2,185
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Brief Report

Early development in Rett syndrome – the benefits and difficulties of a birth cohort approach

, , , , , & show all
Pages 68-72 | Received 22 Jun 2016, Accepted 25 Apr 2017, Published online: 23 May 2017

ABSTRACT

Purposes: Typically, early (pre-diagnostic) development in individuals later diagnosed with Rett syndrome (RTT) has been investigated retrospectively using parent reports, medical records and analysis of home videos. In recent years, prospective research designs have been increasingly applied to the investigation of early development in individuals with late phenotypical onset disorders, for example, autism spectrum disorder. Methods: In this study, data collected by the Danish National Birth Cohort lent itself to prospective exploration of the early development of RTT, in particular early motor-, speech-language, and socio-communicative behaviors, mood, and sleep. Results and Conclusions: Despite limitations, this quasi prospective methodology proved promising. In order to add substantially to the body of knowledge, however, specific questions relating to peculiarites in early development could usefully be added to future cohort studies. As this involves considerable work, it may be more realistic to consider a set of indicators which point to a number of developmental disorders rather than to one.

Introduction

Mutations in the X-linked methyl-CpG-binding protein 2 gene (MECP2) are the main cause of Rett syndrome (RTT; OMIM #312750), a genetic disorder affecting neurodevelopment, with a prevalence of approximately 1 in 10,000 live female births.Citation1Citation4 The natural history of this disorder follows a four-stage trajectory including a period of regression with subsequent recovery or stabilization.Citation3,Citation5 HagbergCitation5 reported on 35 females characterized inter alia by loss of verbal communicative abilities and purposeful hand use with simultaneously occurring hand stereotypies (hand wringing, washing character). He published his findings under the eponym of the author who first described this symptom-complex some 50 years ago, the Austrian neuropediatrician Andreas Rett.Citation6 Since then, consensus clinical criteria have been developed and constantly modified according to the scientific and clinical advances in the study of RTT.Citation4,Citation7 The main mutation responsible for this clinical condition was discovered more than 30 years after its first description,Citation1 and a classification system differentiating between typical RTT and atypical RTT (or RTT variants, such as preserved speech variant, early seizure variant, congenital variant) has been established.Citation4,Citation8 In recent years, numerous studies on the aetiology of the disorder, the epidemiology, the treatment, and potential advances in facilitating earlier diagnosis have been conducted. Notwithstanding growing evidence about early neurobehavioral alterations in infants and toddlers with RTT prior to the onset of regression and diagnosis,Citation9Citation20 there is still a widespread believe among many clinicians and scientists that early development is normal before obvious regression.

The body of evidence for atypical early development in RTT and the attempts to define behavioral biomarkers or neurofunctional markers of maldevelopment in motor-, speech-language, and socio-communicative domains stem almost exclusively from retrospective data analyses. The two major approaches to outlining developmental trajectories prior to diagnosis of individuals with any type of late phenotypical onset disorders are based on (a) medical records and parental reports, or (b) analyses of home videos. Both approaches have contributed to our understanding of developmental characteristics of RTT, but they clearly have limitations.Citation21Citation26

In recent years, prospective research designs have been increasingly applied to the investigation of early development in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and even some rare diseases such as fragile X syndrome.Citation27Citation29 Prospective studies have without any doubt advantages over retrospective designs, that are influenced, e.g., by time lag, memory bias and forward telescoping effects, awareness of the diagnosis at the time of the interview, etc.,Citation21,Citation22,Citation30 yet is hard to apply in rare disorders without a family history. Other sources of prospective data include cohort and register studies. Register studies usually provide more general core data (medical, social, etc.) not intended to answer specific research questions. Access to the Danish National Birth Cohort (DNBC) allowed us to prospectively explore the early development of RTT. The aim of our study was to determine the added value of assessing medical and behavioral data from a cohort study to the understanding of the early phenotype of RTT. With the data available we specifically aimed to shed light on the following aspects of early development of RTT: (a) motor skills, (b) speech-language, (c) socio-communicative behaviors, and (d) mood and sleep.

