et al. Citation2004)

The affective processing model of negative reinforcement was cited by 125 articles, of which 38 contained the key terms ‘negative affect’ or ‘negative’ or ‘affect’. Articles with the key terms ‘negative’ and ‘affect’ were also separately screened to avoid excluding articles that, for example, addressed both positive and negative affect but merely used the key term affect instead. Of these 38 articles, 21 were excluded. This resulted in a total of 17 articles based on the affective processing model in which negative affect is addressed, as further described in the following sections.

Negative affect

Fifteen articles found that negative affect is an important predictor of drinking or relapse (Hussong Citation2007; Daughters et al. Citation2009; Spada & Wells Citation2009; Witkiewitz & Villarroel Citation2009; Simons et al. Citation2010; Witkiewitz & Bowen Citation2010; Cranford et al. Citation2011; Ostafin & Brooks Citation2011; Witkiewitz et al. Citation2011; Macpherson et al. Citation2012; McHugh et al. Citation2013; Schlauch et al. Citation2013; Simons et al. Citation2014; Treloar et al. Citation2015; Woud et al. Citation2015). Only two articles found that negative affect had no impact on relapse (Holt et al. Citation2012; Kabbani et al. Citation2014). Two studies found that higher positive affect – not negative affect – was associated with increased alcohol consumption (Peacock et al. Citation2015; Treloar et al. Citation2015). Only McHugh et al. (Citation2013) found an effect of both positive and negative affect.

To conclude, given the strong evidence in favor of Baker and colleagues’ (Baker et al. Citation2004) model, negative affect is an important predictor of relapse. There are only some articles that find no relationship or that claim that positive and not negative affect is a relevant cause of relapse.

Conclusion

The model should incorporate negative affect as a predictor of relapse.

Model 2. Cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999)

For the cognitive processing model, the key concepts found in the literature with the key term extraction algorithm were: attentional bias and working memory. In our literature selection, attentional bias occurred in 34 of the 290 articles, of which 13 were excluded. Additionally, 15 articles contained the key term working memory of which seven articles were excluded. Of the eight articles remaining, the findings were uninformative about the place and relevance of working memory in the process of craving, therefore, work memory was excluded in the review. The 20 articles relating to attentional bias are discussed below.

Attentional bias

In the context of substance abuse, attentional bias refers to the observation that experienced alcohol users pay more attention to alcohol-related cues than neutral cues (Field et al. Citation2009). According to the cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999), alcohol use is an automatic process, upon which patients have no influence. Therefore, high attentional bias towards alcohol cues compared to neutral cues in alcohol dependent subjects is expected.

In 12 articles, it was shown that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias towards alcohol cues than non-dependent controls (Carrigan et al. Citation2004; Dickter et al. Citation2014; Fadardi & Cox Citation2006, Citation2009; Fridrici et al. Citation2013; Garland et al. Citation2012a,Citationb; Lusher et al. Citation2004; Noël et al. Citation2007; Sharma et al. Citation2001; Townshend & Duka Citation2001; Wilcockson & Pothos Citation2015). Only two studies found no or a negative difference between respondents who are alcohol dependent and respondents who are not (Ryan Citation2002; Townshend & Duka Citation2007).

Field et al. (Citation2009) performed a meta-analysis of 68 independent data sets that together suggested a small but significant relationship between attentional bias and craving. In their review on attentional bias in addictive behaviors, Field and Cox (Citation2008) concluded that attentional bias causes craving and vice versa. Cox et al. (Citation2007) found that low attentional bias predicts alcohol reduction in excessive drinkers. Snelleman et al. (Citation2015) found no prediction by attentional bias on relapse.

Three articles reviewed the effect of attentional bias retraining or alcohol attention control training. Schoenmakers et al. (Citation2010) and Field et al. (Citation2007) reported no effects of attentional bias retraining on craving nor relapse. Only alcohol attention control training (Fadardi & Cox Citation2009) led to a reduction of both attentional bias and alcohol consumption in harmful drinkers.

Although attentional bias occurs in dependent drinkers and may cause craving, there is less conclusive evidence that attentional bias predicts relapse or that modifying attentional bias contributes to recovery. The proposed automaticity of alcohol addiction according to the cognitive processing model still remains debatable.

Conclusion

The model should explain that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias towards alcohol cues than nondependent drinkers.

Model 3. Three-pathway psychobiological model (Verheul et al. Citation1999)

Two key terms were notably present: reward and withdrawal. Note that these represent the reward and relief pathways, but not the third obsessive pathway of the three-pathway model (Verheul et al. Citation1999). Seventeen articles were found with the keyterm reward, from which 11 articles were excluded after scanning the full article. For the key term withdrawal, nine articles were found of which five were excluded because of irrelevance. The remaining four articles did not provide enough information to draw inferences on; therefore, withdrawal was excluded as keyterm.

Reward

Heinz et al. (Citation2003) found that drinking in positive emotional states contributes to reward craving. Two studies developed relief and reward craving test (Ooteman et al. Citation2006; Glöckner-Rist et al. Citation2013), where groups of relief and reward craving drinkers were both smaller than the remaining subjects which were defined as combination or no craving drinkers. Correspondingly, Grusser et al. (Citation2006) found that problem drinkers showed a significantly higher amount of both reward and relief craving. Only Martinotti and colleagues (Martinotti et al. Citation2013) examined a questionnaire on all pathways of the model, namely reward, relief and obsessive craving. They confirmed that alcohol-dependent subjects had a higher mean score on all craving factors as compared to the normative sample.

To conclude, four studies found that the reward and relief pathways seem to occur either simultaneously or not at all in individual cases. Additionally, alcohol dependents seem to score higher than nondependent drinkers on the reward, relief and compulsive pathways of the model, not merely on reward. These findings are partly in line with the model, since the model claims that multiple pathways can occur simultaneously or be entirely absent. However, these conclusions are tentative due to the small number of studies upon which they are based.

