1,124
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Short Communication

Non-genetic factors affecting hunting ability in Italian Maremma scent hound

, , , , &
Pages 375-378 | Received 11 Feb 2016, Accepted 11 May 2016, Published online: 21 Jul 2016

Abstract

This study aimed to evaluate the effect of four non-genetic factors (sex, coat colour, competition judges, type of trial) on the five hunting traits (search, approach, tracking of prey, standstill barking and physical skills) used to estimate the aptitude for wild boar hunt in Italian Maremma Scent Hound. A total of 1147 dogs (734 males, 399 females, and 14 not sexed dogs) were evaluated in competitions held in North-Central Italy, from 2010 to 2011. Dogs were tested as individuals, pairs and packs. Coat colour had no effect on the five tested traits. A significant difference (p < 0.01) between males and females was observed only for search. Type of trial had a significant effect (p < 0.01) on all the five hunting traits. Judges factor was significant (p < 0.01) for physical skills and barking remaining firm in place. A significant positive phenotypic correlation was observed among tracking of prey, approach and physical skills (p < 0.01). Approach and physical skills were positively correlated with approach (p < 0.05) and tracking of prey (p < 0.01). Search was negatively correlated with all the other four, whereas standstill barking showed no correlation with any traits. These data are the basis to improve our knowledge about the values of variability in considered hunting traits and they provide genetic criteria to the breeders to achieve more stringent selective choices.

Introduction

The Italian Maremma scent hound (IMSH) probably resulted from selections made by hunters towards the end of 1800 to obtain dogs able to hunt for wild boars in the tangled undergrowth of Maremma woods, a locality in the region of Tuscany (Leonardi Citation2007). The breed was officially recognized by the Italian National Kennel Club (ENCI) in 2003 and is fairly distributed in the central regions of Italy (ENCI Citation2003). In 2014 we had 2395 new registrations divided between 164 rough haired and 2231 short haired (ENCI Citation2015). The IMSH is classified as hound and bloodhound. The aptitude for wild boar hunt is estimated in IMSH by scoring five traits that are evaluated during boar-hunting field trials. As the dogs are tested in open field, many environmental factors influence the trial results. The aim of this study was to assess the effects of four non-genetic factors (coat colour, sex, judges, type of trial) on wild boar hunting performance in Italian Maremma Scent Hound.

Materials and methods

The analyses were based on IMSH field trials results from 14 competitions held in North-central Italy. The trial data contained records from January 2010 to March 2011. They were reported in the evaluation forms kindly contributed by ENCI. The total trial data had 1147 records from 734 males, 399 females, and 14 not sexed dogs. Boar-hunting field trials follow the guidelines contained in the Regulation for the Zootechnic Assessment of Hounds (ENCI Citation2009). The trials were held outdoors on normal hunting grounds, mainly forested landscapes. They were divided into three types: individual (single dog), pair (two dogs), or pack (from six to twelve dogs). Each trial was assessed by a maximum of three approved judges for 60 min of accurate observation. Reported scores were reached by consensus. The following five hunting traits were assessed during field trials: search, approach, physical skills, tracking of prey, and ability to bark remaining firm in place (Table ). In addition to the scorings of these traits, the data held information on four non-genetic factors: sex, coat colour (tawny or striped), competition judges, and type of trial.

Table 1. Selected traits that are used by judges to evaluate the performance of dogs during field trial.

The effect of the four non-genetic factors on the five hunting traits was evaluated by means of one-way ANOVA of type I (i.e. with fixed factors). When a factor had more than two level a multiple comparisons was carried out according to Hochberg’s GT2 post hoc test, because of the unequal sample size in the factor levels. Phenotypic correlations were estimated using the Pearson correlation coefficient. Statistical analyses were carried out using the SPSS software, v. 12.0 (IBM SPSS Statistics, Armonk, NY).