Methods

The Danish National Birth Cohort

Between 1996 and 2002, a total of 101,042 pregnant women in Denmark consented to take part in a large nationwide longitudinal study.Citation31,Citation32 They were interviewed by telephone during pregnancy and when the offspring was 6 and 18 months old. The interviews were designed to study the mother’s lifestyle during pregnancy and the short- and long-term effects of this on the child, assessing a wide range of neurodevelopmental features in children but not to detect specific developmental disorders.Citation31 Details can be found at the DNBC homepage (DNBC 2013; www.dnbc.dk). At 12 weeks’ gestation, information on the socio-economic status and educational background of the parents was collected. On occasions, an interview could not be conducted at the allotted time and had to be postponed. In such a case the participant was asked to provide the information pertinent to the originally-scheduled time (therefore quasi prospective design). If a participant was uncertain about an answer or had chosen not to answer a question, the answer was coded as missing.

The 6 and 18 month interviews consisted of more than 200 questions each. Data on motor, cognitive, speech-language and social parameters, as well as feeding, mood, sleeping, crying, hearing abilities, and vision were collected.Citation33,Citation34

Inclusion criteria

All citizens in Denmark are assigned a personal identification number from The Danish Civil Registration System at birth.Citation35 This personal-ID enables identification of Danish individuals across public registries. We included only participants living in Denmark through the entire period who had participated in the 12-week gestation interview and in at least one of the interviews at age 6 or 18 months. From the original cohort, 92% of the participants completed the first interview at 12 weeks’ gestation. Seventy per cent participated at the 6 month interview and 66% at 18 months (DNBC, 2012). The study population comprised 76,322 children (39,046 girls).

Diagnosis of Rett syndrome

In Denmark, children suspected of having RTT are typically referred to a public pediatric department where evaluation and treatment is free of charge. In- and outpatient diagnoses assigned at public hospitals are registered using ICD-10 classifications in the Danish National Patient Register.Citation36 According to the Danish National Patient Register, six girls in the DNBC were identified with RTT (ICD-10 code F84.2).

Ethical considerations

The DNBC steering committee gave permission to use data from the cohort for this project. At the time of inclusion in the DNBC, the mothers gave consent to the usage of data for research and any resulting publications, and to the linkage with public registries in the future. The study was approved by the Danish National Board of Health and registered at the Danish Data Protection Agency. It should be noted that Statistics Denmark, the central authority on Danish statistics and the use of data (http://www.dst.dk), only allows extraction of data at group level where the individual cannot be identified. For any table, only cells containing at least three observations can be reported.

Results and discussion

The prevalence of RTT in this sub-cohort of DNBC is approximately 1 in 7,000 females. Four out of the six parents of individuals later diagnosed with RTT participated in the interview at 6 months post-delivery, and all took part in the 18 month interview. The mean age at the first interview was 6.4 months for the study cohort and 5.8 months for RTT; for the second interview, these were 19.2 months and 19.4 months respectively. The average age of RTT diagnosis was 2 years (range 1.6–2.3) (see ).

Table 1. Characteristics of the DNBC study population (n = 76,322).

At the time of the 6 month interview there appeared to be few differences in many of the developmental milestones that were assessed (see ). One indicator of a possible delay in development that did emerge, however, was in relation to gross motor skills, with the reported mean age of sitting without support. In the cohort this was achieved at an average age of 6.5 months (SD = 1.25) whereas the mean age for those with RTT was 7 months (SD = 1.41). As the sample size of individuals with RTT was extremely small these results should be treated with caution. Yet, by the time of the 18 month interview significantly greater differences were apparent in several areas of gross motor ability, for example, none of the six females with RTT were able to walk without support whereas 98.7% of the cohort had achieved this milestone (mean age of walking was 12.6 months; SD = 1.8).

Table 2. Developmental aspects at 6 and 18 months; only descriptive data are presented due to the small sample size of individuals with Rett.