Conclusion

Inconclusive.

Model 4. Incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993)

For the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993), attentional bias was found to be a relevant key term in the literature. A total of 55 articles discussed attentional bias, of which 35 articles could be excluded due to irrelevance. Eleven articles overlapped the search on attentional bias of the cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999). Nine of these articles mentioned both the cognitive processing and the incentive sensitization models when explaining the reasoning behind attentional bias (Townshend & Duka Citation2001; Lusher et al. Citation2004; Noël et al. Citation2007; Townshend & Duka Citation2007; Field et al. Citation2009; Garland et al. Citation2010; Schoenmakers et al. Citation2010; Dickter et al. Citation2014; Waters & Green Citation2003). The nine additional articles are reviewed below.

Attentional bias

Of the nine articles, three showed that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias than the control group (Waters & Green Citation2003; Miller & Fillmore Citation2011; Weafer & Fillmore Citation2012). Two of these studies (Miller & Fillmore Citation2011; Weafer & Fillmore Citation2012) looked additionally at the effect of alcohol intoxication on attentional bias and found that attentional bias decreases through alcohol drinking. This was in contrast to the findings of Schoenmakers et al. (Citation2008) and Roberts et al. (Citation2014), who found that low doses of alcohol increased attentional bias. Duka and Townshend (Citation2004) found an increasing effect of attentional bias for a low alcohol dose and a decreasing effect on high doses of alcohol. This result differs from Schoenmakers and Wiers (Citation2010) later findings and the findings of van den Wildenberg et al. (Citation2006), where no significant effect of priming on attentional bias was found.

Multiple articles claim that attentional bias exists, as supported by both the cognitive processing model and the incentive sensitization theory of addiction. Furthermore, the incentive sensitization theory claims that repeated alcohol intake increases the automatized process. Multiple studies looked at the effect of alcohol priming, but the results were inconclusive. It is not clear from the model whether an increased attentional bias was expected immediately after priming. Therefore, no additional decision criterion was formulated.

Conclusion

The model should explain that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias towards alcohol cues than nondependent drinkers.

Model 5. Theory of neural opponent motivation (Koob & Le Moal Citation1997)

For the neural opponent motivation model, stress is an important concept as a predictor of relapse. The key term stress was found within 17 articles, and subsequently 12 of these articles were found relevant for this review.

Stress

According to Fox et al. (Citation2007), exposure to stress produced significant increases in alcohol craving, anxiety, and negative emotions. Ayer et al. (Citation2011) found that daily stress predicts alcohol consumption and vice versa. Blomeyer et al. (Citation2013) found that alcohol use increases with the number of stressful life events. Saraceno et al. (Citation2009) performed a literature study and found that three studies reported a link between stress and problematic alcohol use and two studies suggested that individuals drink to regulate negative affect and to cope with negative life events.

Multiple studies found that alcohol-dependent subjects have a decreased physiological reaction to stressors (Lovallo Citation2011), as determined, for example, by measuring heart rate (Panknin et al. Citation2002; Brkic et al. Citation2015) or cortisol levels (Lovallo et al. Citation2000; Pratt & Davidson Citation2009; Nakajima et al. Citation2013). Lovallo (Citation2006) argued that this decreased reaction may predict a high risk for future risky drugs and alcohol behavior. Nakajima et al. (Citation2013) found that moderate alcohol consumption was associated with an increased subjective response and altered hormonal sensitivity to stress in social drinkers who have the potential of becoming problem drinkers.

To conclude, stress seems to increases the risk of relapse. Additionally, multiple studies found a difference between alcoholics and healthy subjects on their stress response and baseline physiology. However, this particular finding is not suitable to formulate as decision criteria.

Conclusion

The model should incorporate stress as a predictor of relapse.

Model 6. Motivational model of alcohol use (Cox & Klinger Citation1988)

Drinking motives or motives were found as relevant key terms in 157 articles. A total of 137 articles were excluded after reading the full text, often because of irrelevant target populations. The remaining 20 articles are reviewed below.

Drinking motives

Three studies looked at the difference between the positive and negative motives of dependent drinkers. Higher scores on negative reinforcement motives were found in dependent drinkers than in moderate drinkers (Mezquita et al. Citation2011) and such higher scores were found to be generally associated with drinking problems (Anderson et al. Citation2013). This effect on alcohol consumption of positive reinforcement motives was not found by Engels et al. (Citation2005). Galen et al. (Citation2001) found that drinking motives mediate the effects of both positive and negative expectations on alcohol use and alcohol-related problems.

Eleven articles reported on different drinking motives as defined by the motivational model of alcohol use (Cooper (Citation1994)): enhancement, coping, social and conformity motives. Articles that found an effect of one of these motives on dependence or drinking are shown in .

Table 3. Effect of drinking motives on dependence or drinking.

Most of the studies related to drinking motives involved the different motivations defined by Cooper (Citation1994). However, these are not directly relevant for the evaluation of the models’ assumption, existing of motivation to drink. These studies do presume that alcoholics have motives to drink. However, it is always difficult to know whether this is a biased effect. When the researcher asks about specific drinking motives, respondents might feel that they are assumed to have drinking motives and, therefore, confabulate them. Consequently, it is not clear, based on these findings, whether drinking is motivated and, therefore, alcohol drinking is the individual’s conscious decision. No decision criteria can be determined based on the studies presented.

Conclusion

Inconclusive.

