3,481
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

The mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance: a meta-analysis

, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon &
Pages 370-397 | Received 15 Mar 2021, Accepted 02 Nov 2021, Published online: 23 Nov 2021

ABSTRACT

Prior self-control exertion is consistently reported to cause decrements in subsequent physical performance. However, research into the explanatory mechanisms underpinning the effect is limited and has not been assessed under a meta-analytical lens. Therefore, the present study reports a meta-analysis examining the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance, as well as the mechanisms underpinning the effect.

A systematic search of relevant databases was conducted to identify studies that utilized the sequential task paradigm, involving self-control manipulations lasting 30 minutes or less, and examined an aspect of physical performance. Random effects meta-analysis demonstrated that the prior exertion of self-control resulted in a statistically significant medium sized negative effect of prior self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance (g = −0.55). Further analysis revealed a small increase in initial perceptions of pain (g = 0.18) and a medium sized reduction in self-efficacy (g = −0.48), while motivation and RPE were unaffected following the exertion of self-control.

The present study provides a novel insight into the mechanisms underpinning the effects of prior self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. Initial perceptions of pain and self-efficacy appear important mechanisms and thus could be targeted in future interventions aimed at attenuating the effects of self-control exertion to enhance subsequent physical performance.

1.0. Introduction

Self-control is defined as the ability to override and manage dominant response tendencies to regulate one's emotions and behaviours (Vohs & Baumeister, Citation2004). Exhibiting high levels of self-control is beneficial for a large range of adaptive behaviours; including those related to achievement, task performance, interpersonal functioning, and health (de Ridder et al., Citation2012). Self-control has also been shown to be important in a magnitude of sport-settings, including athletic performance (Englert, Citation2016, Citation2017), whereby athletes are required to control their impulses and behavioural tendencies to optimize sporting performance. For example, athletes need to force themselves to work persistently during strenuous physical exercise despite the desire to reduce effort to relieve the discomfort associated with achieving optimal performance (Wagstaff, Citation2014). A further example of the importance of self-control in an exercise setting is where individuals must routinely exert their self-control to persevere at gym work-out routines to achieve personal physical fitness goals (Bandura, Citation2005; Gillebaart & Adriaanse, Citation2017). It is however important to distinguish between self-control and self-regulation; self-regulation is considered an umbrella term that captures automatic and nonconscious regulatory processes, whereas self-control has been categorized as a specific form of self-regulation in which an individual exerts deliberate and conscious effort to control the self (Baumeister et al., Citation2007).

The ability to exert self-control has been shown to differ between individuals (i.e. trait self-control; Tangney et al., Citation2004), as well as across situations within the same individual (i.e. state self-control; Gailliot et al., Citation2012). High levels of trait self-control have been associated with various favourable behaviours important for optimal athletic performance, including training adherence (Englert, Citation2017). Regarding the state perspective of self-control, contemporary meta-analytical research has supported the notion that the initial exertion of self-control on one task impairs performance on a subsequent, seemingly unrelated task, also requiring self-control (Dang, Citation2017; Hagger et al., Citation2010). Referred to as the ‘ego-depletion’ effect, this phenomenon has generated a substantial amount of debate within the literature. While such meta-analytical evidence provides support (Dang, Citation2017; Hagger et al., Citation2010), the existence and/or true size of the effect has been questioned with Registered Replication Reports and meta-analyses failing to demonstrate support for the ego-depletion effect (e.g. Carter et al., Citation2015; Dang, Citation2018; Hagger et al., Citation2016; Vohs et al., Citation2021). Furthermore, while some of the meta-analyses (e.g. Carter et al., Citation2015; Hagger et al., Citation2010) included studies with physical outcomes, they did not carry out a sub-group analysis that explored the size of the depletion effect on different types of physical tasks, which has recently been suggested to influence the size of the effect (Graham & Brown, Citation2021). In addition, the multi-lab replication studies (e.g. Dang et al., Citation2021; Hagger et al., Citation2016) did not involve a physical task as the outcome measure. As a result, domain-specific carryover effects on subsequent physical performance cannot be ruled out. Therefore, more domain specific research is necessary to understand the true effect that prior self-control exertion has on subsequent physical task performance, and how this may be different across different physical performance task types.

Despite the ongoing controversy surrounding the ego-depletion effect, several theoretical models have been established to explain why self-control failures are seen in a multitude of performance contexts, including sport and exercise settings. The first of these, the strength model of self-control, implies that individuals possess a limited central resource of self-control, which can become depleted following a period of self-control exertion (Baumeister et al., Citation2007). Although this ‘limited resource’ perspective has received empirical and meta-analytical support (e.g. Dang, Citation2017; Hagger et al., Citation2010), it has also been challenged by evidence demonstrating that performance decrements following prior self-control exertion are not observable when participants were adequately motivated, using techniques such as providing monetary incentives (Brown & Bray, Citation2017a; Muraven & Slessareva, Citation2003) and offering choice (Moller et al., Citation2006). Consequently, doubts have arisen that self-control failure can be attributed to a single universal resource that becomes depleted (Inzlicht & Friese, Citation2019).

An alternative explanation is the shifting priorities model (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, Citation2016; Milyavskaya & Inzlicht, Citation2017); which suggests that initial self-control exertion results in a shift in attentional and motivational foci, whereby the desire to exert additional self-control to achieve distal goals (i.e. optimal performance) is reduced, while the desire to concede to the tempting proximal goal (i.e. reducing discomfort) is increased (Taylor et al., Citation2020). These tenets are also consistent with the opportunity-cost conceptualizations of self-control, whereby individuals weigh the benefits of pursuing a specific task against its costs (Kurzban et al., Citation2013; Wolff & Martarelli, Citation2020).

To provide empirical support for the theoretical models of self-control researchers have typically implemented the ‘sequential-task paradigm’ to examine the effects of self-control exertion on a subsequent, assumedly unrelated task, also requiring self-control (Baumeister et al., Citation2007). Within this paradigm, the experimental (self-control exertion) condition requires participants to complete two tasks necessitating self-control. Conversely, the control (non-self-control exertion) condition requires participants to exert self-control only during the second performance task. The self-control tasks that are frequently employed require participants to resist impulses or temptations created by instinctive and well-learned responses (Arber et al., Citation2017; Baumeister et al., Citation2007). For example, self-control is often manipulated using a Stroop task (e.g. Boat & Taylor, Citation2017; Englert & Wolff, Citation2015). In the self-control exertion condition, participants complete an incongruent Stroop task where the aim of the task is to select the font colour (requiring well-learned responses to be overridden); whereas in the non-self-control exertion condition, participants instead complete a congruent Stroop task (requiring no overriding of well-learned responses). Both versions of the Stroop task involve a central stimulus word (always a colour) being presented to participants. Participants are required to select the font colour instead of the word itself. In the congruent Stroop task, the target word, and the font colour will be matched (e.g. ‘blue’ written in a blue font). In the incongruent Stroop task, the target word and font colour will be mis-matched (e.g. ‘blue’ written in red font), thus requiring the inhibition of well-learned dominant responses.