This pattern, of generally unremarkable differences in development at 6 months with more significant delays, especially in gross motor development, apparent by 18 months can be seen in other results from the 6 and 18 month interviews which are presented in . This shows findings for motor behavior, speech-language and socio-communication at the time of both interviews and, in addition, mood and sleep behaviors at 18 months of age. That said, it is also noticeable that although the six females with RTT appeared to show clear delays in their gross motor skills at 18 months, a number of tasks, particularly those in the speech-language and socio-communications domain, had not been achieved by a large proportion of either the cohort or those with RTT at this age. For example, a majority of the cohort, as well as the six individuals with RTT, did not have a vocabulary of more than 10 productive words nor did they combine words at 18 months of age (). With regard to mood and sleep, no apparent differences could be noted between children with RTT and the cohort.

Overall the results, inconspicuous behavior at 6 months with more obvious deviations by 18 months, were in line with previous findings and the reports by Neul and colleaguesCitation3 in their recent large-scale natural history study on developmental delays in RTT (in a retrospective design), in which they state “Early developmental skills in RTT are acquired by many, but clear differences emerge in skills expected after 6 months of age” (Conclusions, p. 8). They go onto suggest that the commonly denoted appearance of normal early development in individuals with RTT “may be more apparent than real” (Abstract, p. 1). Over the last two-three decades the work of several groups of researchers and clinicians, including ourselves, has seen a steadily accumulating body of evidence which challenges the paradigm of normal early development.Citation9Citation11,Citation13,Citation15Citation18,Citation20,Citation37,Citation38 Such studies have also begun to delineate different profiles in early development according to both RTT variant and mutation type.Citation2,Citation3,Citation39Citation43

The majority of these findings stem from retrospective assessments, conducted through questionnaires/parental interviews (involving large databases such as InterRett or the Australian Rett syndrome database), retrospective trawls through pre-diagnostic medical recordsCitation44,Citation45 or retrospective video analysis.Citation9Citation11,Citation13,Citation15Citation21,Citation37 The prospective approach presented here is entirely novel to the field of studying RTT precursors. From the literature in studying suboptimal development, or development of children with genetic disorders, we know that parents appear to be able to give an accurate estimation of what is happening not only in relation to their child’s general development when asked at the time of ‘happening events’,Citation46Citation49 but also in relation to more specific aspects, such as speech-language development.Citation50,Citation51 The DNBC study offered us the opportunity to develop a new approach for investigating RTT through collecting quasi prospective and concurrent data on early development.

The original idea underpinning this approach was to use prospectively collected data on children later diagnosed with RTT, enabling access to observations made at the time of “events happening” (and not knowing the later diagnosis of the child) rather than reconstructing events from the past, which could introduce bias. In reality, unfortunately, we had to contend with both concurrent and retrospective data due to (a) the study design, for example, parents were asked during the 18 month interview to provide the age at which their child could sit and/or walk alone, which are milestones usually achieved at a younger age; and (b) the fact that not all parents could be reached in the intended two-week timeframe. Thus, the problems associated with reliance on retrospective analysis could not be avoided completely (quasi prospective approach). Moreover, as a few girls were already diagnosed with RTT by the age of 18 months, this may have influenced the way in which the developmental questions were answered. Furthermore, the results must be treated with some caution as the RTT sample in this study was very small in comparison with the main cohort. This will be, by definition, the case for any study of a rare disorder such as RTT and can only be overcome as more studies of this sort are undertaken, ensuring a critical mass of data can be reached. Finally, it should be noted as a further limitation that neither the data acquisition nor the interviews were designed specifically to ask for peculiarities in early development. For further cohort studies aiming to pinpoint specific neurodevelopmental disorders specifically designed questions to potentially detect peculiarities in early development and different time points of assessment should be considered. Early detection is the key to learn more about early development in a prospective rather than retrospective manner in the future and will certainly lead to earlier intervention, be it general symptomatic or targeted.