Results and discussion

Coat colour had no statistically significant effect on any of the five hunting traits. Therefore, the aptitude for wild boar hunt in IMSH is not influenced by the colour of coat. Differently, a relationship between coat colour and aggressive behaviour in the case of English cocker spaniels, where animals with a solid coat colour show higher levels of aggression than those that are parti-coloured (Podberscek & Serpell Citation1996, Citation1997; Pérez-Guisado et al. Citation2006; Amat et al. Citation2009). Also Kim et al. (Citation2010) reported behavioural differences between fawn and white coat Jindo dogs.

A statistically significant difference (p < 0.01) between males and females was observed only for the search trait. Male dogs performed the task slightly better than females (Table ). Behavioural differences between males and females have been reported in previous studies. Karjalainen et al. (Citation1994, Citation1996) showed that, in Finnish Spitz, males seemed to have better scores for measures expressing dog’s speed (searching and following scores), while females seemed to have better scores for traits that measured the skills of dog when working with birds, like barking. However, the differences between sexes were small at a practical level. Boenigk et al. (Citation2006), in their evaluation of genetic and environmental sources of variation in the Hovawart dog, found significant differences between males and females. Also, Lindberg et al. (Citation2004) reported similar results in their study on Swedish Flatcoated Retrievers. However, Liinamo et al. (Citation1997) showed that the effect of sex was not statistically significant on any of the studied traits in Finnish Hounds.

Table 2. Effect of sex on search hunting trait.

Type of trial had a statistically significant effect (p < 0.01) on all the five hunting traits (Table ). The multiple comparison tests showed that there was always a statistically significant difference between individual versus pair (p = 0.03) as well as between individual versus pack type of competition (p < 0.05) between pack versus individual and pair. Yet, whereas for search, approach, tracking of prey and physical skill traits, the pack evaluation got scores statistically smaller than those of both pair and single evaluations, for the standstill barking trait the situation was reversed. Arvelius and Klemetsdal (Citation2013), in their study about the evaluation of hunting traits in the English Setter population in Sweden and Norway, found that the type of competition had a significant effect on four of the six tested traits for the Swedish data and on all tested traits for the Norwegian data.

Table 3. Effect of type of trial on the five hunting traits.

Competition judges had a statistically significant effect on physical skills (p < 0.001) and ability to bark remaining firm in place (p < 0.01) (Table ). For the physical skills trait, the multiple comparison test showed that there was a statistically significant difference between two competition judges versus one and three competition judges (p = 0.05). Physical skills trait has higher scores when evaluation is done by two judges. For the ability to bark remaining firm in place, the multiple comparison test showed that the mean score given by one competition judges was statistically higher (p < 0.05) than that given by two competition judges. In contrast to Pfleiderer-Högner (Citation1979) who found no significant differences between judges, Ruefenacht et al. (Citation2002) reported that the effect of number of judges was highly significant for all the seven tested traits in German shepherd dog.

Table 4. Effect of competition judges on physical skills and standstill hunting traits.

Lastly, a Pearson’s product–moment correlation was run to assess the relationship between the five tested hunting traits (Table ). A significant positive phenotypic correlation was observed among tracking of prey, approach and physical skills. On the other hand, approach and physical skills were negatively correlated with search, whereas standstill barking was not correlated with any of the other four traits. Karjalainen et al. (Citation1996), in their study about the evaluation of the effects of the environmental factors on seven measures of hunting performance in Finnish Spitz, found that phenotypic correlations among the measures of hunting performance were low between barking frequency and searching scores. Also, barking frequency did not correlate very strongly with the following scores.

Table 5. Correlation coefficients among tested hunting traits.

Conclusions

Our study showed that, in the Italian Maremma Scent Hound (IMSH), coat colour and sex have no, or limited effect, on the five hunting traits investigated. Hence these factors could not be used in selecting IMSH for wild boar hunt. Differently, for as concern the type of trials it is advisable to avoid the evaluation of dogs in pack, as the different scores assigned to the dogs evaluated with this trial respect to those evaluated singly or in pair.