The detrimental effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical task performance have been substantiated during endurance cycling tasks (Englert & Wolff, Citation2015; Boat et al., Citation2017), skill-based tasks (e.g. Darts; McEwan et al., Citation2013; Basketball; Englert, Bertrams, et al., Citation2015) and simple physical persistence tasks (e.g. wall-sit task; Boat & Taylor, Citation2017; handgrip task; Bray et al., Citation2012; sit-up task; Dorris et al., Citation2012). This has been corroborated by recent meta-analytical evidence demonstrating that prior self-control exertion impairs subsequent physical performance (Brown et al., Citation2020; Giboin & Wolff, Citation2019). However, it is important to note that these previous meta-analyses have combined studies examining the effects of self-control exertion and mental fatigue on subsequent physical performance, despite suggestions that there are clear differences between these two constructs (Englert, Citation2016, Citation2019). For example, tasks that are utilized to induce mental fatigue typically last considerably longer (e.g. 90 minutes AX-continuous performance task; Marcora et al., Citation2009) than the tasks that are employed in self-control exertion research (e.g. 4-minute Stroop task; Boat et al., Citation2021). Therefore, it has been argued that typical self-control depletion tasks are not long enough to induce subjective feelings of effort and mental fatigue (Pageaux et al., Citation2013). However, other researchers have suggested that ego-depletion may be a brief manifestation of mental fatigue (Inzlicht & Berkman, Citation2015) and shorter, yet more effortful cognitive manipulations can promote equivalent levels of mental fatigue to the traditional longer manipulations (Brown & Bray, Citation2017a; Brown & Bray, Citation2019). Moreover, self-control exertion and mental fatigue both evidently lead to performance decrements, which may be a result of an unwillingness to employ further effort rather than incapacity (Englert, Citation2017; Hockey, Citation2013; Inzlicht & Schmeichel, Citation2012) and may be overcome with adequate task-motivation (Muraven & Slessareva, Citation2003). There may also be similarities in the mechanistic underpinnings of self-control and mental fatigue. For example, theories of self-control (e.g. Inzlicht & Schmeichel, Citation2016; Kurzban et al., Citation2013; Milyavskaya & Inzlicht, Citation2017) and the psychobiological model of fatigue (Marcora, Citation2008; Marcora et al., Citation2008; Marcora et al., Citation2009; Marcora & Staiano, Citation2010) highlight aspects of motivation as a key mechanism underpinning exercise tolerance. However, the mechanisms specific to self-control, or mental fatigue, have never been collated and analyzed under a meta-analytical lens.

Understanding the mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance is important to allow for a more complete understanding of how and why self-control exertion affects performance, and to allow the development of specific targeted interventions aimed at attenuating the effects. The mechanisms that have been proposed to date derive from the two key theories of self-control previously mentioned, yet evidence for these mechanisms is limited and discordant. For instance, support for the shifting priorities model has been demonstrated with suggestions that differences in initial perceptions of pain and motivation provide quantifiable shifts in motivational and attentional foci to explain self-control failures (e.g. Boat et al., Citation2020). Conversely, measurements of motivation, emotion, and attention did not mediate the relationship between self-control exertion and physical task performance (e.g. Stocker et al., Citation2020). Using a meta-analytical lens to provide a consensus regarding which proposed mechanisms of self-control failure appear to be associated with subsequent reductions in physical performance after completing a self-control exertion task lasting 30 minutes or less, would provide some clarity on this important issue.

Therefore, the aim of this present study is two-fold. Firstly, we will provide a comprehensive meta-analysis of the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. This will include an examination of key moderating variables such as study design and physical performance task type, to explore methodological factors that may influence the reported effects of self-control exertion on physical performance. Secondly, we will adopt a meta-analytical approach to examine the mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance.

2.0. Methods

The PRISMA guidelines on protocols and reporting in systematic reviews and meta-analyses were followed (Moher et al., Citation2009). A full overview of the checklist can be found with the additional material (see Electronic Supplementary Material: Table S1).

2.1. Eligibility criteria

Studies published prior to June 2021 (the month selected to conclude the systematic search) were considered for review. Studies had to be performed on healthy humans and written in English. Studies also had to utilize the sequential task paradigm in which participants engaged in two consecutive tasks (Baumeister et al., Citation2007). In the self-control exertion condition, there was the necessity that studies included a cognitive exertion task that has been shown to deplete self-control by requiring well-learned responses to be overridden (e.g. incongruent Stroop task, transcription task with instruction to omit letters). In the control condition, it was essential that self-control was not exerted in the first task, with tasks employed not requiring any overriding of well-learned responses (e.g. congruent Stroop task, transcription task with no additional instructions). For clarity, the common cognitive exertion tasks that were employed in the included studies were: (i) incongruent Stroop task; (ii) transcribing tasks; (iii) solving hard labyrinths; (iv) regulating emotions while watching an emotion-based video clip. In an attempt to focus solely on studies examining self-control exertion, the duration of the initial exertion task was required to be less than 30 minutes, as this duration is typically associated with self-control exertion studies (e.g. Boat et al., Citation2020; Englert & Wolff, Citation2015; Wagstaff, Citation2014), whereas cognitive manipulations exceeding 30 minutes in duration are suggested to elicit mental fatigue (e.g. Marcora et al., Citation2009). Therefore, in the present meta-analysis we focused only on self-control tasks 30 minutes or less in duration.

In accordance with the sequential-task paradigm, performance tasks had to require self-control and be objective measures of physical performance (e.g. handgrip task, cycling time trial). Outcome performance tasks were split into four categories: (i) isometric: outcomes included holding a posture or producing maximal force for as long as possible; (ii) aerobic: outcomes involved any type of endurance activity, namely covering a given distance in as short a time as possible, or covering as much distance or generating as much work as possible until volitional exhaustion; (iii) dynamic: outcomes involved completing as many repetitions of a particular movement, in a given time or until volitional exhaustion/failure; (iv) motor skill performance: outcomes involved measures such as number of false starts and reaction times/accuracy on skill-based tasks. Once studies met the above criteria, they were also assessed to see if they measured any potential mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance.

All relevant statistical information to calculate effect sizes was required for all studies. The Cochrane Handbook (Higgins et al., Citation2019) was used to follow protocols surrounding missing data (e.g. using data from previous studies to impute missing standard deviations; Furukawa et al., Citation2006). A full breakdown of methods used can be found in the Data Synthesis section. In addition, missing data from eligible studies was also requested by contacting the corresponding authors. If these protocols could not be implemented or missing data was not received from authors, studies were excluded.

