1,491
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

(Re)examining the effects of athlete brand image (ABI) on psychological commitment: an empirical investigation using structural equation modelling (SEM) and fuzzy set qualitative comparative analysis (fsQCA)

& ORCID Icon
Pages 244-264 | Received 05 Jan 2018, Accepted 30 Jul 2018, Published online: 15 Aug 2018
 

ABSTRACT

Research question: How does athlete brand image (ABI) affect psychological commitment (PC) when operationalised at the dimension- (attribute-) level, and measured using reflective indicators? Previous studies operationalise ABI at a higher-order construct level, and/or measure ABI using formative measures. Such operationalisations obscure potentially different relationships between ABI’s image attributes and PC.

Research methods: A questionnaire was used to collect data from 197 UK respondents over a six-day period within two weeks of the Rio 2016 Olympics concluding. Data were analysed through structural equation modelling (SEM) and fuzzy set Qualitative Comparative Analysis (fsQCA) techniques.

Results and findings: Through SEM, the ABI attributes, athletic expertise, life story, role model, and competition style are positively related to PC, sportsmanship and symbol are negatively related, and rivalry, physical attractiveness, body fit, and relationship effort are nonsignificantly related. Many structural paths between ABI’s attributes and PC are also significantly different. Through fsQCA, high PC exists under three complex ABI attribute configurations, while it is absent under four complex configurations.

Implications: Theoretically, finding different relationships between ABI’s attributes and PC highlights why operationalising ABI at the dimension-level provides a more in-depth understanding of athlete brand image’s effects on PC. Managerially, the findings suggest athletes need only a subset of ABI attributes for high PC. Subsequently, managers can more-efficiently and effectively direct resources towards those attributes that best-capitalise on athletes’ images.

Acknowledgement

The researchers would like to thank Professor Ian Hodgkinson (Loughborough University) for his constructive comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Notes

1. Arai and colleagues name the ten attributes, ‘subdimensions’, and the three higher order factors, ‘dimensions’.

2. Multi-value QCA (mvQCA) also exists. However, there is debate surrounding its set-theoretic status. csQCA and fsQCA are also regarded as the main QCA approaches (see e.g. Schneider & Wagemann, Citation2013).

3. Following the logic of Wagemann et al. (Citation2016) and Ragin (Citation2000) we would have removed truth table rows (i.e. before the final analysis stage) contradicting this sufficiency condition for high PC’s absence if they had existed empirically. However, whenever empirical cases of competition style’s absence occurred, empirical cases of high PC’s absence also occurred.

4. To demonstrate this we reanalysed our data using (a) binary logistic regression (bLR) to mimic conditions’ presence/absence, and (b) multinomial logistic regression (mLR) to – conceptually at least – mimic fsQCA’s gradation (with our ‘crossover point’ acting as the baseline category). The bLR analysis suggests significantly more variance is explained than when no predictors are included [Δχ2(10) = 41.234, p < .001; R2 = .189 (Cox & Snell), .264 (Nagelkerke)]. Subsequently, the individual parameter estimates suggest role model (b = .478, p = .018, Odds Ratio = 1.613) and life story (b = .385, p = .008, Odds Ratio = 1.469) significantly predict high PC. Meanwhile, including the ten ABI dimensions in the mLR model resulted in significantly more variance explained than in the baseline model [Δχ2(20) = 78.209, p < .001; R2 = .328 (Cox & Snell), .369 (Nagelkerke)], and the Pearson (p = .728) and Deviance (p = .754) statistics were nonsignificant. Subsequently, the individual parameter estimates suggest sportsmanship (b = .874, p = .014, Odds Ratio = 2.396) significantly predicts high PC, while role model (b = .441, p = .054, Odds Ratio = 1.555) is marginally significant. Conversely, expertise (b = −.684, p = .048, Odds Ratio = .505), sportsmanship (b = .926, p = .003, Odds Ratio = 2.524), relationship effort (b = −.658, p = .009, Odds Ratio = .518), and life story (b = −.641, p = .001, Odds Ratio = .527) are significant predictors of high PC’s absence.

5. We also reanalysed the data through bLR and mLR using only the ABI dimensions and the associated second-order interactions retained in the final fsQCA stage. For bLR, few significant predictors were found and some odds ratios appeared implausibly high. Similarly, few significant relationships were found in mLR and some odds ratios were questionable. This demonstrates the high demands placed on data when so many interactions are considered (Schneider & Wagemann, Citation2013). We do not report these results because we would only really know which ABI dimensions to include in the logistics regression models after fsQCA was undertaken (even with painstakingly exploring parsimony). In turn, this strengthens our view that fsQCA and SEM are complementary.

Log in via your institution

Log in to Taylor & Francis Online

PDF download + Online access

  • 48 hours access to article PDF & online version
  • Article PDF can be downloaded
  • Article PDF can be printed
USD 53.00 Add to cart

Issue Purchase

  • 30 days online access to complete issue
  • Article PDFs can be downloaded
  • Article PDFs can be printed
USD 389.00 Add to cart

* Local tax will be added as applicable

Related Research

People also read lists articles that other readers of this article have read.

Recommended articles lists articles that we recommend and is powered by our AI driven recommendation engine.

Cited by lists all citing articles based on Crossref citations.
Articles with the Crossref icon will open in a new tab.