Decision criteria

As mentioned before, based on the review, decision criteria were derived from Skinner and Aubin’s (Citation2010) critical appraisal and the literature review. The following criteria were found in the literature review:

(1) The model should incorporate negative affect as a predictor of relapse. (Affective processing model of negative reinforcement)

(2) The model should explain that dependent drinkers have a higher attentional bias towards alcohol cues than nondependent drinkers. (Cognitive processing and Incentive sensitization model)

(3) The model should incorporate stress as an increased risk of relapse. (Theory of neural opponent motivation)

In addition to decision criteria based on the literature, decision criteria were also retrieved from Skinner and Aubin’s (Citation2010) critical assessment of models (as summarized in ). As the literature search strategy yielded six of these models to be included in the final review, only their criticisms regarding these models were included as decision criteria. This generated the following additional criteria:

(4) The model explains why people experience craving and relapse, sometimes even after long periods of abstaining. (Affective processing model of negative reinforcement, (Skinner & Aubin Citation2010, p. 611))

(5) The model should explain irrational/compulsive side of craving is. (Motivational Model, (Skinner & Aubin Citation2010, p. 618))

(6) The model should explain how craving can occur without consumption or how consumption occurs without craving. (Three-pathway psychobiological model, (Skinner & Aubin Citation2010, p. 614))

For the affective processing model of negative reinforcement, Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) also formulated the criticism that the relief of aversive withdrawal symptoms cannot be an important determinant of addiction because some drugs are highly addictive without producing highly unpleasant syndromes (p. 613). However, this criterion was not included as it is not accurate for alcohol addiction.

Determining viable theoretical frameworks for alcohol craving

Based on these criteria the viability of the six remaining models was assessed. shows how the models are evaluated on the six decision criteria defined in the previous steps. Only the critical findings for each of the six included models are described next. When a model suffices on a criterion this is not separately discussed and can be found in the table.

Table 4. Results for each model on the decision criteria in order to determine the most suitable model.

The affective processing model of negative reinforcement (Baker et al. Citation2004) claims that attentional bias occurs, because the organism focuses on the response option (i.e., drug use) that is associated with quick and efficient control next to relief of negative affect. Whether this bias is the same attentional bias found in the empirical studies in this review is unclear, since no articles reviewed the effect of negative affect on attentional bias. However, the model does not seem to pose a contradiction to this decision criterion. The only other critique is the weak justification of craving and relapse after long periods of abstaining. Since the model does not violates either criteria, the model is included as theoretical framework.

The cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999) claims that drinking is an automatized process and not an alleviation of negative affect and stress, as found in the literature review. Tiffany (Citation1990) argues that alcohol cues from the environment, the emotional network or the time of the day, trigger this automatized process. Negative affect and stress could be triggers from the emotional network; however, this is merely interpretation. Tiffany (Citation1999) claims that drinking is an automated process, which can be blocked when an individual choosesto abstain, which subsequently causes craving. However, it is not clear why a person does chooses not to abstain, but 'chooses' alcohol above other important things in life (e.g. having work, friendships). However, since the model does not violate the criteria explicitly, this model is included as theoretical framework.

The literature review showed that empirical evidence only supports the relief (decision criterion 1) and obsessive (decision criterion 5) of the three pathways model. This could be an artifact of the focus in this review on addicted individuals as the target population. For example, the reward path defined by the model could be evident in social drinkers, but not in dependent drinkers. Additionally, the model does not explain why people relapse without experiencing craving or why they experience craving and relapse after long periods of abstaining. More importantly, though, is the lack of validation of the pathways. The obsessive path was only investigated by one study (Martinotti et al. Citation2013). Other studies related merely focused on the relief and reward pathways, showing that participants experience either both craving paths or none of the paths (Grusser et al. Citation2006; Ooteman et al. Citation2006; Glöckner-Rist et al. Citation2013). Although the authors propose that individuals can indeed experience all or none of the craving paths, the apparent lack of studies showing any differentiation between paths indicates this core assumption of having different pathways to be invalid and the model is therefore rejected.

For the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993), only the attentional bias experiments provided support. It is evident that attentional bias towards alcohol cues exist in dependent drinkers. However, it is not clear from the fact that it is present in dependent drinkers, what the role is of this attentional bias in the process of craving (Christiansen et al. Citation2015). It is therefore questionable whether attentional bias provides viable evidence for the model. Additionally, attentional bias only yielded small effect on craving and no effect on relapse. The model does not explain why craving is not always followed by relapse. Furtermore, when describing this process of relapse, Robinson and Berridge (Citation1993) mention stress and priming as predictors of relapse and not negative affect. Concluding, the case could be made that the model is incomplete rather than invalid and therefore the model is included as part of the theoretical frameworks.

The theory of neural opponent motivation (Koob & Le Moal Citation1997) is a neural model based primarily on research with animals, whereas this review was limited to human research. However, enough studies with a human sample were found to evaluate the model. Koob and Le Moal (Citation1997) give no explicit explanation of attentional bias. An additional limitation of the model seems to be that it is not explained how relapse could occur without craving. However, these two limitations expose rather an incomplete model than an invalid one, leading to the inclusion of this model as theoretical framework.

The motivational model claimed that drinking is always a motivated choice. The motivational model Averting negative affect situations is one of the motivation for drinking. However, stress and attentional bias are not explicitly explained. Additionally, Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) effectively argued that this model ignores the irrational side of craving. Moreover, no sufficient evidence for deliberate decision making was found and as a result this model is currently rejected. Further research should be focused on substantiating this motivated choice.

Summarizing, the affective processing model of negative reinforcement (Baker et al. Citation2004), the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993), cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999) and the theory of neural opponent motivation (Koob and Le Moal Citation1997) are determined to be (partly) relevant as viable theoretical frameworks.

Selecting parameters for real-time and real world craving research

In this final step, parameters are retrieved from the theoretical framework determined in the previous section. As we stated previously, the parameter should be suitable for real-time monitoring, implying that the parameters should:

  1. describe the continuous and cyclical process of craving and relapse in an enduring addiction, rather than the process of developing an addiction.

  2. be an episodic state that fluctuates over time.

  3. be operationalizable in a natural environment.

shows how the models are evaluated for parameter selection to fit the real-time and real world focus of this review.

Table 5. Parameter selection from the different models in the theoretical frameworks defined.