2.2. Search strategy and study selection

A systematic review of the literature was carried out using Science Direct (n = 213), Web of Science (n = 749), PubMed (n = 119) and SPORTDiscuss (n = 576). To find relevant publications, searches were conducted with the following keywords search: ‘(self-control OR ego-depletion) AND (physical OR task OR activity OR endurance OR exercise OR skill OR exert)’. This search resulted in 1,682 publications which was reduced to 1,411 once duplicates were removed (search concluded June 2021). Publications were screened for eligibility from their title and abstract, resulting in 55 papers being selected for a full text review. An additional reverse citation search produced 5 additional papers to be included. The full text review led to an additional 16 papers being excluded due to not fully meeting the eligibility criteria. Forty-four articles were included in the meta-analysis, producing 50 comparisons for the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. Furthermore, these studies provided 61 comparisons for the exploration of the mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance (see for PRISMA flow diagram).

Figure 1. PRISMA study selection flowchart (Moher et al., Citation2009).

Figure 1. PRISMA study selection flowchart (Moher et al., Citation2009).

2.3. Data extraction

Data including sample characteristics (e.g. participant demographic and number) and study characteristics (e.g. depletion task used, study design) were collated, alongside means and standard deviations for measures of physical performance and the underpinning mechanisms. The mechanisms that provided a substantial amount of data (at least 3 effect sizes; Valentine et al., Citation2010) were pain (split by overall pain, pain at the start of the physical performance task, and pain at the end of the physical performance task), motivation, self-efficacy, and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE). For all variables, if the data were presented graphically, numerical data were attained using ImageJ (ImageJ 1.53v, NIH, Bethesda, MD, USA).

2.4. Risk of bias

The risk of bias for each publication was assessed by three reviewers independently, using the risk bias tool in Review Manager (RevMan 5.4; The Cochrane Collaboration, 2020) (https://tech.cochrane.org/revman) software. Across five domains (selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias and reporting bias) publications were labelled as ‘low risk’, ‘unclear risk’ or ‘high risk’. Where there was disagreement, a consensus was achieved through discussions (see for risk of bias assessment).

Figure 2. Summary of risk of bias assessment of the included studies. Key: ‘+’ low risk, ‘−’ high risk, ‘?’ unclear risk.

Figure 2. Summary of risk of bias assessment of the included studies. Key: ‘+’ low risk, ‘−’ high risk, ‘?’ unclear risk.

2.5. Data synthesis

Hedge's g effect sizes were calculated to summarize estimates of effect, calculated using standard procedures (M1M2 / SDpooled) (Higgins et al., Citation2019). For the handful of studies that used handgrip tasks as a performance outcome, Hedge's g was calculated using provided change scores (i.e. effect sizes were calculated using the difference of performance before and after the cognitive manipulation was administered) to enhance the precision of effect size estimation (Higgins et al., Citation2019). Two studies (Graham et al., Citation2017; McEwan et al., Citation2013) provided multiple effect sizes for one physical performance outcome; following the Cochrane Handbook recommendations (Higgins et al., Citation2019) these outcomes were combined using the RevMan 5.4 calculator to produce a single pair-wise comparison. Two studies (Boat et al., Citation2020; Brown & Bray, Citation2017b) provided multiple effect sizes over several time points. To avoid a unit-of-analysis error (i.e. double counting), the control group participant sample was shared across the pairwise comparisons (Higgins et al., Citation2019). Two studies (Brown & Bray, Citation2017b; Ciarocco et al., Citation2001) did not provide sufficient standard deviation data to calculate the necessary effect sizes; as per recommendations (Furukawa et al., Citation2006; Higgins et al., Citation2019) an average of standard deviations was taken from similar handgrip studies included in the meta-analysis (Alberts et al., Citation2007; Bray et al., Citation2008; Bray et al., Citation2013; Graham & Bray, Citation2012; Graham & Bray, Citation2015) to impute estimated standard deviations (SD self-control depletion condition = 17.49 s; SD control condition = 23.71 s). Five studies (Alberts et al., Citation2007; Brown & Bray, Citation2017a, Citation2019; Shaabani et al., Citation2020; Yusainy & Lawrence, Citation2015) included a secondary experimental manipulation (e.g. persistence priming vs. neutral priming; mindfulness intervention vs. no mindfulness intervention), in this instance only the data from the condition that did not involve the secondary manipulation were included (i.e. high self-control exertion & neutral condition compared to low self-control exertion & neutral condition). Similar protocols were followed for the investigation into the mechanisms. To calculate one single effect size for the ‘overall pain’ subgroup, the start of task pain and end of task pain effect sizes provided in four studies were combined (Boat & Taylor, Citation2017; Boat et al., Citation2018, Citation2020, Citation2021). These effect sizes were also analyzed independently. Seven studies provided two or more effect sizes for measures of motivation (Boat et al., Citation2018, Citation2020, Citation2021; Brown & Bray, Citation2017b; Brown & Bray, Citation2019; Graham & Bray, Citation2015; Stocker et al., Citation2020). Similarly, these were combined to align with recommendations (Higgins et al., Citation2019).

2.6. Meta-analysis

All analyses were conducted using RevMan 5.4. Due to the varied methods of data collection, a random effects model was used. Firstly, a comprehensive meta-analysis of the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance was performed, including an examination of key moderating variables such as study design and physical performance task type. Heterogeneity was explored using the Cochrane Q (x2) test and summarized with the I2 statistic. Sensitivity analysis was conducted by removing one study at a time to analyse its influence on the overall effect size (see Electronic Supplementary Material: Table S2). Subsequently, a separate meta-analysis was conducted to examine the mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance.

3.0. Results

3.1. Included studies

Forty-four articles provided a total of 50 comparisons with 2315 participants (not adjusted for within-subject designs) (see ). Study characteristics and outcomes are outlined in .

Figure 3. Forest plot of the studies examining the effects of prior self-control exertion on physical task performance.

Figure 3. Forest plot of the studies examining the effects of prior self-control exertion on physical task performance.

Table 1. Study characteristics and outcomes.

3.2. Risk of bias

A summary of the risk of bias for each included study is presented in . All studies were rated as low risk for selective reporting as all relevant information was considered present. Twenty-three out of 44 were rated high-risk in at least one domain. A rating of high risk or unclear risk was commonly a result of a lack of allocation concealment (selection bias) and blinding protocols (performance and detection bias) being employed. In addition, studies often reported incomplete outcome data (attrition bias).