Very few studies exist today about the genetic quantification of aptitudes in hound breeds. Aptitudes are very complex characters, strongly influenced by genetic factors as well as by non-genetic ones, like diet, canine welfare, and training. Our results are the first steps toward the genetic quantification of aptitudes in a hound breed. Also, they represent a starting point to assess the link between standard traits and functional aptitudes of a given breed. Lastly, our findings are the basis to provide genetic and scientific criteria for selection to breeders. Using this information, they will achieve more stringent selective choices that are not based solely on purely empirical methods or aesthetical standards.

Funding information

The research was financially supported by the Italian Society Pros Hound (SIPS) ‘Luigi Zacchetti’.

Disclosure statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this article.

References

  • Amat M, Manteca X, Mariotti VM, Ruiz de la Torre JL, Fatjò J. 2009. Aggressive behaviour in English cocker spaniel. J Vet Behav. 4:111–117.
  • Arvelius P, Klemetsdal G. 2013. How Swedish breeders can substantially increase the genetic gain for the English setter's hunting traits. J Anim Breed Genet. 130:142–153.
  • Boenigk K, Hamann H, Disti O. 2006. Genetic-statistical analysis of environmental and genetic influences on the outcome of the juvenile and breeding performance tests for behaviour traits in Hovawart dogs. Berl Munch Tierarztl Wochenschr. 119:258–269.
  • ENCI. 2003. The Italian National Kennel Club. Resolution n. 192/03/SB (07/07/2003).
  • ENCI. 2009. The Italian National Kennel Club, Regulation for the Zootechnic Assessment of Hound Breeds (30/11/2009).
  • ENCI. 2015. The Italian National Kennel Club Website. Available from: http://www.enci.it/
  • Karjalainen L, Ojala M, Vilva V. 1994. Estimates of heritabilities for measures of hunting performance in the Finnish Spitz. In: Smith C, Gavora JS, Benkel B, Chesnais J, Fairfull W, Gibson JP, Kennedy BW, Burnside EB, editors. Proceedings of the Fifth World Congress on Genetics Applied to Livestock Production. Vol. 20. Guelph (ON, Canada): University of Guelph. pp. 165–168.
  • Karjalainen L, Ojala M, Vilva V. 1996. Environmental effects and genetic parameters for measurements of hunting performance in the Finnish Spitz. J Anim Breed Genet. 113:525–534.
  • Kim YK, Lee SS, Oh SI, Kim JS, Suh EH, Houpt KA, Lee HC, Lee HJ, Yeon SC. 2010. Behavioural reactivity of the Korean native Jindo dog varies with coat colour. Behav Processes. 84:568–572.
  • Leonardi S. 2007. Conoscere il segugio maremmano. Editoriale il Veliero, Imperia, Italy.
  • Liinamo AE, Karjalainen L, Ojala M, Vilva V. 1997. Estimates of genetic parameters and environmental effects for measures of hunting performance in Finnish hounds. J Anim Sci. 75:622–629.
  • Lindberg S, Strandberg E, Swenson L. 2004. Genetic analysis of hunting behaviour in Swedish flatcoated retrievers. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 104:95–106.
  • Pérez-Guisado J, Lopez-Rodrìguez R, Munoz-Serrano A. 2006. Heritability of dominant-aggressive behaviour in English cocker spaniels. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 100:219–227.
  • Pfleiderer-Hogner M. 1979. Moglichkeiten der Zuchtwertschatzung beim Deutschen Schaferhund anhand der Schutzhundeprufung (Possibilities for breeding value evaluation of German Shepherd dog) I. Vet. Med. Diss., Ludwig-Maximilian University of Munich.
  • Podberscek AL, Serpell JA. 1996. The English cocker spaniel: preliminary findings on aggressive behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 47:75–89.
  • Podberscek AL, Serpell JA. 1997. Environmental influences on the expression of aggressive behaviour in English cocker spaniels. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 52:215–227.
  • Ruefenacht S, Gebhardt-Henrich S, Miyake T, Gaillard C. 2002. A behaviour test on German Shepherd dogs: heritability of seven different traits. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 79:113–132.