With regard to the first two criteria, parameters from all four models appear suitable. The cognitive processing model and the affective processing model of negative reinforcement describe the process of relapse rather than the development of addiction, as preferred in this review. The incentive sensitization model and the theory of neural opponent motivation explain both the process of relapse and the development of addiction. All four models incorporate parameters that fluctuate over time. The specific time varying parameters can be found in .

With regard to the third criterion, operationalizability in a real world setting, from three models potentially suitable parameters emerge. The affective processing model of negative reinforcement (Baker et al. Citation2004) suggests negative affect and stress as parameters. Negative affect and stress are already used in some real-time and real world studies, for multiple examples see Serre et al. (Citation2015). For the cognitive processing model, Tiffany (Citation1990) argues that alcohol cues from the environment, the emotional network or the time of the day, trigger the automatized process. The nature of the triggers is described in a rather general way and the translation to concrete cues seem to be dependent on the individual. In the articles included in the review neither clear operationalizations of these cues were formulated, nor was the predictive value of such cues on the automatized process studied. Therefore, additional research is warranted to prevent arbitrary selection of specific cues. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993) would lead to stress and priming by alcohol or other drugs as parameters. However, since priming is difficult to operationalize in a natural context and is not supported by empirical evidence within this review, it is not selected as relevant parameter. The theory of neural opponent motivation (Koob & Le Moal Citation1997) also proposes stress and negative affect as relevant parameters, but adds no other explicit parameters in line with the focus on real-time and real world research. Additionally, attentional bias, since emerging from this review, could be deemed a relevant parameter for the cognitive processing model and incentive sensitization theory of addiction. However in the case of attentional bias, two issues remain. First, although it is suggested that attentional bias is an output of the underlying automated process at a moment in time, evidence to support this hypothesis is lacking. Second, attentional bias is found to have only a small effect on craving (Field Citation2009) and no convincing evidence for a causal influence on relapse is found, in agreement with a meta-analysis of Christiansen et al. (Citation2015). Christiansen et al. (Citation2015) propose that future research should use experience sampling methodology to assess the clinical significance of fluctuations in attentional bias over time. However, currently it is not a parameter supported by evidence.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to identify viable theoretical frameworks of craving as the first step towards selecting essential parameters for real-time alcohol craving research. The results of this review show that negative affect and stress are relevant parameters, since the three craving models with sufficient empirical foundations support these. Below, we further discuss our findings, offer suggestions on how to use the finding in real-time and real world craving research and argue as to where our results or research methodology could be of further use in current research fields.

In this review, six empirically supported models from all three domains (cognitive, psychobiological, and motivational models) emerged that contribute to our understanding of craving and relapse in alcohol addiction. Based on these six models, three key terms with robust evidence appear to be stress, negative affect and attentional bias. Additionally, for drinking motives, reward, relief, withdrawal and working memory limited support was found. When restricting the evidence to the focus on ecological monitoring, stress and negative affect clearly remain as viable constructs. Attentional bias, although found as a possible predictor of craving, is not proposed as a parameter in the framework since it has not shown to be predictive of relapse. Two parameters found within the viable models without empirical support were alcohol cues and priming. Although alcohol cues have been shown relevant triggers for craving in a lab setting, often using exposure to alcohol related pictures, this concept lacks ecological validation as well as standardized operationalization procedures in an ecological context (Carter & Tiffany Citation2001). Similarly, priming seems difficult to operationalize in a natural context, and was also not supported by sufficient empirical evidence within this review. Concluding, in addition to stress and negative affect the parameter alcohol cues could be included for lab experiments, and when focusing solely on relatively stable conditions of dependent drinkers attentional bias is a well proven concept.

The findings of this review, and in particular the identified theoretical frameworks and corresponding parameters for craving, could be useful in studying high-risk negative affect and stress situations with increased chances of relapsing. As suggested by Rohsenow and Monti (Citation1999), creating awareness of and especially prompting individuals to activate their coping skills in these high-risk situations might lower the chances of relapse. However, in order to timely detect high-risk negative affect and stress situations, continuous measurement of the fluctuating parameters is needed. Baker (in: Shaw Citation2006) recommends using biomonitoring (with wearables) to detect stressful events, since individuals may actually be unaware of upcoming stress or negative moods. Shaw (Citation2006) then explains that heightened skin conductance, heart pulse rate and muscle tension, usually accompany higher states of arousal, and that these physiological indicators can be measured by wearable technology available today. We propose therefore that there might be a benefit of creating awareness and prompting individuals based on physiological responses instead of assuming that a person self will be always aware of underlying negative affect and stress. This implies that, physiological measurements of psychological events has added potential to detect high-risk situations early and prevent relapse by alarming an individual prior to the affective peak of the event.

Notably, this review also illustrates the dearth of evidence, both on the level of models, as with regard to single parameters within models. For twelve models insufficient (alcohol related) human studies were retrieved to make any inferences. For the six remaining models with empirical support, no more than a single key term emerged per model, implying that multiple other concepts still need to be explored for full model validation. In fact, this review showed that research directly validating foundations and assumptions of craving models remains limited, as was already mentioned by Drummond in Citation2001. We found that, although most craving researchers do justify their study design and rationale with one or more model, an explicit attempt to discuss the theoretical implications of their findings is often lacking or neglected. Apparently, theories are primarily applied deductively, while the inductive loop from observations back to theory is largely overlooked. In sum, this calls for more studies validating theory, and for scholars to discuss in more depth the implications of their studies on the theories used. In fact, this review showed that the research on the foundations and assumptions of craving models remains limited.

We specifically developed the present review method for the purposes of determining viable theoretical frameworks, without having to perform a full meta-analysis for every model or every relationship within each model. In this, we relied on the scrutiny within the scientific community to cite the founding article of each model. Aside from otherwise being practically infeasible, this would probably have turned out inproductive as well, as multiple models are descriptive in nature, rather than having a clear predictive factor structure. The review methodology presented in this paper could potentially be useful for multiple other purposes in which there is a similar need to feasibly condense such a large quantity of evidence. The only prerequisite would be possessing founding publications that would allow a snowballing search approach. The method has shown to be a promising way to efficiently arrive at relevant theoretical frameworks within a complex, multidisciplinary field.