3.3. Meta-analyses

3.3.1. Overall effect

Results showed that 44 of the 50 comparisons resulted in a negative effect of self-control exertion on physical task performance (see ). Overall, a significant medium negative effect of prior self-control exertion on physical task performance was found (g = −0.55 [−0.70, −0.39], Z = 7.01, p < 0.001). Heterogeneity analysis demonstrated significant heterogeneity for the overall effect (Q(49) = 145.40, p < 0.001, T2 = 0.19, I2 = 66), therefore, the decision to conduct subgroup analyses examining the factors that could impact the effect was justified (see ).

Results of the sensitivity analyses (see Electronic Supplementary Material: Table S2) revealed a stable significant effect size ranging from g= −0.53 [−0.67, −0.40] (when excluding the study by Boat & Taylor, Citation2017) to g = −0.59 [−0.74, −0.44] (when excluding the study by Xu et al., Citation2014).

3.3.2. Study design

Studies that implemented a within-subject design demonstrated a similar significant medium negative effect of self-control exertion on physical task performance (g = −0.53 [−0.87, −0.20], Z = 3.13, p = 0.002), when compared to studies that implemented a between-subject design (g = −0.54 [−0.68, −0.40], Z = 7.40, p < 0.001) (see a).

Figure 4. Forest plots displaying the results of the subgroup analysis of the effects of prior self-control exertion on physical performance (a: Study design; b: Performance task type).

Figure 4. Forest plots displaying the results of the subgroup analysis of the effects of prior self-control exertion on physical performance (Figure 4a: Study design; Figure 4b: Performance task type).

There was significant heterogeneity in terms of study design for publications that employed a within-subjects design (Q(17) = 95.50, p < 0.001, T2 = 0.42, I2 = 82), and between-subjects design (Q(31) = 48.85, p = 0.02, T2 = 0.06, I2 = 37).

3.3.3. Type of physical performance task

The subgroup analysis demonstrated a significant negative effect of self-control exertion on physical task performance for all physical task types. The largest negative effect was found for isometric physical tasks (g = −0.62 [−0.84, −0.39], Z = 5.32, p < 0.001). A large negative effect size was also found for dynamic physical tasks (g = −0.61 [−1.09, −0.12], Z = 2.44, p = 0.01), while smaller negative effect sizes were found for studies that implemented aerobic (g = −0.36 [−0.58, −0.14], Z = 3.19, p = 0.001) and motor skill (g = −0.45 [−0.71, −0.20], Z = 3.47, p < 0.001) tasks (see b).

There was significant heterogeneity in terms of type of physical performance task for studies that employed an isometric physical performance task (Q(30) = 120.33, p < 0.001, T2 = 0.29, I2 = 75). Heterogeneity was not observed in studies that employed aerobic (Q(7) = 4.92, p = 0.67, T2 = 0.00, I2 = 0), motor skill (Q(7) = 9.50, p = 0.22, T2 = 0.04, I2 = 26) or dynamic (Q(2) = 4.22, p = 0.12, T2 = 0.10, I2 = 53) physical performance tasks.

3.4. Mechanisms analyses

3.4.1. Pain

There was no statistically significant effect of self-control exertion on participants’ overall perceptions of pain (g = 0.08 [−0.08, 0.24], Z = 1.01, p = 0.31) (see a). However, there was a statistically significant small effect of self-control exertion on participants’ initial perceptions of pain (g = 0.18 [0.02, 0.34], Z = 2.18, p = 0.03), whereby initial perceptions of pain tended to be higher following self-control exertion (see b). There was no statistically significant effect of self-control exertion on participants’ perceptions of pain at the end of the physical task (g = −0.03 [−0.19, 0.13], Z = 0.31, p = 0.75) (see c). Heterogeneity was not observed for any of the pain subgroups (overall pain, p = 1.00; initial pain, p = 0.56; end pain, p = 0.45).

Figure 5. Forest plots examining the mechanisms for the effects of prior self-control exertion on physical performance (a: Overall pain; b: Start pain; c: End pain; d: Motivation; e: Self-efficacy; f: RPE).

3.4.2. Motivation

There was no statistically significant effect of self-control exertion on participants’ motivation (g = −0.03 [−0.36, 0.29], Z = 0.20, p = 0.84). Significant heterogeneity was observed for the effects on motivation (Q(15) = 56.46, p < 0.001, T2 = 0.30, I2 = 73).

3.4.3. Self-efficacy

There was a statistically significant medium negative effect of self-control exertion on self-efficacy (g = −0.48 [−0.86, −0.10], Z = 2.47, p = 0.01), whereby participants displayed lower levels of self-efficacy following self-control exertion, compared to the control group/condition (see e). Significant heterogeneity was observed for the effects on self-efficacy (Q(8) = 15.64, p = 0.05, T2 = 0.16, I2 = 49).

3.4.4. RPE

There was no statistically significant effect of self-control exertion on participants’ RPE (g = 0.03 [−0.25, 0.32], Z = 0.21, p = 0.83) (see f). Significant heterogeneity was observed for the effects on RPE (Q(11) = 19.59, p = 0.05, T2 = 0.10, I2 = 44).

4.0. Discussion

The findings of the present study suggest that the prior exertion of self-control resulted in a statistically significant medium sized negative effect on subsequent physical task performance (g = −0.55). Subgroup analyses revealed a statistically significant medium sized negative effect for studies employing both a within-subject design (g = −0.53) and a between-subject design (g = −0.54). Furthermore, the type of physical performance task also influenced the results, with prior self-control exertion demonstrating a medium-to-large sized negative effect on isometric (g = −0.62) and dynamic (g = −0.61) based physical tasks, while a small-to-medium sized negative effect was found for studies that utilized aerobic (g = −0.36) and motor skill (g = −0.45) tasks. In addition, the present study is the first to meta-analytically examine the mechanisms underpinning the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. The findings demonstrated that the prior exertion of self-control had a medium-sized negative effect on self-efficacy (g = −0.48), and a small effect on initial perceptions of pain during the subsequent physical task (g = 0.18); while there was no statistically significant effect of prior self-control exertion on perceptions of pain overall (g = 0.08), pain in the latter stages of the physical performance task (g = −0.03), motivation (g = −0.03) or RPE (g = 0.03). The findings provide novel evidence for self-efficacy and initial perceptions of pain to be recognized as the mechanisms by which prior self-control exertion affects subsequent physical performance.

An important finding of the present study is that prior self-control exertion had a statistically significant medium sized negative effect on subsequent physical performance (g = −0.55). This finding extends a recent meta-analysis which combined studies examining both prior self-control exertion and mental fatigue studies, which yielded an effect size of g = −0.38 (Brown et al., Citation2020). Furthermore, when considering studies within initial cognitive task with a duration of less than 30 minutes, Brown et al. (Citation2020) reported an effect size of g = −0.45. The differences between the effect sizes could be attributed to the inclusion of unpublished studies in the meta-analysis of Brown et al. (Citation2020), therefore, greater influence of the ‘file drawer effect’, in which null effect sizes may reduce the overall effect size.