Conclusion

Even though the mean age of diagnosis has decreased to around 2 1/2 years for classic RTT and a little more than a year later for atypical RTT,Citation52 we still have some way to go in delivering a comprehensive description of the early development of girls later diagnosed with RTT. In our sample the mean age of diagnosis was even some six months earlier than the recently reported average diagnostic age.Citation52 We cannot yet say with absolute certainty whether the absence of certain behaviors is within the range of normal development, or is shared by all those with certain other developmental disorders, or by those with a severe intellectual disability regardless of cause, or whether they are unique to a certain disorder such as RTT,Citation10 (p.S7). The quasi prospective methodology of the current study is promising, yet in order to add substantially to what we already know about RTT, specific questions addressed to parents need to be designed. As this involves much work when conducting large cohort studies, it may be more realistic to consider a set of indicators which could potentially point to a number of developmental disorders. Professionals need to be aware that parents are valuable informants about their child’s concurrent behaviors and should take their thoughts and concerns into account.Citation23,Citation44 With early alerts, however, come professional and ethical/moral responsibilities, a duty to be able to offer early help and support to families.Citation10,Citation53 Early detection also requires early intervention and this should not be forgotten.

Declaration of interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Acknowledgments

The Danish Society for Rett Syndrome supported the idea of this study. We would like to thank Florian B. Pokorny, Katrin D. Bartl-Pokorny, and Iris Tomantschger for their input on this manuscript.

Funding

The Danish National Research Foundation established the Danish Epidemiology Science Centre that initiated and created the DNBC. The DNBC received a major grant from this Foundation. Additional support for the DNBC is provided by the Pharmacy Foundation, the Egmont Foundation, the March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation, the Augustinus Foundation, and the Health Insurance Foundation. PBM was supported by FWF 19581, 25241 and the Franz Lanyar Foundation. Sanne Lemcke was supported by the Tryg Foundation.

Additional information

Funding

The Danish National Research Foundation established the Danish Epidemiology Science Centre that initiated and created the DNBC. The DNBC received a major grant from this Foundation. Additional support for the DNBC is provided by the Pharmacy Foundation, the Egmont Foundation, the March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation, the Augustinus Foundation, and the Health Insurance Foundation. PBM was supported by FWF 19581, 25241 and the Franz Lanyar Foundation. Sanne Lemcke was supported by the Tryg Foundation.