Finally, even though the models are denoted as craving theories by Skinner and Aubin (Citation2010) no empirical findings in this review show the essential position of craving in the process of relapse. Several explanations for this finding are conceivable. First, the large majority of studies employ relapse or alcohol problems as dependent variable, rather than craving, which in turn might indicate a difficulty in operationalizing craving. Second, many studies may have included analyzes on craving, however found no significant results. Meaning that as in any review the risk of a publication bias (Ioannidis Citation2005) may also have occurred in this review. Third, assuming that the first explanation would be falsified, this lack of evidence regarding the concept of craving may actually reflect the non-viable nature of this construct. However, we tend to agree with Drummond (2001) who argues that it seems premature to dismiss the relevance craving, prior to consensus in the appropriate method to measure craving. Particularly he mentions that first more must be known about the human craving experience in the natural drinking environment.

Conclusions

In this review, the affective processing model of negative reinforcement (Baker et al. Citation2004), the cognitive processing model (Tiffany Citation1999), the incentive sensitization theory of addiction (Robinson & Berridge Citation1993) and the theory of neural opponent motivation (Koob & Le Moal Citation1997) were determined to be relevant for viable theoretical frameworks, leading to negative affect and stress as relevant parameters to include in real-time, real world alcohol craving research. Attentional bias, although found as a possible predictor of craving, is not proposed as a parameter in the framework since it has not shown to be predictive of relapse. Similarly, alcohol cues could be relevant triggers for craving, however this concept needs further operationalization and empirical foundation. Ultimately, we indicate that negative affect and stress emerge as the most promising parameters to include in real-time ecological craving research.

Supplemental material

IART_A_1309525__Supplemenatry_information.txt

Download Text (6.6 KB)

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Paul Mannée for constructing the key extraction algorithm in Matlab.

Disclosure statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Funding

This work was supported by the University of Twente’s Tech4people program.