Another key finding of the present study was that studies that employed a within-subject design produced a similar medium negative effect size (g = −0.53) to studies employing a between-subject design (g = −0.54). Previous meta-analytical evidence has displayed larger effect sizes for studies that have employed a between-subjects design (Brown et al., Citation2020). Subsequently, the inclusion of more recent studies in the present meta-analysis, most of which employ a within-subjects design, could explain the discrepancy with previous findings.

The findings of the present study also suggest that the type of physical task used is another important factor to consider when interpreting studies examining the effects of prior self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. Specifically, the present meta-analysis demonstrated a medium-to-large negative effect for isometric (g = −0.62) and dynamic (g = −0.61) physical tasks, while small-to-medium negative effect sizes were found for aerobic (g = −0.36) and motor skill (g = −0.45) physical tasks. However, the discrepancies between physical task type subgroups may be confounded by study design. For example, 20 out of 31 studies that employed an isometric task utilized between-subject study designs, which typically yield larger effect sizes (e.g. Alberts et al., Citation2007; Brown & Bray, Citation2017a). Furthermore, some effect sizes in the present meta-analysis are derived from a low number of studies (e.g. dynamic subgroup n = 3). Nonetheless, these findings support the notion that prior cognitive exertion has a greater detrimental effect on subsequent physical performance in isolated tasks (e.g. wall-sit), compared to whole-body endurance tasks (e.g. cycling) (Giboin & Wolff, Citation2019). The varying physiological and psychological demands of different performance tasks could explain the differences in effects seen in the present study. For instance, isometric performance tasks such as a wall-sit may demand greater levels of attentional control for optimal performance compared to whole-body endurance tasks, such as cycling, that rely on more automatic motor processes (Dimitrijevic et al., Citation1998; Giboin & Wolff, Citation2019). Therefore, future research should continue to investigate the effects of self-control exertion on differing physical performance tasks to advance this debate; and consider examining sport-specific performance tasks with ecological validity for real-world sporting performance.

A key novel aspect of the present study is the meta-analytical investigation of the mechanisms that underpin the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. The largest effect size was found for self-efficacy, with a statistically significant medium-sized negative effect (g= −0.48). As a result of initial self-control exertion, individuals may have reduced belief that they possess the capabilities to mobilize the resources required to exert further self-control, which would be required to achieve optimal performance on a subsequent physical task (Bandura, Citation1977; Graham & Bray, Citation2015). In accordance with the opportunity-cost conceptualization of self-control (Kurzban et al., Citation2013; Wolff & Martarelli, Citation2020), following prior self-control exertion individuals may be less motivated to exert further self-control if they do not feel confident that they can persevere at the task and if they do not see any additional benefit in investing further self-control and effort. This will result in the cons of persisting at the task (i.e. feelings of pain and discomfort) outweighing the benefits (i.e. optimal performance), and could lead to reduced physical performance and/or the termination of effort (Inzlicht et al., Citation2018; Kool & Botvinick, Citation2014; Kurzban et al., Citation2013). Acknowledging self-efficacy as a key mechanism for physical task performance could have valuable implications for sport and exercise practitioners. Specifically, athletes and those in their support network should be aware of the impact that prior self-control exertion can have on the athlete's self-efficacy for a subsequent physical task. Moreover, researchers should develop specific interventions that aim to increase self-efficacy to combat the negative effects of prior self-control exertion on subsequent performance.

Prior self-control exertion was also discovered to have a small-sized negative effect on individuals’ initial perception of pain during a physical performance task (g= 0.18). However, there was no effect on overall pain (g = 0.08) or pain towards the end of a physical performance task (g = −0.03). These findings are in accordance with previous research suggesting that the prior exertion of self-control results in elevated perceptions of pain, but only during the early stages of a physical task (e.g. Boat & Taylor, Citation2017; Boat et al., Citation2020). Theoretically, the importance of perceptions of pain can be explained by the shifting priorities model (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, Citation2016; Milyavskaya & Inzlicht, Citation2017) and opportunity-cost conceptualization (Kurzban et al., Citation2013; Wolff & Martarelli, Citation2020) of self-control. Specifically, it is suggested that the prior exertion of self-control causes increased perceptions of pain during the early stages of a subsequent physical performance task, causing an individuals’ attention during the task to shift to the proximal goal (e.g. ceasing exercise to alleviate pain) and away from the distal goal (e.g. optimal physical performance); ultimately causing a reduction in subsequent physical performance. The present study has investigated this using a meta-analytical approach for the first time.

In the present study, there was no effect of prior self-control exertion on individual's motivation (g = −0.03). This finding is in accordance with previous research showing motivation did not change in response to cognitive exertion (e.g. Brown & Bray, Citation2017b, Citation2019; Graham & Bray, Citation2015). The current findings present challenges for the motivational aspect of the shifting priorities model (Inzlicht & Schmeichel, Citation2016; Milyavskaya & Inzlicht, Citation2017), as this meta-analysis suggests that there is no effect of prior self-control exertion on subsequent motivation during the subsequent physical task. Interestingly, changes in other more nuanced aspects of motivation, such as goal commitment and exercise intentions, have been suggested to decrease following prior self-control exertion (Brown & Bray, Citation2019). However, the evidence base is very limited and thus requires further investigation. Such research is necessary to examine multiple aspects of motivation to provide a more detailed explanation of an individual's motivational intentions, than the more commonly used broad measures of task and intrinsic motivation. In addition, it has been suggested that future research should aim to investigate more complex motivational processes through qualitative methods (i.e. think aloud) to record the reasons behind changes to individuals’ intentions or commitment (Brown & Bray, Citation2019; Marcora, Citation2008; Marcora et al., Citation2008, Citation2009; Marcora & Staiano, Citation2010). Moreover, an explanation for the findings of the present study could be a result of motivation being measured at varied time-points in the included studies. Some research has measured motivation at pre-selected intervals throughout the physical task (e.g. Boat et al., Citation2018), while other studies have acquired a singular measurement prior to the physical task (e.g. Graham et al., Citation2018). These inconsistencies could explain the discrepant findings of previous research, and future studies should continue to examine the time course of the changes in multiple aspects of motivation as a result of the prior exertion of self-control.