References

  • Amir RE, Van Den Veyver IB, Wan M, Tran CQ, Francke U, Zoghbi HY. Rett syndrome is caused by mutations in X-linked MECP2, encoding methyl-CpG-binding protein 2. Nature Genetics 1999;23(2):185–188.
  • Fehr S, Bebbington A, Ellaway C, Rowe P, Leonard H, Downs J. Altered attainment of developmental milestones influences the age of diagnosis of Rett syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology 2011;26(8):980–987.
  • Neul JL, Lane JB, Lee HS, Geerts S, Barrish JO, Annese F, et al. Developmental delay in Rett syndrome: data from the natural history study. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 2014;6(1):20.
  • Neul JL, Kaufmann WE, Glaze DG, Christodoulou J, Clarke AJ, Bahi-Buisson N, et al. Rett syndrome: revised diagnostic criteria and nomenclature. Annals of Neurology 2010;68(6):944–950.
  • Hagberg B, Aicardi J, Dias K, Ramos O. A progressive syndrome of autism, dementia, ataxia, and loss of purposeful hand use in girls: Rett’s syndrome: report of 35 cases. Annals of Neurology 1983;14(4):471–479.
  • Rett A. On a unusual brain atrophy syndrome in hyperammonemia in childhood. Wiener Medizinische Wochenschrift 1966;116(37):723–726.
  • Hagberg B. Clinical manifestations and stages of Rett syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 2002;8(2):61–65.
  • Hagberg BA, Skjeldal OH. Rett variants: a suggested model for inclusion criteria. Pediatric Neurology 1994;11(1):5–11.
  • Leonard H, Bower C. Is the girl with Rett syndrome normal at birth? Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 1998;40(2):115–121.
  • Burford B. Perturbations in the development of infants with Rett disorder and the implications for early diagnosis. Brain and Development 2005;27(Suppl 1):S3–S7.
  • Einspieler C, Kerr AM, Prechtl HF. Is the early development of girls with Rett disorder really normal? Pediatric Research 2005; 57 (5 Pt 1): 696–700.
  • Einspieler C, Marschik PB, Domingues W, Talisa VB, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Wolin T, et al. Monozygotic twins with Rett syndrome: phenotyping the first two years of life. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities 2014;26(2):171–182.
  • Kerr AM, Prescott RJ. Predictive value of the early clinical signs in Rett disorder. Brain and Development 2005;27(Suppl 1):S20–S24.
  • Townend GS, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Sigafoos J, Curfs LM, Bolte S, Poustka L, et al. Comparing social reciprocity in preserved speech variant and typical Rett syndrome during the early years of life. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2015:;43-4480–86.
  • Marschik PB, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Tager-Flusberg H, Kaufmann WE, Pokorny F, Grossmann T, et al. Three different profiles: early socio-communicative capacities in typical Rett syndrome, the preserved speech variant and normal development. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2014;17(1):34–38.
  • Marschik PB, Vollmann R, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Green VA, Van Der Meer L, Wolin T, et al. Developmental profile of speech-language and communicative functions in an individual with the preserved speech variant of Rett syndrome. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2014;17(4):284–290.
  • Marschik PB, Sigafoos J, Kaufmann WE, Wolin T, Talisa VB, Bartl-Pokorny KD, et al. Peculiarities in the gestural repertoire: an early marker for Rett syndrome? Research in Developmental Disabilities 2012;33(6):1715–1721.
  • Marschik PB, Pini G, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Duckworth M, Gugatschka M, Vollmann R, et al. Early speech-language development in females with Rett syndrome: focusing on the preserved speech variant. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 2012;54(5):451–456.
  • Marschik PB, Kaufmann WE, Einspieler C, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Wolin T, Pini G, et al. Profiling early socio-communicative development in five young girls with the preserved speech variant of Rett syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2012;33(6):1749–1756.
  • Marschik PB, Kaufmann WE, Sigafoos J, Wolin T, Zhang D, Bartl-Pokorny KD, et al. Changing the perspective on early development of Rett syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2013;34(4):1236–1239.
  • Marschik PB, Einspieler C. Methodological note: video analysis of the early development of Rett syndrome—one method for many disciplines. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 2011;14(6):355–357.
  • Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Baguio F, Cook IC, Hill MM, Hutman T, et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2010;49(3):256–66e1–2.
  • Marschik PB. The pivotal role of parents in documenting early development. North American Journal of Medical Sciences 2014;6(1):48–49.
  • Goldberg WA, Thorsen KL, Osann K, Spence MA. Use of home videotapes to confirm parental reports of regression in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2008;38(6):1136–1146.
  • Palomo R, Belinchon M, Ozonoff S. Autism and family home movies: a comprehensive review. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 2006;27(2 Suppl):S59–68.
  • Zhang D, Kaufmann WE, Sigafoos J, Bartl-Pokorny KD, Krieber M, Marschik PB, et al. Parents’ initial concerns about the development of their children later diagnosed with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability 2017;42:114–122.