References

  • Agrawal A, Dick DM, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Cooper ML, Sher KJ, Heath AC. 2008. Drinking expectancies and motives: a genetic study of young adult women. Addiction (Abingdon, Engl). 103:194–204.
  • Anderson KG, Briggs KEL, White HR. 2013. Motives to drink or not to drink: longitudinal relations among personality, motives, and alcohol use across adolescence and early adulthood. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 37:860–867.
  • Anton RF, Carolina S. 1999. What is craving? Models and implications for treatment. Alcohol Res Health. 23:165–173.
  • Ayer LA, Harder VS, Rose GL, Helzer JE. 2011. Drinking and stress: an examination of sex and stressor differences using IVR-based daily data. Drug Alcohol Depend. 115:205–212.
  • Baker TB, Morse E, Sherman JE. 1986. The motivation to use drugs: a psychobiological analysis of urges. Nebr Symp Motiv. 34:257–323.
  • Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. 2004. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 111:33–51.
  • Beseler CL, Aharonovich E, Keyes KM, Hasin DS. 2008. Adult transition from at-risk drinking to alcohol dependence: the relationship of family history and drinking motives. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 32:607–616.
  • Blomeyer D, Buchmann AF, Lascorz J, Zimmermann US, Esser G, Desrivieres S, Schmidt MH, Banaschewski T, Schumann G, Laucht M. 2013. Association of PER2 genotype and stressful life events with alcohol drinking in young adults. PLoS One. 8:e59136.
  • Breiner MJ, Stritzke WGK, Lang AR. 1999. Approaching avoidance. A step essential to the understanding of cravingAlcohol Res Health. 23:197–206.
  • Brkic S, Söderpalm B, Söderpalm A. 2015. A family history of Type 1 alcoholism differentiates alcohol consumption in high cortisol responders to stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 130:59–66.
  • Cadigan JM, Martens MP, Herman KC. 2015. A latent profile analysis of drinking motives among heavy drinking college students. Addict Behav. 51:100–105.
  • Carrigan MH, Drobes DJ, Randall CL. 2004. Attentional bias and drinking to cope with social anxiety. Psychol Addict Behav. 18:374–380.
  • Carter BL, Tiffany ST. 2001. The cue-availability paradigm: the effects of cigarette availability on cue reactivity in smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 9:183–90.
  • Christiansen P, Schoenmakers TM, Field M. 2015. Less than meets the eye: reappraising the clinical relevance of attentional bias in addiction. Addict Behav. 44:43–50.
  • Cooper ML. 1994. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess. 6:117–128.
  • Cox WM, Klinger E. 1988. A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol. 97:168–180.
  • Cox WM, Pothos EM, Hosier SG. 2007. Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers’ success in changing. Psychopharmacology. 192:499–510.
  • Cranford JA, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Zucker RA. 2011. Alcohol involvement as a function of co-occurring alcohol use disorders and major depressive episode: evidence from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Drug Alcohol Depend. 117:145–151.
  • Daughters SB, Reynolds EK, MacPherson L, Kahler CW, Danielson CK, Zvolensky M, Lejuez CW. 2009. Distress tolerance and early adolescent externalizing and internalizing symptoms: the moderating role of gender and ethnicity. Behav Res Ther. 47:198–205.
  • Dickter CL, Forestell CA, Hammett PJ, Young CM. 2014. Relationship between alcohol dependence, escape drinking, and early neural attention to alcohol-related cues. Psychopharmacology. 231:2031–2040.
  • Doyle SR, Donovan DM, Simpson TL. 2011. Validation of a nine-dimensional measure of drinking motives for use in clinical applications: the desired effects of drinking scale. Addict Behav. 36:1052–1060.
  • Drerup ML, Johnson TJ, Bindl S. 2011. Mediators of the relationship between religiousness/spirituality and alcohol problems in an adult community sample. Addict Behav. 36:1317–1320.
  • Drummond DC. 2001. Theories of drugs craving, ancient and modern. Addiction. 96:33–46.
  • Duka T, Townshend JM. 2004. The priming effect of alcohol pre-load on attentional bias to alcohol-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology. 176:353–362.
  • Engels RCME, Wiers RW, Lemmers L, Overbeek G. 2005. Drinking motives, alcohol expectancies, self-efficacy, and drinking patterns. J Drug Educ. 35:147–166.
  • Fadardi JS, Cox WM. 2006. Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment?. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 185:169–178.
  • Fadardi JS, Cox WM. 2009. Reversing the sequence: reducing alcohol consumption by overcoming alcohol attentional bias. Drug Alcohol Depend. 101:137–145.
  • Field M, Cox WM. 2008. Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 97:1–20.
  • Field M, Duka T, Eastwood B, Child R, Santarcangelo M, Gayton M. 2007. Experimental manipulation of attentional biases in heavy drinkers: do the effects generalise? Psychopharmacology. 192:593–608.
  • Field M, Munafò MR, Franken IHA. 2009. A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull. 135:589–607.
  • Fitzgerald K-A, Long CG. 2012. The influence of drinking motives and anxiety on low-, moderate- and high-risk drinkers. J Subst Use. 17:240–248.
  • Fox HC, Bergquist KL, Hong K-I, Sinha R. 2007. Stress-induced and alcohol cue-induced craving in recently abstinent alcohol-dependent individuals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 31:395–403.
  • Fridrici C, Leichsenring-Driessen C, Driessen M, Wingenfeld K, Kremer G, Beblo T. 2013. The individualized alcohol Stroop task: no attentional bias toward personalized stimuli in alcohol-dependents. Psychol Addict Behav. 27:62–70.
  • Galen LW, Henderson MJ, Coovert MD. 2001. Alcohol expectancies and motives in a substance abusing male treatment sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 62:205–214.
  • Garland EL, Franken IHA, Howard MO. 2012a. Cue-elicited heart rate variability and attentional bias predict alcohol relapse following treatment. Psychopharmacology. 222:17–26.
  • Garland EL, Franken IH, Sheetz JJ, Howard MO. 2012b. Alcohol attentional bias is associated with autonomic indices of stress-primed alcohol cue-reactivity in alcohol-dependent patients. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 20:225–235.
  • Garland EL, Gaylord SA, Boettiger CA, Howard MO. 2010. Mindfulness training modifies cognitive, affective, and physiological mechanisms implicated in alcohol dependence: results of a randomized controlled pilot trial. J Psychoact Drugs. 42:177–192.
  • Gilson K-M, Bryant C, Bei B, Komiti A, Jackson H, Judd F. 2013. Validation of the Drinking Motives Questionnaire (DMQ) in older adults. Addict Behav. 38:2196–2202.
  • Glavak Tkalić R, Sučić I, Dević I. 2013. Motivation for substance use: why do people use alcohol, tobacco and marijuana? Drustvena Istrazivanja/ J Gen Social Iss. 22:601–625.
  • Glöckner-Rist A, Lémenager T, Mann K. 2013. Reward and relief craving tendencies in patients with alcohol use disorders: results from the PREDICT study. Addict Behav. 38:1532–1540.
  • Greenhalgh T, Peacock R. 2005. Effectiveness and efficiency of search methods in systematic reviews of complex evidence: audit of primary sources. BMJ. 331:1064–1065.