The findings of this meta-analysis provide very limited evidence for RPE to be considered a mechanism that could underpin the effects of prior self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance (g= 0.03). RPE, however, has been considered as the main explanatory mechanism underpinning the effects of mental fatigue on subsequent physical performance (Pageaux & Lepers, Citation2018; Van Cutsem et al., Citation2017). Taken together, these findings allude to there being key differences between the constructs of self-control exertion and mental fatigue. Our findings support the notion that typical self-control depletion tasks are not long enough to induce subjective feelings of effort (Pageaux, Marcora, & Lepers, Citation2013), and thus self-control depletion tasks may not invoke the same mechanistic responses that underpin the effects of mental fatigue on subsequent physical performance. Therefore, caution should be taken when combining both research streams for future investigation as results may not be attributed to the same mechanisms. Furthermore, RPE findings are difficult to interpret as both self-control exertion and mental fatigue result in individuals reducing the absolute intensity that they are exercising at. Thus, RPE measurements may be comparable to participants in a non-depleted/non-fatigued state, while the absolute exercise intensity would be different, resulting in differences in performance but no differences in RPE (Pageaux, Citation2014; Van Cutsem et al., Citation2017). Further research is required to fully uncouple the relationship between RPE following cognitive exertion and physical performance and how this may be different for studies that induce mental fatigue and those that require self-control exertion.

4.1. Limitations and future direction

Although yielding novel findings surrounding the effects of self-control exertion on physical task performance, some limitations must be addressed. It must be acknowledged that the findings of this study only relied on published literature and no research teams were contacted regarding unpublished papers. We decided to base our meta-analysis on published literature only, that was accessible to the scientific community, and that we were confident had been through the peer-review process. However, we acknowledge that the omission of unpublished work may skew the present effect sizes and conceal the impact of the ‘file-drawer’ effect, with the omission of studies that reported null effects. Moreover, the risk of bias assessment could not identify any study included in the meta-analysis as completely low risk. Factors associated with higher risks were namely associated with detection bias and attrition bias. For example, several studies did not provide information surrounding the blinding of researchers, and thus future studies should encourage and explicitly state the use of double-blind techniques. In addition, researchers must openly report the reasons and handling of incomplete data outcomes to safeguard the internal validity of studies.

Furthermore, while the present study has provided the first meta-analysis on the mechanisms that are affected by prior self-control exertion, it must be noted that some findings were interpreted from a low number of effect sizes due to the limited evidence base. The significance level of an effect size can be influenced considerably by the inclusion of an additional publication when dealing with effect sizes calculated from a relatively small number of comparisons. Therefore, further research into the mechanisms identified in this meta-analysis is required to create an extensive and stronger evidence base. Finally, future mediational research is required to investigate the relationship of the ‘causal chain’ between self-control exertion, the mechanisms identified in this meta-analysis, and subsequent physical performance. This will develop our understanding of how these mechanisms are impacted by prior self-control exertion, and as a result how they impact subsequent physical performance. Building upon the findings of this meta-analysis, future research should aim to create interventions that target the suggested mechanisms, to identify strategies to attenuate the effects of self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. For example, researchers could develop strategies to alter perceptions of pain in the initial stages of a physical task. Such strategies could reduce the initial perception of pain, resulting in individuals being able to continue exerting the self-control required to achieve optimal performance.

Moreover, some additional mechanisms could not be included in the meta-analysis due to a lack of empirical evidence. For example, state anxiety could not be included as a potential mechanism because the studies that have examined this have included an additional direct manipulation of state-anxiety and thus have not solely measured the effects on self-control exertion on state anxiety in isolation (Englert & Bertrams, Citation2012; Englert, Zwemmer, Bertrams, & Oudejans, Citation2015). Similarly, motivational incentives (Brown & Bray, Citation2017a), biofeedback (Brown & Bray, Citation2019) and autonomy supportive instructions (Graham et al., Citation2014) have been employed to attenuate the depletion effect, and should be further investigated to provide valuable insight into the potential role of specific aspects of motivation. Furthermore, more recently, it has been hypothesized that feelings of boredom may be provoked once self-control has been exerted (Wolff & Martarelli, Citation2020); yet no empirical studies have investigated this to date. Therefore, task-induced boredom should be examined as a psychological factor that may explain performance reductions on physical tasks following self-control exertion.

5.0. Conclusion

Results from the current meta-analysis showed that 50 comparisons (and over 2200 participants) resulted in a medium negative effect (g = −0.55) of prior self-control exertion on subsequent task performance. Explanatory mechanisms that underpin the effect were also established, whereby self-efficacy was lower and initial perceptions of pain were higher, following the prior exertion of self-control. Future research should continue to mechanistically investigate the effects of prior self-control exertion on subsequent physical performance. Ultimately, this knowledge can be used to design and implement interventions aimed at attenuating the effects of self-control exertion, to enhance physical task performance.

Supplemental material

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (29.4 KB)

Supplemental Material

Download MS Word (15.8 KB)