  • Bölte S, Marschik PB, Falck-Ytter T, Charman T, Roeyers H, Elsabbagh M. Infants at risk for autism: a european perspective on current status, challenges and opportunities. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2013;22:341–348.
  • Roberts JE, Mankowski JB, Sideris J, Goldman BD, Hatton DD, Mirrett PL, et al. Trajectories and predictors of the development of very young boys with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 2009;34(8):827–836.
  • Hatton DD, Wheeler A, Sideris J, Sullivan K, Reichardt A, Roberts J, et al. Developmental trajectories of young girls with fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 2009;114(3):161–171.
  • Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Garon N. Early identification of autism spectrum disorders. Behavioural Brain Research 2013;251:133–146.
  • Olsen J, Melbye M, Olsen SF, Sorensen TI, Aaby P, Andersen AM, et al. The Danish National Birth Cohort–its background, structure and aim. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2001;29(4):300–307.
  • Andersen AM, Olsen J. The Danish National Birth Cohort: selected scientific contributions within perinatal epidemiology and future perspectives. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2011;39(7 Suppl):115–120.
  • Lemcke S, Parner ET, Bjerrum M, Thomsen PH, Lauritsen MB. Early development in children that are later diagnosed with disorders of attention and activity: a longitudinal study in the Danish National Birth Cohort. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2016;25:1055–1066.
  • Lemcke S, Juul S, Parner ET, Lauritsen MB, Thorsen P. Early signs of autism in toddlers: a follow-up study in the Danish National Birth Cohort. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2013;43(10):2366–2375.
  • Pedersen CB. The Danish civil registration system. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2011;39(7):Suppl):22-25.
  • Lynge E, Sandegaard JL, Rebolj M. The Danish national patient register. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 2011;39(7Suppl):30–33.
  • Bartl-Pokorny KD, Marschik PB, Sigafoos J, Tager-Flusberg H, Kaufmann WE, Grossmann T, et al. Early socio-communicative forms and functions in typical Rett syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2013;34(10):3133–3138.
  • Marschik PB, Einspieler C, Oberle A, Laccone F, Prechtl HFR. Case report: retracing atypical development: a preserved speech variant of Rett syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 2009;39(6):958–961.
  • Fehr S, Downs J, Bebbington A, Leonard H. Atypical presentations and specific genotypes are associated with a delay in diagnosis in females with Rett syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 2010;152A(10):2535–2542.
  • Leonard H, Moore H, Carey M, Fyfe S, Hall S, Robertson L, et al. Genotype and early development in Rett syndrome: the value of international data. Brain and Development 2005;27(Suppl 1):S59–S68.
  • Lee JY, Leonard H, Piek JP, Downs J. Early development and regression in Rett syndrome. Clinical Genetics 2013;84(6):572–576.
  • Bebbington A, Anderson A, Ravine D, Fyfe S, Pineda M, De Klerk N, et al. Investigating genotype-phenotype relationships in Rett syndrome using an international data set. Neurology 2008;70(11):868–875.
  • Urbanowicz A, Downs J, Girdler S, Ciccone N, Leonard H. Aspects of speech-language abilities are influenced by MECP2 mutation type in girls with Rett syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A 2015;167A(2):354–362.
  • Bisgaard AM, Schonewolf-Greulich B, Ravn K, Ronde G. Is it possible to diagnose Rett syndrome before classical symptoms become obvious? Review of 24 Danish cases born between 2003 and 2012. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology 2015;19(6):679–687.
  • Fabio RA, Colombo B, Russo S, Cogliati F, Masciadri M, Foglia S, et al. Recent insights into genotype-phenotype relationships in patients with Rett syndrome using a fine grain scale. Research in Developmental Disabilities 2014;35(11):2976–2986.
  • Glascoe FP, Altemeier WA, MacLean WE. The importance of parents’ concerns about their child’s development. American Journal of Diseases of Children 1989;143(8):955–958.
  • Glascoe FP. Parents’ evaluation of developmental status: how well do parents’ concerns identify children with behavioral and emotional problems? Clinical Pediatrics 2003;42(2):133–138.
  • Bailey DB, Skinner D, Hatton D, Roberts J. Family experiences and factors associated with the diagnosis of fragile X syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 2000;21(5):315–321.
  • De Giacomo A, Fombonne E. Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 1998;7(3):131–136.
  • Marschik PB, Einspieler C, Garzarolli B, Prechtl HF. Events at early development: are they associated with early word production and neurodevelopmental abilities at the preschool age? Early Human Development 2007;83(2):107–114.
  • Eriksson M, Marschik PB, Tulviste T, Almgren M, Perez Pereira M, Wehberg S, et al. Differences between girls and boys in emerging language skills: evidence from 10 language communities. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2012;30(Pt 2):326–343.
  • Tarquinio DC, Hou W, Neul JL, Lane JB, Barnes KV, O’Leary HM, et al. Age of diagnosis in Rett syndrome: patterns of recognition among diagnosticians and risk factors for late diagnosis. Pediatric Neurology 2015;52(6):585–591e2.
  • Ozonoff S. Editorial: early detection of mental health and neurodevelopmental disorders: the ethical challenges of a field in its infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 2015;56(9):933–935.