  • Grusser SM, Mörsen CP, Flor H. 2006. Alcohol craving in problem and occasional alcohol drinkers. Alcohol Alcohol. 41:421–425.
  • Hasking P, Lyvers M, Carlopio C. 2011. The relationship between coping strategies, alcohol expectancies, drinking motives and drinking behaviour. Addict Behav. 36:479–487.
  • Heinz A, Lober S, Georgi A, Wrase J, Hermann D, Rey E-R, Welleck S, Mann K. 2003. Reward craving and withdrawal relief craving: assessment of different motivational pathways to alcohol intake. Alcohol Alcohol. 38:35–39.
  • Higgins JPT, Green S. 2011. Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Vol. 4. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Holt LJ, Litt MD, Cooney NL. 2012. Prospective analysis of early lapse to drinking and smoking among individuals in concurrent alcohol and tobacco treatment. Psychol Addict Behav. 26:561–572.
  • Hussong AM. 2007. Predictors of drinking immediacy following daily sadness: an application of survival analysis to experience sampling data. Addict Behav. 32:1054–1065.
  • Intille SS. 2012. Emerging technology for studying daily life. In: Mehl MR, Conner TS, editors. Handbook of research methods for studying daily life [Internet]. New York: The Guilford Press. Available from: http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=psyc9&NEWS=N&AN =2012-05165-015
  • Ioannidis JPA. 2005. Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Med. 2:0696–0701.
  • Kabbani R, Kambouropoulos N, Loxton NJ, Bunker R. 2014. Examining the affective tone of alcohol craving in young drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav. 28:1259–1264.
  • Koob GF, Le Moal M. 1997. Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science. 278:52–58.
  • Kristjansson SD, Agrawal A, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, Madden PAF, Cooper ML, Bucholz KK, Sher KJ, Lynskey MT, Heath AC. 2012. The relationship between rs3779084 in the dopa decarboxylase (DDC) gene and alcohol consumption is mediated by drinking motives in regular smokers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 36:162–170.
  • Kristjansson SD, Agrawal A, Littlefield AK, Pergadia ML, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, Sartor CE, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Cooper ML, Sher KJ, Heath AC. 2011. Drinking motives in female smokers: factor structure, alcohol dependence, and genetic influences. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 35:345–354.
  • Larimer ME, Palmer RS, Marlatt GA. 1999. Relapse prevention: an overview of Marlatt?s Cognitive-Cehavioral Model. Alcohol Res Health. 23:151–60.
  • Levitt A, Leonard KE. 2015. Insecure attachment styles, relationship-drinking contexts, and marital alcohol problems: testing the mediating role of relationship-specific drinking-to-cope motives. Psychol Addict Behav. 29:696–705.
  • Littlefield AK, Agrawal A, Ellingson JM, Kristjansson S, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Sher KJ. 2011. Does variance in drinking motives explain the genetic overlap between personality and alcohol use disorder symptoms? A twin study of young women. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 35:2242–2250.
  • Lovallo W. 2011. Do low levels of stress reactivity signal poor states of health? Biol Psychol. 86:121–128.
  • Lovallo WR. 2006. Cortisol secretion patterns in addiction and addiction risk. Int J Psychophysiol. 59:195–202.
  • Lovallo WR, Dickensheets SL, Myers DA, Thomas TL, Nixon SJ. 2000. Blunted stress cortisol response in abstinent alcoholic and polysubstance-abusing men. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 24:651–658.
  • Lowman C, Hunt WA, Litten RZ, Drummond DC. 2000. Research perspectives on alcohol craving: an overview. Addiction. 95(S2):45–54.
  • Lusher J, Chandler C, Ball D. 2004. Alcohol dependence and the alcohol Stroop paradigm: evidence and issues. Drug Alcohol Depend. 75:225–231.
  • Lyvers M, Hasking P, Hani R, Rhodes M, Trew E. 2010. Drinking motives, drinking restraint and drinking behaviour among young adults. Addict Behav. 35:116–122.
  • Mackie CJ, Conrod PJ, Rijsdijk F, Eley TC. 2011. A systematic evaluation and validation of subtypes of adolescent alcohol use motives: genetic and environmental contributions. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 35:420–430.
  • Macpherson L, Calvin NT, Richards JM, Guller L, Mayes LC, Crowley MJ, Daughters SB, Lejuez CW. 2012. Development and preliminary validation of a behavioral task of negative reinforcement underlying risk-taking and its relation to problem alcohol use in college freshmen. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 36:426–433.
  • Marlatt G. 1985. Cognitive factors in the relapse process. In. Marlatt J, Gordon G, editors. Relapse prevention: maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New York: Guilford Press; p. 128–200.
  • Martinotti G, Di Nicola M, Tedeschi D, Callea A, Di Giannantonio M, Janiri L. 2013. Craving Typology Questionnaire (CTQ): a scale for alcohol craving in normal controls and alcoholics. Comprehens Psychiatry. 54:925–932.
  • McHugh RK, Kaufman JS, Frost KH, Fitzmaurice GM, Weiss RD. 2013. Positive affect and stress reactivity in alcohol-dependent outpatients. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 74:152–157.
  • Mezquita L, Stewart SH, Ibáñez MI, Ruipérez MA, Villa H, Moya J, Ortet G. 2011. Drinking motives in clinical and general populations. Eur Addict Res. 17:250–261.
  • Miller MA, Fillmore MT. 2011. Persistence of attentional bias toward alcohol-related stimuli in intoxicated social drinkers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 117:184–189.
  • Nakajima M, Kumar S, Wittmers L, Scott MS, al’Absi M. 2013. Psychophysiological responses to stress following alcohol intake in social drinkers who are at risk of hazardous drinking. Biol Psychol. 93:9–16.
  • Noël X, Van Der Linden M, D’Acremont M, Bechara A, Dan B, Hanak C, Verbanck P. 2007. Alcohol cues increase cognitive impulsivity in individuals with alcoholism. Psychopharmacology. 192:291–298.
  • Ooteman W, Koeter M, Verheul R, Schippers G, Brink W. 2006. Development and validation of the Amsterdam Motives for Drinking Scale (Amds): an attempt to distinguish relief and reward drinkers. Alcohol Alcohol. 41:284–292.
  • Ostafin BD, Brooks JJ. 2011. Drinking for relief: negative affect increases automatic alcohol motivation in coping-motivated drinkers. Motiv Emot. 35:285–295.
  • Panknin TL, Dickensheets SL, Nixon SJ, Lovallo WR. 2002. Attenuated heart rate responses to public speaking in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 26:841–847.
  • Paulus MP, Tapert SF, Schulteis G. 2009. The role of interoception and alliesthesia in addiction. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 94:1–7.
  • Peacock A, Cash C, Bruno R, Ferguson SG. 2015. Day-by-day variation in affect, arousal and alcohol consumption in young adults. Drug Alcohol Rev. 34:588–594.
  • Piasecki TM, Cooper ML, Wood PK, Sher KJ, Shiffman S, Heath AC. 2014. Dispositional drinking motives: associations with appraised alcohol effects and alcohol consumption in an ecological momentary assessment investigation. Psychol Assess. 26:363–369.
  • Pratt WM, Davidson D. 2009. Role of the HPA axis and the A118G polymorphism of the mu-opioid receptor in stress-induced drinking behavior. Alcohol Alcohol (Oxford, UK). 44:358–365.