Disclosure statement

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

References

  • Alberts, H. J., Martijn, C., Greb, J., Merckelbach, H., & de Vries, N. K. (2007). Carrying on or giving in: The role of automatic processes in overcoming ego depletion. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46(2), 383–399. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466606X130111
  • Arber, M. M., Ireland, M. J., Feger, R., Marrington, J., Tehan, J., & Tehan, G. (2017). Ego depletion in real-time: An examination of the sequential-task paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1672. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01672
  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
  • Bandura, A. (2005). The primacy of self-regulation in health promotion. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 54(2), 245–254. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00208.x
  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351–355. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
  • Boat, R., Atkins, T., Davenport, N., & Cooper, S. (2018). Prior self-control exertion and perceptions of pain and motivation during a physically effortful task. In M. Sarkar & S. Marcora (Eds.), Sport and the brain: The science of preparing, enduring, and winning, part C (pp. 19–34). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.08.007
  • Boat, R., Hunte, R., Welsh, E., Dunn, A., Treadwell, E., & Cooper, S. B. (2020). Manipulation of the duration of the initial self-control task within the sequential-task paradigm: Effect on exercise performance. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 14, 1093. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2020.571312
  • Boat, R., & Taylor, I. M. (2017). Prior self-control exertion and perceptions of pain during a physically demanding task. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 33, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.07.005
  • Boat, R., Taylor, I. M., & Hulston, C. J. (2017). Self-control exertion and glucose supplementation prior to endurance performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 29, 103–110. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.12.007
  • Boat, R., Williamson, O., Read, J., Jeong, Y. H., & Cooper, S. B. (2021). Self-control exertion and caffeine mouth rinsing: Effects on cycling time-trial performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 53, 101877. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2020.101877
  • Bray, S. R., Ginis, K. A. M., & Woodgate, J. (2011). Self-regulatory strength depletion and muscle-endurance performance: A test of the limited-strength model in older adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 19(3), 177–188. https://doi.org/10.1123/japa.19.3.177
  • Bray, S. R., Graham, J. D., Martin Ginis, K. A., & Hicks, A. L. (2012). Cognitive task performance causes impaired maximum force production in human hand flexor muscles. Biological Psychology, 89(1), 195–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.008
  • Bray, S. R., Martin Ginis, K. A., Hicks, A. L., & Woodgate, J. (2008). Effects of self-regulatory strength depletion on muscular performance and EMG activation. Psychophysiology, 45(2), 337–343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00625.x
  • Bray, S. R., Oliver, J. P., Graham, J. D., & Martin Ginis, K. A. (2013). Music, emotion, and self-control: Does listening to uplifting music replenish self-control strength for exercise? Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 18(3), 156–173. https://doi.org/10.1111/jabr.12008
  • Brown, D. M., & Bray, S. R. (2017a). Effects of mental fatigue on physical endurance performance and muscle activation are attenuated by monetary incentives. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 39(6), 385–396. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2017-0187
  • Brown, D. M., & Bray, S. R. (2017b). Graded increases in cognitive control exertion reveal a threshold effect on subsequent physical performance. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 6(4), 355–369. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000091
  • Brown, D. M., & Bray, S. R. (2019). Heart rate biofeedback attenuates effects of mental fatigue on exercise performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 41, 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.12.001
  • Brown, D. M., Graham, J. D., Innes, K. I., Harris, S., Flemington, A., & Bray, S. R. (2020). Effects of prior cognitive exertion on physical performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Medicine, 50(3), 497–529. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-019-01204-8
  • Carter, E. C., Kofler, L. M., Forster, D. E., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(4), 796–815. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000083
  • Ciarocco, N. J., Sommer, K. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Ostracism and ego depletion: The strains of silence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(9), 1156–1163. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201279008
  • Dang, J. (2017). An updated meta-analysis of the ego-depletion effect. Advancing Psychology Research, 82, 645–651.
  • Dang, J. (2018). An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Research, 82(4), 645–651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-017-0862-x
  • Dang, J., Barker, P., Baumert, A., Bentvelzen, M., Berkman, E., Buchholz, N., Buczny, J., Chen, Z., De Cristofaro, V., de Vries, L., Dewitte, S., Giacomantonio, M., Gong, R., Homan, M., Imhoff, R., Ismail, I., Jia, L., Kubiak, T., Lange, F., … Zinkernagel, A. (2021). A multilab replication of the ego depletion effect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(1), 14–24. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619887702
  • de Ridder, D. T., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 76–99. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749
  • Dimitrijevic, M. R., Gerasimenko, Y., & Pinter, M. M. (1998). Evidence for a spinal central pattern generator in humansa. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 860(1), 360–376. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09062.x
  • Dorris, D. C., Power, D. A., & Kenefick, E. (2012). Investigating the effects of ego depletion on physical exercise routines of athletes. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13(2), 118–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.10.004
  • Englert, C. (2016). The strength model of self-control in sport and exercise psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 314. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00314
  • Englert, C. (2017). Ego depletion in sports: Highlighting the importance of self-control strength for high-level sport performance. Current Opinion in Psychology, 16, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.028
  • Englert, C. (2019). The self-regulation of human performance: A critical discussion and future directions for self-control research. Performance Enhancement & Health, 6(3-4), 156–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peh.2019.04.001
  • Englert, C., & Bertrams, A. (2012). Anxiety, ego depletion, and sports performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 34(5), 580–599. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.34.5.580
  • Englert, C., & Bertrams, A. (2014). The effect of ego depletion on sprint start reaction time. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 36(5), 506–515. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2014-0029
  • Englert, C., Bertrams, A., Furley, P., & Oudejans, R. R. (2015). Is ego depletion associated with increased distractibility? Results from a basketball free throw task. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 18, 26–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.12.001
  • Englert, C., Persaud, B., Oudejans, R., & Bertrams, A. (2015). The influence of ego depletion on sprint start performance in athletes without track and field experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1207. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01207
  • Englert, C., & Wolff, W. (2015). Ego depletion and persistent performance in a cycling task. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 46(2), 137–151.
  • Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., Dalton, A. N., Scarbeck, S. J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2006). High-maintenance interaction: Inefficient social coordination impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 456–475. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.456
  • Furukawa, T. A., Barbui, C., Cipriani, A., Brambilla, P., & Watanabe, N. (2006). Imputing missing standard deviations in meta-analyses can provide accurate results. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59(1), 7–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2005.06.006
  • Gailliot, M. T., Gitter, S. A., Baker, M. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2012). Breaking the rules: Low trait or state self-control increases social norm violations. Psychology, 03(12), 1074–1083. https://doi.org/10.4236/psych.2012.312159
  • Giboin, L., & Wolff, W. (2019). The effect of ego depletion or mental fatigue on subsequent physical endurance performance: A meta-analysis. Performance Enhancement & Health, 7(1-2), 100150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peh.2019.100150
  • Gillebaart, M., & Adriaanse, M. A. (2017). Self-control predicts exercise behavior by force of habit, a conceptual replication of Adriaanse et al. (2014). Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 190. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00190
  • Graham, J. D., & Bray, S. R. (2012). Imagery and endurance: Does imagery impair performance by depleting self-control strength. Journal of Imagery Research in Sport and Physical Activity, 7(1).
  • Graham, J. D., & Bray, S. R. (2015). Self-control strength depletion reduces self-efficacy and impairs exercise performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 37(5), 477–488. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2015-0064
  • Graham, J. D., & Brown, D. M. (2021). Understanding and interpreting the effects of prior cognitive exertion on self-regulation of sport and exercise performance. In C. Englert & I. Taylor (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation and motivation in sport and exercise. Routledge, 1–21.
  • Graham, J. D., Li, Y., Bray, S. R., & Cairney, J. (2018). Effects of cognitive control exertion and motor coordination on task self-efficacy and muscular endurance performance in children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 379. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00379
  • Graham, J. D., Martin Ginis, K. A., & Bray, S. R. (2017). Exertion of self-control increases fatigue, reduces task self-efficacy, and impairs performance of resistance exercise. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 6(1), 70–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000074
  • Graham, J. D., Sonne, M. W., & Bray, S. R. (2014). It wears me out just imagining it! Mental imagery leads to muscle fatigue and diminished performance of isometric exercise. Biological Psychology, 103, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.018
  • Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., Brand, R., Brandt, M. J., Brewer, G., Bruyneel, S., Calvillo, D. P., Campbell, W. K., Cannon, P. R., Carlucci, M., Carruth, N. P., Cheung, T., Crowell, A., De Ridder, D. T. D., Dewitte, S., … Zwienenberg, M. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546–573. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616652873
  • Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(4), 495–525. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019486
  • Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., & Welch, V. A. (Eds.). (2019). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 6.0 (updated July 2019). Cochrane. Retrieved June 2020, from https://www.training.cochrane.org/handbook
  • Hockey, R. (2013). The psychology of fatigue: Work, effort and control. Cambridge University Press.
  • Inzlicht, M, & Berkman, E. (2015). Six questions for the resource model of control (and some answers). Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 511–524.
  • Inzlicht, M., & Friese, M. (2019). The past, present, and future of ego depletion. Social Psychology, 50(5-6), 370–378. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000398
  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2012). What is ego depletion? Toward a mechanistic revision of the resource model of self-control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(5), 450–463. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454134
  • Inzlicht, M., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2016). Beyond limited resources: Self-control failure as the product of shifting priorities. In K. D. Vohs & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed.). The Guilford Press, 1–32.
  • Inzlicht, M., Shenhav, A., & Olivola, C. Y. (2018). The effort paradox: Effort is both costly and valued. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(4), 337–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.007
  • Kool, W., & Botvinick, M. (2014). A labor/leisure trade-off in cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 131–141. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031048
  • Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 661–679. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12003196
  • Marcora, S. M. (2008). Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance? European Journal of Applied Physiology, 104(5), 929–931. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-008-0818-3
  • Marcora, S. M., Bosio, A., & de Morree, H. (2008). Locomotor muscle fatigue increases cardiorespiratory responses and reduces performance during intense cycling exercise independently from metabolic stress. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 294(3), R874–R883. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00678.2007
  • Marcora, S. M., & Staiano, W. (2010). The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: Mind over muscle? European Journal of Applied Physiology, 109(4), 763–770. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-010-1418-6
  • Marcora, S. M., Staiano, W., & Manning, V. (2009). Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Journal of Applied Physiology, 106(3), 857–864. https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.91324.2008
  • Martijn, C., Alberts, H. J., Merckelbach, H., Havermans, R., Huijts, A., & de Vries, N. K. (2007). Overcoming ego depletion: The influence of exemplar priming on self-control performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(2), 231–238. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.350
  • Martijn, C., Tenbult, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E., & de Vries, N. K. (2002). Getting a grip on ourselves: Challenging expectancies about loss of energy after self-control. Social Cognition, 20(6), 441–460. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.20.6.441.22978
  • Martin Ginis, K. A., & Bray, S. R. (2010). Application of the limited strength model of self-regulation to understanding exercise effort, planning and adherence. Psychology and Health, 25(10), 1147–1160. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440903111696
  • McEwan, D., Ginis, K. A. M., & Bray, S. R. (2013). The effects of depleted self-control strength on skill-based task performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 35(3), 239–249. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.35.3.239
  • Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). Attentional and motivational mechanisms of self-control. In D. de Ridder, M. Adriaanse, & K. Fujita (Eds.), Handbook of self-control in health & well-being. Routledge, 11–23.
  • Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & Prisma Group. (2009). Reprint—preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Physical Therapy, 89(9), 873–880. https://doi.org/10.1093/ptj/89.9.873
  • Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(8), 1024–1036. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167206288008
  • Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self-control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 894–906. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167203029007008
  • Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 774–789. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774
  • Murtagh, A. M., & Todd, S. A. (2004). Self-regulation: A challenge to the strength model. Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis, 3(1), 19–51.
  • O’Brien, J., Parker, J., Moore, L., & Fryer, S. (2020). Cardiovascular and cerebral hemodynamic responses to ego depletion in a pressurized sporting task. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 9(2), 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000199
  • Pageaux, B. (2014). The psychobiological model of endurance performance: an effort-based decision-making theory to explain self-paced endurance performance. Sports Medicine, 44, 1319–1320.
  • Pageaux, B., & Lepers, R. (2018). The effects of mental fatigue on sport-related performance. Progress in Brain Research, 240, 291–315. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.004
  • Pageaux, B., Marcora, S., & Lepers, R. (2013). Prolonged mental exertion does not alter neuromuscular function of the knee extensors. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 45(12), 2254–2264. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31829b504a
  • Schücker, L., & MacMahon, C. (2016). Working on a cognitive task does not influence performance in a physical fitness test. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 25, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.03.002
  • Shaabani, F., Naderi, A., Borella, E., & Calmeiro, L. (2020). Does a brief mindfulness intervention counteract the detrimental effects of ego depletion in basketball free throw under pressure? Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 9(2), 197–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000201
  • Stocker, E., Seiler, R., Schmid, J., & Englert, C. (2020). Hold your strength! Motivation, attention, and emotion as potential psychological mediators between cognitive and physical self-control. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 9(2), 167–182. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000173
  • Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality, 72(2), 271–324. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
  • Taylor, I. M., Boat, R., & Murphy, S. L. (2020). Integrating theories of self-control and motivation to advance endurance performance. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1080/1750984X.2018.1480050
  • Tyler, J. M., & Burns, K. C. (2008). After depletion: The replenishment of the self’s regulatory resources. Self and Identity, 7(3), 305–321. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860701799997
  • Valentine, J. C., Pigott, T. D., & Rothstein, H. R. (2010). How many studies do you need? A primer on statistical power for meta-analysis. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 35(2), 215–247. https://doi.org/10.3102/1076998609346961
  • Van Cutsem, J., Marcora, S., De Pauw, K., Bailey, S., Meeusen, R., & Roelands, B. (2017). The effects of mental fatigue on physical performance: A systematic review. Sports Medicine, 47(8), 1569–1588. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016-0672-0
  • Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Understanding self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 1–12). Guilford.
  • Vohs, K. D., Schmeichel, B. J., Lohmann, S., Gronau, Q. F., Finley, A., Whenyoup, I., Wagenmakers, E. J., & Albarracín, D. A. (2021). Multi-site preregistered paradigmatic test of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Science [Epub ahead of print]. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/e497p
  • Wagstaff, C. R. (2014). Emotion regulation and sport performance. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 36(4), 401–412. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2013-0257
  • Wolff, W., & Martarelli, C. (2020). Bored into depletion? Towards a tentative integration of perceived self-control exertion and boredom as guiding signals for goal-directed behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(5), 1272–1283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620921394
  • Xu, X., Demos, K. E., Leahey, T. M., Hart, C. N., Trautvetter, J., Coward, P., Middleton, K. R., Wing, R. R., & Shu, H. (2014). Failure to replicate depletion of self-control. PLoS ONE, 9(10), e109950. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0109950
  • Yusainy, C., & Lawrence, C. (2015). Brief mindfulness induction could reduce aggression after depletion. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 125–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.008
  • Zering, J. C., Brown, D. M., Graham, J. D., & Bray, S. R. (2017). Cognitive control exertion leads to reductions in peak power output and as well as increased perceived exertion on a graded exercise test to exhaustion. Journal of Sports Sciences, 35(18), 1799–1807. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2016.1237777