  • Redish AD, Jensen S, Johnson A. 2008. A unified framework for addiction: vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behav Brain Sci. 31:415–437.
  • Redish a. D. 2004. Addiction as a computational process gone awry. Science. 306:1944–1947.
  • Roberts W, Miller M. a, Weafer J, Fillmore MT. 2014. Heavy drinking and the role of inhibitory control of attention. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 22:133–140.
  • Robinson TE, Berridge KC. 1993. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Rev. 18:247–291.
  • Rodriguez LM, Knee CR, Neighbors C. 2014. Relationships can drive some to drink: relationship-contingent self-esteem and drinking problems. J Social Personal. 31:270–290.
  • Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM. 1999. Does urge to drink predict relapse after treatment? Alcohol Res Health. 23:225–232.
  • Ryan F. 2002. Attentional bias and alcohol dependence: a controlled study using the modified Stroop paradigm. Addict Behav. 27:471–482.
  • Saraceno L, Munafó M, Heron J, Craddock N, van den Bree MBM. 2009. Genetic and non-genetic influences on the development of co-occurring alcohol problem use and internalizing symptomatology in adolescence: a review. Addiction. 104:1100–1121.
  • Schlauch RC, Gwynn-shapiro D, Stasiewicz PR, Molnar DS, Lang AR. 2013. Addictive behaviors affect and craving: positive and negative affect are differentially associated with approach and avoidance inclinations. Addict Behav. 38:1970–1979.
  • Schoenmakers TM, de Bruin M, Lux IFM, Goertz AG, Van Kerkhof DHAT, Wiers RW. 2010. Clinical effectiveness of attentional bias modification training in abstinent alcoholic patients. Drug Alcohol Depend. 109:30–36.
  • Schoenmakers TM, Wiers RW. 2010. Craving and attentional bias respond differently to alcohol priming: a field study in the pub. Eur Addict Res. 16:9–16.
  • Schoenmakers T, Wiers RW, Field M. 2008. Effects of a low dose of alcohol on cognitive biases and craving in heavy drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 197:169–178.
  • Serre F, Fatseas M, Swendsen J, Auriacombe M. 2015. Ecological momentary assessment in the investigation of craving and substance use in daily life: a systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. 148:1–20.
  • Sharma D, Albery IP, Cook C. 2001. Selective attentional bias to alcohol related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers. Addiction. 96:285–295.
  • Shaw BR. 2006. Can new technologies prevent relapse? Behav Healthc. 26:30–31.
  • Shiffman S. 2009. Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) in studies of substance use. Psychol Assess. 21:486–497.
  • Siddiqi S, Sharan A. 2015. Keyword and keyphrase extraction techniques: a literature review. Int J Comp App. 109:18–23.
  • Siegel S. 1983. Classical conditioning, drug tolerance, and drug dependence. In: Research advances in alcohol and drug problems. New York: Springer; p. 207–246.
  • Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Batien BD, Wray TB. 2010. Event-level associations between affect, alcohol intoxication, and acute dependence symptoms: effects of urgency, self-control, and drinking experience. Addict Behav. 35:1045–1053.
  • Simons JS, Wills TA, Neal DJ. 2014. The many faces of affect: a multilevel model of drinking frequency/quantity and alcohol dependence symptoms among young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 123:676–694.
  • Skinner MD, Aubin HJ. 2010. Craving’s place in addiction theory: contributions of the major models. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 34:606–623.
  • Snelleman M, Schoenmakers TM, van de Mheen D. 2015. Attentional bias and approach/avoidance tendencies do not predict relapse or time to relapse in alcohol dependency. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 39:1734–1739.
  • Solomon RL, Corbit JD. 1974. An opponent-process theory of motivation. I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychol Rev. 81:119–145.
  • Spada MM, Wells A. 2009. A metacognitive model of problem drinking. Clin Psychol Psychother. 16:383–393.
  • Stewart J, de Wit H, Eikelboom R, Wit H, De, Eikelboom R. 1984. Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self-administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychol Rev. 91:251–268.
  • Tiffany ST. 1990. A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychol Rev. 97:147–168.
  • Tiffany ST. 1999. Cognitive concepts of craving. Alcohol Res Health. 23:215–224.
  • Townshend JM, Duka T. 2001. Attentional bias associated with alcohol cues: differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 157:67–74.
  • Townshend JM, Duka T. 2007. Avoidance of alcohol-related stimuli in alcohol-dependent inpatients. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res. 31:1349–1357.
  • Treloar H, Piasecki TM, McCarthy DM, Sher KJ, Heath AC. 2015. Ecological evidence that affect and perceptions of drink effects depend on alcohol expectancies. Addiction. 110:1432–1442.
  • van den Wildenberg E, Beckers M, van Lambaart F, Conrod PJ, Wiers RW. 2006. Is the strength of implicit alcohol associations correlated with alcohol-induced heart-rate acceleration? Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 30:1336–1348.
  • Verheul R, van den Brink W, Geerlings P. 1999. A three-pathway psychobiological model of craving for alcohol. Alcohol Alcohol. 34:197–222.
  • Waters H, Green MW. 2003. A demonstration of attentional bias, using a novel dual task paradigm, towards clinically salient material in recovering alcohol abuse patients? Psychol Med. 33:491–498.
  • Weafer J, Fillmore MT. 2012. Acute alcohol effects on attentional bias in heavy and moderate drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav. 27:32–41.
  • West R, Hardy A. 2006. Theory of addiction. Alcohol and alcoholism. Vol. 42 [Internet]. Oxford (UK): Blackwell Publishing. Available from: https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl107
  • Wikler A. 1948. Recent progress in research on the neurophysiologic basis of morphine addiction. Am J Psychiatry. 105:329–338.
  • Wilcockson TDW, Pothos EM. 2015. Measuring inhibitory processes for alcohol-related attentional biases: Introducing a novel attentional bias measure. Addictive Behaviors. 44:88–93.
  • Witkiewitz K, Bowen S. 2010. Depression, craving, and substance use following a randomized trial of mindfulness-based relapse prevention. J Consult Clin Psychol. 78:362–374.
  • Witkiewitz K, Bowen S, Donovan DM. 2011. Moderating effects of a craving intervention on the relation between negative mood and heavy drinking following treatment for alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 79:54–63.
  • Witkiewitz K, Villarroel NA. 2009. Dynamic association between negative affect and alcohol lapses following alcohol treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol. 77:633–644.
  • Woud ML, Becker ES, Rinck M, Salemink E. 2015. The relationship between drinking motives and alcohol-related interpretation biases. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 47:102–110.
  • Wray TB, Merrill JE, Monti PM. 2014. Using Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) to assess situation-level predictors of alcohol use and alcohol-related consequences. Alcohol Res. 36:19–27.